Journal of Human Sport and Exercise E-ISSN: Universidad de Alicante España

Similar documents
The effect of different corrective feedback methods on the outcome and self confidence of young athletes

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Examining Action Effects in the Execution of a Skilled Soccer Kick by Using Erroneous Feedback

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

NCEO Technical Report 27

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

Downey, California, USA Published online: 08 Feb 2013.

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

Using GIFT to Support an Empirical Study on the Impact of the Self-Reference Effect on Learning

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

Evaluation of Hybrid Online Instruction in Sport Management

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

P. Belsis, C. Sgouropoulou, K. Sfikas, G. Pantziou, C. Skourlas, J. Varnas

The effects of imagery-based occupation on duration of prone extension in four-and five-year old children

Inside the mind of a learner

Physical Education in Kindergarten Promotes Fundamental Motor Skill Development

Faculty of Health and Behavioural Sciences School of Health Sciences Subject Outline SHS222 Foundations of Biomechanics - AUTUMN 2013

The University of Salamanca, Cursos Internacionales

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

Developing a College-level Speed and Accuracy Test

Evaluation of Teaching the IS-LM Model through a Simulation Program

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

King-Devick Reading Acceleration Program

Adler Graduate School

Textbook Evalyation:

A Decade of Research Literature in Physical Education Pedagogy

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

DOES OUR EDUCATIONAL SYSTEM ENHANCE CREATIVITY AND INNOVATION AMONG GIFTED STUDENTS?

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Instructor: Mario D. Garrett, Ph.D. Phone: Office: Hepner Hall (HH) 100

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

The Use of Statistical, Computational and Modelling Tools in Higher Learning Institutions: A Case Study of the University of Dodoma

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

The Contribution of Perceptual and Cognitive Skills in Anticipation Performance of Elite and Non-Elite Soccer Players

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

Math 1313 Section 2.1 Example 2: Given the following Linear Program, Determine the vertices of the feasible set. Subject to:

NEALE ANALYSIS OF READING ABILITY FOR READERS WITH LOW VISION

Reduce the Failure Rate of the Screwing Process with Six Sigma Approach

Concept mapping instrumental support for problem solving

PHYSICAL EDUCATION LEARNING MODEL WITH GAME APPROACH TO INCREASE PHYSICAL FRESHNESS ELEMENTARY SCHOOL STUDENTS

Utilizing Soft System Methodology to Increase Productivity of Shell Fabrication Sushant Sudheer Takekar 1 Dr. D.N. Raut 2

10.2. Behavior models

Examinee Information. Assessment Information

Strategy for teaching communication skills in dentistry

Planning a research project

JONATHAN H. WRIGHT Department of Economics, Johns Hopkins University, 3400 N. Charles St., Baltimore MD (410)

Faculty and Student Perceptions of Providing Instructor Lecture Notes to Students: Match or Mismatch?

ONE TEACHER S ROLE IN PROMOTING UNDERSTANDING IN MENTAL COMPUTATION

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Developing Students Research Proposal Design through Group Investigation Method

SY 6200 Behavioral Assessment, Analysis, and Intervention Spring 2016, 3 Credits

Study Abroad Housing and Cultural Intelligence: Does Housing Influence the Gaining of Cultural Intelligence?

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

Geo Risk Scan Getting grips on geotechnical risks

OPAC and User Perception in Law University Libraries in the Karnataka: A Study

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 136 ( 2014 ) LINELT 2013

Prevalence of Oral Reading Problems in Thai Students with Cleft Palate, Grades 3-5

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

A Program Evaluation of Connecticut Project Learning Tree Educator Workshops

Science Fair Project Handbook

Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

MSE 5301, Interagency Disaster Management Course Syllabus. Course Description. Prerequisites. Course Textbook. Course Learning Objectives

Saeed Rajaeepour Associate Professor, Department of Educational Sciences. Seyed Ali Siadat Professor, Department of Educational Sciences

PROJECT MANAGEMENT AND COMMUNICATION SKILLS DEVELOPMENT STUDENTS PERCEPTION ON THEIR LEARNING

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

Adults with traumatic brain injury (TBI) often have word retrieval problems (Barrow, et al., 2003; 2006; King, et al., 2006a; 2006b; Levin et al.

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

Clock Hour Workshop. June 28, Clock Hours

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Scaling the Equipment and Play Area in Children s Sport to improve Motor Skill Acquisition: A Systematic Review

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

ECON 365 fall papers GEOS 330Z fall papers HUMN 300Z fall papers PHIL 370 fall papers

THE UNIVERSITY OF WESTERN ONTARIO. Department of Psychology

TEACHING AND EXAMINATION REGULATIONS (TER) (see Article 7.13 of the Higher Education and Research Act) MASTER S PROGRAMME EMBEDDED SYSTEMS

Teacher intelligence: What is it and why do we care?

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Ministry of Education General Administration for Private Education ELT Supervision

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

5 Early years providers

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

What effect does science club have on pupil attitudes, engagement and attainment? Dr S.J. Nolan, The Perse School, June 2014

Transcription:

Journal of Human Sport and Exercise E-ISSN: 1988-5202 jhse@ua.es Universidad de Alicante España NÚÑEZ SÁNCHEZ, FRANCISCO JAVIER; GÁLVEZ GONZÁLEZ, JAVIER Influence of three accuracy levels of knowledge of results on motor skill acquisition Journal of Human Sport and Exercise, vol. 5, núm. 3, 2010, pp. 476-484 Universidad de Alicante Alicante, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=301023489017 How to cite Complete issue More information about this article Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Scientific Information System Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative

Original Article Influence of three accuracy levels of knowledge of results on motor skill acquisition FRANCISCO JAVIER NÚÑEZ SÁNCHEZ 1, JAVIER GÁLVEZ GONZÁLEZ Departamento de Deporte e Informática. Universidad Pablo de Olavide, Sevilla, Spain ABSTRACT Núñez FJ, Gálvez J. Influence of three accuracy levels of knowledge of results on motor skill acquisition. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 476-484, 2010. This study analyse the influence of Knowledge of Results (KR) with different accuracy levels on the learning rate of a throwing skill of spatial non-vision accuracy, during the acquisition stage. We assessed the number of successful events, the distance from the target and the consistency of the responses obtained by 180 subjects. We applied three accuracy levels: KR1 subjects were informed about reaching the target or not; KR2 informed about the direction of the failure events; and KR3 informed about the direction and quantification of the failure events. All groups improved their rate of success 15.56% in KR1, 14.45% in KR3 (p<0.001) and 14.16% in KR2 (p=0.001) as well as their consistency level after the acquisition stage. After 15 minutes without KR, we found main differences related to the rate of success in the retention stage between KR2 and KR1 (p=0.026), and between KR3 and KR1 (p=0.001), but not between KR2 and KR3. We can conclude that, a less precise KR, aimed just to the direction of the failure events, resulted more efficient at an initial learning stage than a more precise KR, aimed to the direction and the distance of the failure events. Key words: MOTOR LEARNING, NON-VISION, THROWING SKILL, FEED-BACK. 1 Corresponding author. Universidad Pablo de Olavide. Carretera de Utrera, km 1. 41013, Sevilla Phone: +954977960; Fax: +91 954348377 E-mail: fjnunsan@upo.es Submitted for publication March 2010. Accepted for publication July 2010. ISSN 1988-5202 Faculty of Education. University of Alicante doi:10.4100/jhse.2010.53.17 VOLUME 5 ISSUE 3 2010 476

INTRODUCTION Considering "learning" as the ability to detect and correct failures, the Knowledge of Results (KR) was considered by many authors in 60's and 70's (Bilodeau, 1969) and, more recently, in 80's (Salmoni, Schmidt, & Walter, 1984), as one of the most important variables to control and acquire motor skills. Given the prominence acquired by the influence of KR on learning processes, it was even asserted that the more frequency, accuracy and immediacy when providing KR, the better learning rates would be reached (Salmoni et al., 1984). Several studies qualified this direct and positive relation of KR with the learning of motor skills. Firstly, different researches showed that motor learning can occur without KR (Swinnen, Vandenberghe, & Van Assche, 1985). In fact, this kind of studies, instead of the lack of KR, demanded the importance of the inherent information obtained by the subject from his own movement when acquiring a skill (internal feedback or IFB). However, it has been proved that, in predominantly inherent time anticipation tasks, the subjects with 0% of KR learned at a lesser extent than those with 100% of KR (Young & Schmidt, 1990). Some studies even showed that providing KR during the acquisition of a skill (external feedback or EFB) had more influence than the subject's inherent information itself, although this could be enough to learn the task and the provided KR could be incorrect or redundant with the IFB (Buekers, Magill, & Hall, 1992; Buekers, Magill, & Sneyers, 1994; McNevin, Magill, & Buekers, 1994; Buekers & Magill, 1995). Secondly, the inclusion of retention tests when analysing the tasks showed that a precise KR, large and immediate, contributed to a positive effect on the acquisition, although was a negative influence on the retention of the tasks to be learned (Butki & Hoffman, 2003; Schmidt, Lange, & Young, 1990; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990). This is called the "double effect" of KR, which is not valid for every kind of tasks. Kohl and Guadagnoli (1996) carried out a research with three experimental groups provided with 100% of KR, 50% (6 repetitions with KR and 6 without), and 50% (12 with KR and 12 without KR), respectively. The analysis of the 6 and 12 first attempts showed that the third group was better than the first one, which was for its part better than the second group; moreover, the retention of the first group (100% of KR) was better than the retention of the second group (50% of KR). Also Badets, Blandin, Wright, and Shea (2006) as shown in the retention phase after learning, subjects who received half the KR show greater stability in the results. We can then concluded that there is not an established pattern of improvement, at the same time questioning the double effect theory. Given these aspects, Magill (1994, 2001) pointed out that KR can be essential when acquiring skills if the information provided by the IFB is not enough or cannot be used by the subject. It may not be required to acquire skills when the IFB is enough, evident and achievable, becoming the EFB a redundant information without usefulness. At the same time, it can help the acquisition of skills, without resulting essential or redundant, while accelerating the learning processes of basically inherent skills; it may also disrupt the skill acquisition by creating a dependence of the EFB which causes a reduction of performance (Winstein & Schmidt, 1990; Anderson, Magill, & Sekiya, 2001). These changeable results when applying KR seems to depend on the complexity of the task. According to Swinnen (1996), the complexity of the task is related to the richness of the provided IFB the more complexity of the task, the more richness of the IFB; he also asserted that the tasks with a simple IFB (less complex tasks) could be more vulnerable to the negative effects of KR than the tasks with a complex IFB. To go into this aspect in more depth, we analysed the different variables of application of KR on simple and complex tasks. Wulf, Shea, and Matschiner (1998) studied the influence of the KR frequency on learning 477 2010 ISSUE 3 VOLUME 5 2010 University of Alicante

the complex skill of skiing slalom; they found out that the group with 100% of KR had a better performance than the group with 50%, and this one performed better than the group with 0% during retention tests without KR. Anderson et al. (2001) used a simple linear-positioning task without vision, offering a KR without delay or with a 2-attempts delay. The results showed that, in the acquisition stage, the groups without delay had a better performance than the groups with some delay; however, this trend changed during the retention stage, although this effect did not always appear at the same extent. The same study showed that the inclusion of a spring (to avoid the static positioning of the implement) caused difficulties when using the IFB and reduced the effect of handling KR. Guadagnoli, Leis, Van Gemmert, and Stelmach (2002) compared the effect on acquisition and retention of a linear-positioning task with time requirement and two different frequencies of KR (20% and 100%), both in healthy subjects and in Parkinson's patients with clear proprioception difficulties (use of IFB). The results showed that the healthy subjects of the group with the reduced frequency obtained better values in the retention stage than the healthy subjects of the group with 100% of KR; on the other hand, the Parkinson's patients did not follow the same trend, since, both during the acquisition and retention stages, the group with a 100% was better than the one with 20% of KR. We could reach the conclusion that a high frequency of KR improves the acquisition while reduces the retention in simple tasks, although when these tasks are complex and less consolidated, a high frequency of KR would lead to improvements during the acquisition and retention stages (Anderson et al., 2001). One of the variables of the application of KR more often analysed is the accuracy of the information provided to the trainee. As well as for the assessment of the frequency variable, there are several studies asserting that it cannot be established a direct relation between the accuracy level and learning (Wright, Smith Munyon, & Sidaway, 1997). The studies by Rogers (1974) raised the theory of an ideal accuracy level of KR, above and below which the learning rate is reduced. Nevertheless, some replicas made of these studies argued that, if the subjects know the meaning of the units used to provide KR, there would be no differences in learning between the groups with a higher or lower accuracy level of KR (Salmoni, Ross, Dill, & Zoeller, 1983). Reeve and Magill (1981) carried out a study about the influence of providing information related to the distance or direction of the failure events in a simple positioning test of a blind implement. These authors' conclusion established that KR about the direction of the failure events (less precise) is more useful in the initial stage of the learning process, while KR about the distance of the failure events (more precise) requires a certain level of experience in the subjects, so they can acquire it as useful information. By providing significant KR to the trainee and carrying out retention tests, Magill and Wood (1986) obtained similar results in a study about the influence of providing quantitative or qualitative KR in a complex task of reproduction of a specific movement. There were no main differences between the groups during the acquisition stage, while in the retention stage, the group with quantitative KR was better than the group with qualitative one. These results confirmed that both kinds of KR would be useful at the same degree, since they provided information about the direction of the failure events (less precise) at the initial learning stages; moreover, at the end of the acquisition stage and during retention, the quantitative information would be more useful than qualitative, since it provided information about the magnitude of the failure events (more precise). Reeve, Dornier, and Weeks (1990) came to a similar conclusion asserting that the precision of KR influenced on learning in a way that, at the earlier learning stages, a high accuracy level could be negative but, from a significant and durable point of view, the more accuracy provided, the more learning would be reached. Moreover, Chiviakowsky, and Wulf (2007) pointed out the importance of providing more information about success events than about failures. Wright et al. (1997), concluded that quantitative KR was useful when the failure was high, but that it was impossible to assert that qualitative KR would be more useful at long-term than quantitative when correcting small failure events. VOLUME 5 ISSUE 3 2010 478

In the present study, we aimed to analyse the influence of KR with different accuracy levels on the learning rate of a throwing skill of spatial non-vision accuracy, during the acquisition stage and after a period without applying KR. MATERIAL AND METHODS Participants One hundred eighty university students (n=180, 74% male-26% female) subjects volunteered for this study after providing informed consent approved by the University Pablo de Olavide (Seville) Ethics Committee. Their average age ± SD was 19.2±1.2 yrs. Subjects were students of physical education, and were chosen to have sufficient motor maturity; Then, the results were due solely to the effect of KR on motor learning learning. Experimental approach to the issue. In this study we analysed the influence of a KR with three accuracy levels on the learning rate of a throwing skill of spatial non-vision accuracy. The accuracy levels of KR were: KR1 (n=60) subjects were provided with information about their successful or failure events; KR2 (n=60) subjects were provided with information about the direction of the failure events, pointing out if the throwing went past or did not reach the target; and KR3 (n=60) subjects were provided with information about the direction and quantification of failure events, pointing out if the throwing went past or did not reach the target, and the centimetres of separation. Procedures The situation of assessment and acquisition (see Figure 1) involved the throwing of a tennis ball with the eyes covered, trying to put it inside a target 36 cm deep, 3 meters away. We registered the centimetres of distance between the final position of the ball and the target in each throwing: the deviation was negative when the ball did not reach the target and positive when it went further. We applied a previous assessment (Pre-Test) involving 6 shots without any provided KR. The acquisition stage consisted of 4 sets of 6 shots, with the KR which corresponded to each experimental group (KR1, KR2 and KR3). It was carried out a later assessment, under the same conditions than during Pre-Test and just after the acquisition stage (Post- Test), as well as after 15 minutes without activity (Re-Test). Figure 1. Situation of assessment and acquisition of the throwing skill of non-vision accuracy. 479 2010 ISSUE 3 VOLUME 5 2010 University of Alicante

Dependant variables and analysis We analysed two accuracy variables in the subjects' response: on one hand, the "separation distance" variable, measured through the median of centimetres between the final position of the ball and the target in every shot (by subject and assessment set); and, on the other hand, the "success" variable, measured through the times that the ball was placed inside the target (by subject and assessment set). We then registered a variable of response consistency, the "diffusion". To obtain it, we measured the median of 75 and 25 percentiles on the data of centimetres between the final position of the ball and the target in every shot (by subject and assessment set); from the 75 percentile, we subtracted the 25 one, resulting the diffusion of the response. To compare intragroup results we used the Wilconson test for signal ranges, with a reliability index of 95% (p<0.05). When analysing the intragroup results we used the Kruskal-Wallis H test of variance, with a reliability index of 95% (p<0.05), to apply then the Mann-Whitney U test to the significant variables. RESULTS Table 1 shows the results obtained for each analysed variable. The three experimental groups improved their accuracy levels of throwing after the acquisition stage, regarding the separation distance variable; however, the KR2 group was the only one with significant differences (p=0.006). During Re-Test, the KR3 group significantly reduced its distance from the target (p=0.045), while the other groups showed no significant differences. Nevertheless, the KR2 group maintained significant values compared to Pre-Test (p=004). There were no main intragroup differences for the separation distance variable. Table 1. Results of accuracy and consistency variables for responses during the assessment tests, by experimental group. Pre-Test Post-Test Re-Test Average SD Average SD Average SD KR1 31.65 119.60 16.11 113.96 15.43 113.06 Separation KR2 39.41 105.75-7.02 22.43-10 23.74 Distance (cm) KR3 32.13 101.38 3.33 25.55-2.32 27.32 KR1 85.46 59.16 45.3 38.5 50.94 37.45 Diffusion KR2 82.79 52.72 49.93 38.68 40.87 24.73 (cm) KR3 84.37 56.05 48.98 47.69 36.97 26.93 Note: SD = Standard Deviation The three groups showed a significant improvement in the accuracy level of the throwing after the acquisition stage for the success variable: 15.56% in KR1 and 14.45% in KR3 (P<0.001), while 14.16% in KR2 (p=0.001). During Re-Test, the number of successful throwings decreased -7.5% in KR1 and -2.5% in KR2, while the value of the KR3 group increased a 3.05%; however, none of them reached main differences compared to Post-Test. The number of successful shots was still significantly higher than in Pre-Test for the three groups: KR1 (P=0.003), KR2 (p=0.001) and KR3 (p<0.001). We also found main differences related to the success events during Re-Test between KR2-KR1 groups (p=0.026) and KR3- KR1 (p=0.001) groups, while there were not main differences between KR2-KR3 groups. VOLUME 5 ISSUE 3 2010 480

Figure 2. Percentages of success during the assessment tests, by experimental group (EG) 40 35 % Successful 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 KR1 KR2 KR3 Pre-Test 12.77 16.11 16.38 Post-Test 28.33 30.27 30.83 Re-Test 20.83 27.77 33.88 EG In the analysis of consistency of performance for every group, every group significantly reduced their diffusion values after the acquisition stage: KR1, KR2 and KR3 (p<0.001). During the Re-Test, KR2 and KR3 reduced also their diffusion, while KR1 increased it. None of the groups showed a statistically remarkable change compared to Post-Test, although the differences compared to Pre-Test were still significant (p<0.001). There were no main intragroup differences for the diffusion variable. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION We could consider this skill as a complex task (Swinnen, 1996), with a high frequency of application of KR, in which we combined different levels of accuracy of KR during the acquisition stage. The proposed learning situation was clearly dependent on an EFB, since the information provided by the IFB was not, a priori, enough to acquire the skill. In that situation, the KR was defined as essential (Magil, 1994, 2001) or as an accelerator of the learning process (Winstein & Schmidt, 1990). Nevertheless, we could prove (as Butki & Hoffman did, 2003) that the rate of success and the consistency of the proposed skill after the learning stage showed a significant improvement in every group, but at a higher extent in the group with less information. Although these differences of information between the groups are not significant, we can conclude that KR provided to the KR1 group is almost irrelevant to the proposed learning situation, since we did not provide enough information to acquire the skill, unless the subject reached a success event. The only main information for the subjects of this group was the provided IFB from a successful execution of the task. These results would agree with the findings by Swinnen et al. (1985), according to which the motor learning could occur without KR, claiming the importance of inherent information obtained by the subject from his own movement when acquiring a skill; however, in our study, the information provided was really minimal, given the lack of vision. On the other hand, those findings would disagree with the results obtained by Young and Schmidt (1990), since the subjects with less KR learned at a higher extent than subjects with 481 2010 ISSUE 3 VOLUME 5 2010 University of Alicante

a more precise KR. When analysing the retention levels, we could see that, even without main intragroup differences, the KR3 and KR2 groups obtained rates of success significantly higher than KR1. It seems that our results, regarding success events, would disagree with the "double effect" theories (Schmidt et al., 1990; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990), given that the precise KR did not reach a more positive effect on acquisition than a less precise one; at the same time, and opposite to the meaning of that effect, there was a positive effect on the retention of the task to be learned. In the present study, we could not confirm the existence of an optimal level of KR accuracy, as Rogers (1974) did, although it seemed impossible to establish a direct relation between the accuracy level and learning, such as other authors asserted (Wright et al., 1997). A similar procedure was followed by Ishikura (2008) applied to putt in golf. However, this time, the subjects with reduced KR could see part of the path followed by the ball, but the end result, and obtaining results similar to ours. According to the results by Reeve and Magill (1981), and Magill and Wood (1986), the more qualitative and less accurate information provided to the KR2 group, aimed to the direction of the failure events, was more useful in an initial stage; this is the only group obtaining a significant reduction in the separation distance from the target after the learning period. Nevertheless, the more quantitative and more accurate information given to the KR3 group, aimed to the distance, obtained better results after the retention stage, being the only group with main differences for the separation distance variable after the period without KR. Our study would be an example of the conclusions by Reeve et al. (1990), according to which the accuracy of KR affects the learning in a way that, in the earlier stages, a high level of accuracy can be negative but, from a significant and durable point of view, the higher accuracy of KR means the higher learning reached. Given our results, we cannot agree with the assertion that, the more frequency, accuracy and immediacy when providing KR, the better rates of success can be obtained (see Salmoni et al., 1984). The results related to a throwing skill of non-vision accuracy confirmed the theories by Kohl and Guadagnoli (1996), asserting that there is not an established pattern of improvement, questioning with it the double effect theory. However, we can conclude that, in a non-vision situation, with less precise KR providing significant information to the trainee and aimed just to the direction of the failure events, was more efficient in the earlier stages of learning than a more precise KR, aimed to the direction and the distance of the failure events. Nevertheless, the rate of success acquired through the provision of less accurate and qualitative KR remained less time than the rate of success obtained through the provision of more accurate and quantitative KR. REFERENCES 1. ANDERSON DI, MAGILL RA, SEKIYA H. Motor learning as a function of KR schedule and characteristics of task-intrinsic feedback. Journal of motor behaviour. 2001; 33(1):59-56. doi:10.1080/00222890109601903 [Back to text] 2. BADETS A, BLANDIN Y, WRIGHT D, SHEA CH. Error detection processes during observational learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2006; 77(2):177-184. [Abstract] [Back to text] 3. BILODEAU EA. Supplementary feedback and instructions. In: EA Bilodeau (Ed.). Principles of skill acquisition. New York: Academic; 1969. [Back to text] VOLUME 5 ISSUE 3 2010 482

4. BUEKERS MJ, MAGILL RA, HALL KG. The effect of erroneus knowledge of results on skill acquisition when augmented information is redundant. The Quarterly journal of Experimental Psychology. 1992; 44(1):105-117. doi:10.1080/14640749208401285 [Back to text] 5. BUEKERS MJ, MAGILL RA, Sneyers KM. Resolving a conflict between sensory feedback and knowledge of results, while learning a motor skill. Journal of motor behavior. 1994; 26(1):27-35. [Abstract] [Back to text] 6. BUEKERS MJ, MAGILL RA. The role of task experience and prior knowledge for detecting invalid augmented feedback while learning a motor skill. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1995; 48(1):84-97. doi:10.1080/14640749508401377 [Back to text] 7. BUTKI BD, HOFFMAN SJ. Effects of reducing frecuency of intrinsic knowledge of results on the learning of a motor skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2003; 97:569-580. [Abstract] [Back to text] 8. CHIVIAKOWSKY S, WULF G. Feedback after good trials enhances learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2007; 78(2):40-47. [Full text] [Back to text] 9. GUADAGNOLI MA, LEIS B, VAN GEMMERT AWA, STELMACH GE. The relationship between knowledge of results and motor learning in parkinsonian patients. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders. 2002; 9:89-95. doi:10.1016/s1353-8020(02)00007-x [Back to text] 10. ISHIKURA T. Reduced relative frequency of knowledge of results without visual feedback in learning a golf-putting task. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2008; 106:225-233. doi:10.2466/pms.106.1.225-233 [Back to text] 11. KOHL RM, GUADAGNOLI MA. The Scheduling of knowledge of results. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1996; 28(3):233-240. [Abstract] [Back to text] 12. MAGILL RA, WOOD CA. Knowledge of results precision as a learning variable in motor skill acquisition. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1986; 57(2):170-173. [Back to text] 13. MAGILL RA. Augmented feedback in motor skill acquisition. In: RN Singer, HA Hausenblas, CM Janelle (Eds.). Handbook of sport psychology. 2 nd Ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2001. [Back to text] 14. MAGILL RA. The influence of augmented feedback on skill learning depends on characteristics of the skill and the learner. Quest. 1994; 46:314-327. [Abstract] [Back to text] 15. MCNEVIN N, MAGILL RA, BUEKERS MJ. The effects of erroneus knowledge of results on transfer of anticipation timing. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1994; 65(4):324-329. [Abstract] [Back to text] 16. REEVE TG, DORNIER LA, WEEKS DJ. Precision of knowledge of results: considerations of the accuracy requirements imposed by the task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1990; 61(3):284-290. [Abstract] [Back to text] 17. REEVE TG, MAGILL RA. The role of components of knowledge of results information in error correction. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1981; 52(1):82-85. [Abstract] [Back to text] 18. ROGERS CA Jr. Feedback precision and postfeedback interval duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1974; 102(4):604-608. [Back to text] 19. SALMONI AW, ROSS D, DILL S, ZOELLER M. Knowledge of results and perceptual-motor learning. Human Movement Science. 1983; 2:77-89. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(83)90008-8 [Back to text] 20. SALMONI AW, SCHMIDT RA, WALTER ChB. Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin. 1984; 95(3):355-386. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.355 [Back to text] 21. SCHMIDT RA, LANGE C, YOUNG DE. Optimizing summary knowledge of results for skill learning. Human Movement Science. 1990; 9:325-348. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(90)90007-z [Back to text] 483 2010 ISSUE 3 VOLUME 5 2010 University of Alicante

22. SWINNEN S, VANDENBERGHE J, VAN ASSCHE EV. Role of cognitive style constructs field dependence-independence and reflection-impulsivity in skill acquisition. Journal of Sport Psychology. 1985; 8:51-69. [Abstract] [Back to text] 23. SWINNEN SP. Information feedback for motor skill learning: a review. In: HN Zelaznik (Ed.). Advances in motor learning and control. Champaign ILL: Human Kinetics; 1996. Pp. 37-66. [Back to text] 24. WINSTEIN CJ, SCHMIDT RA. Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skills learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1990; 16(4):677-691. [Abstract] [Back to text] 25. WRIGHT DL, SMITH-MUNYON VL, SIDAWAY B. How close is too close for precise knowledge of results? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1997; 68(2):172-176. [Abstract] [Back to text] 26. WULF G, SHEA CHH, MATSCHINER S. Frequent feedback enhances complex motor skill learning. Journal of motor behavior. 1998; 30(2):180-192. [Abstract] [Back to text] 27. YOUNG DE, SCHMIDT RA. Units of motor behavior: Modifications with practice and feedback. In: M Jeannerod (Ed.). Attention and performance XIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum; 1990. Pp. 763-795. [Back to text] VOLUME 5 ISSUE 3 2010 484