Original Article. Effects of school-based intervention program on motor performance skills

Similar documents
Developmental coordination disorder DCD. Overview. Gross & fine motor skill. Elisabeth Hill The importance of motor development

E NHANCING. Physical Education with a Supplemental Physical Activity Program MEGAN ADKINS MATTHEW R. BICE KATE HEELAN JAMES BALL

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Process Evaluations for a Multisite Nutrition Education Program

Lancaster Lane CP School. The Importance of Motor Skills

Physical Education in Kindergarten Promotes Fundamental Motor Skill Development

Sheila M. Smith is Assistant Professor, Department of Business Information Technology, College of Business, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana.

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

Instructor: Mario D. Garrett, Ph.D. Phone: Office: Hepner Hall (HH) 100

Evidence to inform effective physical education policy, pedagogy and practice in New South Wales secondary schools

Active Ingredients of Instructional Coaching Results from a qualitative strand embedded in a randomized control trial

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

TRAFFORD CHILDREN S THERAPY SERVICE. Motor Skills Checklist and Advice for Children in PRIMARY & SECONDARY Schools. Child s Name.Dob. Age.

Mathematical learning difficulties Long introduction Part II: Assessment and Interventions

ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS

Tomball College and Community Library Occupational Therapy Journals

ADHD Classroom Accommodations for Specific Behaviour

Scaling the Equipment and Play Area in Children s Sport to improve Motor Skill Acquisition: A Systematic Review

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

A Decade of Research Literature in Physical Education Pedagogy

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Bayley scales of Infant and Toddler Development Third edition

The effect of different corrective feedback methods on the outcome and self confidence of young athletes

THE USE OF TINTED LENSES AND COLORED OVERLAYS FOR THE TREATMENT OF DYSLEXIA AND OTHER RELATED READING AND LEARNING DISORDERS

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

Milton Public Schools Special Education Programs & Supports

The effects of imagery-based occupation on duration of prone extension in four-and five-year old children

Student s Edition. Grade 6 Unit 6. Statistics. Eureka Math. Eureka Math

PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS FOR RESIDENCY EDUCATION IN DEVELOPMENTAL-BEHAVIORAL PEDIATRICS

PSIWORLD Keywords: self-directed learning; personality traits; academic achievement; learning strategies; learning activties.

Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11)

Biological Sciences, BS and BA

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Examinee Information. Assessment Information

ECON 365 fall papers GEOS 330Z fall papers HUMN 300Z fall papers PHIL 370 fall papers

Over-Age, Under-Age, and On-Time Students in Primary School, Congo, Dem. Rep.

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

Running head: NONLINEAR PEDAGOGY AND PHYSICAL EDUCATION

A. What is research? B. Types of research

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Enhancing Van Hiele s level of geometric understanding using Geometer s Sketchpad Introduction Research purpose Significance of study

Charlton Kings Infants School

Shyness and Technology Use in High School Students. Lynne Henderson, Ph. D., Visiting Scholar, Stanford

Prevalence of Oral Reading Problems in Thai Students with Cleft Palate, Grades 3-5

On the Combined Behavior of Autonomous Resource Management Agents

Effect of Cognitive Apprenticeship Instructional Method on Auto-Mechanics Students

IMPROVING ICT SKILLS OF STUDENTS VIA ONLINE COURSES. Rozita Tsoni, Jenny Pange University of Ioannina Greece

The My Class Activities Instrument as Used in Saturday Enrichment Program Evaluation

PHYSICAL EDUCATION AND KINESIOLOGY

Contents. Foreword... 5

This is the author s version (post-print) of a work that was accepted for publication in the following source:

THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR MODEL IN ELECTRONIC LEARNING: A PILOT STUDY

Zoo Math Activities For 5th Grade

Empowering Students Learning Achievement Through Project-Based Learning As Perceived By Electrical Instructors And Students

WELCOME! Of Social Competency. Using Social Thinking and. Social Thinking and. the UCLA PEERS Program 5/1/2017. My Background/ Who Am I?

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

Interprofessional educational team to develop communication and gestural skills

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

PERFORMANCE COMPETENCE LIFESPAN FRAMEWORK

Investigations for Chapter 1. How do we measure and describe the world around us?

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Students attitudes towards physics in primary and secondary schools of Dire Dawa City administration, Ethiopia

Prevent Teach Reinforce

Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-Based Learning

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity.

Kimberly J. Hills Curriculum Vitae

KENTUCKY FRAMEWORK FOR TEACHING

Post-intervention multi-informant survey on knowledge, attitudes and practices (KAP) on disability and inclusive education

IS FINANCIAL LITERACY IMPROVED BY PARTICIPATING IN A STOCK MARKET GAME?

Game-based formative assessment: Newton s Playground. Valerie Shute, Matthew Ventura, & Yoon Jeon Kim (Florida State University), NCME, April 30, 2013

Number Race as an intensified instruction for low performing children in mathematics in grade one

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 191 ( 2015 ) WCES Why Do Students Choose To Study Information And Communications Technology?

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Examining the Use of Play Activities to Increase Appropriate Classroom Behaviors

The Effects of Jigsaw and GTM on the Reading Comprehension Achievement of the Second Grade of Senior High School Students.

What Women are Saying About Coaching Needs and Practices in Masters Sport

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

The Impact of Formative Assessment and Remedial Teaching on EFL Learners Listening Comprehension N A H I D Z A R E I N A S TA R A N YA S A M I

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

learners basic psychological needs (BPNs; i.e., autonomy, competency, and relatedness).

Science Fair Project Handbook

Informal Comparative Inference: What is it? Hand Dominance and Throwing Accuracy

Role Models, the Formation of Beliefs, and Girls Math. Ability: Evidence from Random Assignment of Students. in Chinese Middle Schools

Section 1: Basic Principles and Framework of Behaviour

Professional Teachers Strategies for Promoting Positive Behaviour in Schools

Teaching Language Skills to Preschool Students with Developmental Delays and Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Language for Learning

Occupational Therapist (Temporary Position)

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

E-learning Strategies to Support Databases Courses: a Case Study

The Comparative Study of Information & Communications Technology Strategies in education of India, Iran & Malaysia countries

Ph.D. in Behavior Analysis Ph.d. i atferdsanalyse

WHO ARE SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGISTS? HOW CAN THEY HELP THOSE OUTSIDE THE CLASSROOM? Christine Mitchell-Endsley, Ph.D. School Psychology

The ABCs of FBAs and BIPs Training

CORRELATION FLORIDA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS CORRELATION COURSE STANDARDS / BENCHMARKS. 1 of 16

Transcription:

Journal of Physical Education and Sport (JPES), 16(3), Art 119, pp. 737-742., 2016 online ISSN: 2247-806X; p-issn: 2247 8051; ISSN - L = 2247-8051 JPES Original Article Effects of school-based intervention program on motor performance skills MATHISEN, GUNNAR E. School of Sport Sciences, UiT The Artic University of Norway, NORWAY Published online: September 26, 2016 (Accepted for publication August 02 2016) DOI:10.7752/jpes.2016.03119 Abstract: The purpose of this study was to identify the effects of a four-week intervention program on motor skill performance in primary school children. Twenty-one children with mean age of 6.5 ± 0.7 years participated. The experimental group (EG) followed a specific movement program in three basic tasks: throwing, climbing, and jumping. The control group (CG) carried out an ordinary physical education program. Results indicated that the experimental group significantly improved their performance compared to the control. It seems that the performance of motor skills can be improved through task-specific organized practice in physical education programs in primary school. Key words: physical education, motor learning, task-specific program, primary school children Introduction It is crucial for children to master motor performance skills, such as running, jumping, climbing and throwing. The skills do not, however, come naturally, but need to be learned, practiced and reinforced (Gallahue et al., 2012; Logan et al., 2015). Research shows a positive relationship between physical activity and motor development (Fisher et al., 2005) and fitness (Stodden et al., 2007; Barnett et al., 2009; Lubans et al., 2010). Improvement of motor skills is associated with positive health-related outcomes, and with increased physical activity engagement (Okely et al., 2001; Lubans et al., 2010; Livonen et al., 2013: Catuzzo et al., 2016). Moreover, fundamental motor skills are the building blocks of more complex movements, which enable children to participate in sport and games that require more advanced movements (Logan et al., 2011). Therefore, it is important to promote physical activity and programs for the development of motor skills performance (Rietmuller et al., 2009). In school, motor performance skills not only allow children to carry out everyday practical tasks, but are also an important determinant of their level of self-esteem, and of their popularity and status in their peer group (Skinner & Piek, 2001). There is a suggestion that children having low skill performance may be drawn into a negative spiral of disagreement, where low skill contributes to low perceived physical competence (Stodden et al., 2007; 2008). Motor skill competency is identified in the curriculum for the 10-year compulsory schooling in Norway as a quality goal in primary school physical education. However, motor skill performance mastery among children appears in some cases inadequately low, and children who lack the opportunity to practice often demonstrate delays in motor skill development (Erwin et al., 2008; Lubans et al., 2010; Mathisen, 2016). When teaching motor skill, educators strive to apply the best practice, and selected methods of learning and teaching may not have a strong scientific evidence base. One method of delivering instruction by physical education (PE) teachers is direct instruction, a method that is teacher-centered, goal-directed and orderly. Research in PE pedagogy has consistently provided support for the use of direct instruction to develop motor skills (Sweeting & Rink, 1999). However, creating a climate that supports the learning of motor skills, which gives the learner more freedom of choice and in decision-making, has been shown to be equally helpful in developing motor skills (Valentini et al., 1999). In accordance with the dynamical system approach, it is suggested that motor skill development is based on the interaction between constraints from the task, the organism, and the environment (Newell, 1984; Davis & Burton, 1991). Motor skills emerge within a dynamic system consisting of a specific task, performed by a learner with given characteristics, in a particular environment (Newell, 1984; Davis & Burton, 1991). Task-specific intervention was put forward by Revie & Larkin (1993) as a method for use with poorly coordinated children, and in contrast to those methods which focus on general abilities or processes. The approach is founded on the assumption that multiple subsystems must be organized and constrained in specific ways for a solution to the task to emerge (Revie & Larkin, 1993; Davis & Burton, 1991). Motor skills can be described as object control, e.g. skills like overarm throw, or locomotor skills, e.g. skills like jumping or climbing (Gallahue et al., 2012). The task indicate how the subjects must behave to complete the movement successfully. Competence in the different motor skills is positively associated with -------737 Corresponding Author MATHISEN, GUNNAR E., E-mail: gunnar.mathisen@uit.no

----------- increased physical activity engagement (Lubans et al., 2010; Livonen et al., 2013), and it appears that improved performance in motor skills among children is positively correlated with participation in organized sport (van Beurden et al., 2002). However, there is not much empirical evidence on the practical learning effects situations in the school. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to assess the effectiveness of interventions designed to improve some fundamental movement skills in primary school children using a specific task teaching approach based on a theoretical framework of the dynamic system approach. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the effects of short-term training programs with primary school children. The effect of the intervention was hypothesized to be effective for the experimental group. Materials and Method Participants The participants of the study were selected from children enrolled in the first stage of primary school in Norway. The experimental group (EG) consisted of 21 participants, 12 girls and 9 boys, with a mean age of 6.5 (± 0.7 years) from one class. The control group (CG), with a mean age of 6.6 (±0.6 years), consisted of 22 students, 9 girls and 13 boys, from another class. Permission for the study was granted from the parents or legal guardians, and the study was approved by the school administration and by the local ethics committee according to the Declaration of Helsinki. Intervention program This study utilized a pre-test to post-test quasi-experimental design due to the lack of random assignment of participants to groups consisting of intact classes at primary school. The intervention program consisted of 20 sessions during a four-week period, with one hour practice per day. A typical session started with warm-up activities such as run- and catch-games. A station-organization approach was carried out, by assigning each skill or activity to a separate location in the gymnasium. Each session lasted 60 minutes and consisted of a 15-minutes period of jumping activities, 15 minutes of climbing, and 15 minutes of throwing. Warm-up activities lasted about 10 minutes, and about two minutes were allowed for transition between each activity station. A task-specific approach was used, in which the instructor provided the task presentation, followed by the student activities individualized at their own level. Jumping tasks was practiced by single foot jumping, or both feet simultaneously in different ways or through games. Throwing was practiced with overarm throw, by working two together throwing to each other, throwing to hit a target or by different games. The climbing activity was practiced on the wall bars or in the climbing wall with different routes or free climbing. The control group followed an ordinary PE program with the same number of lessons with, one hour practice per day. All sessions took place in an indoor gymnasium. Test procedure The three test items were taken from the new test battery designed for testing everyday activities for children, like jumping, throwing, climbing and running (Fjørtoft et al., 2011). The test is designed for 5- to 12- years-olds, most of the test items have appeared in other test batteries. The three items used for this study was: a) Jumping a distance of seven meter on one foot as fast as possible. The test item score is the time needed to cross the distance (measured in seconds). b) Throwing a tennis ball with one hand as far as possible. The child stands with the contralateral foot in front of the ipsilateral foot. The test item score (better of two attempts) is the distance thrown (measured in meters). c) Climbing up wall bars, crossing over two columns to the right, and climbing down the fourth column as fast as possible. Each of the wall bar is 2.55 m high and 0.75 m wide. The test item score (better of two attempts) is the time to completion (measured in seconds). Each test was explained and demonstrated before the child started, and the children were tested individually. If a child made a procedural error, instructions and demonstrations were repeated, and the child made a new attempt (Fjørtoft et al., 2011). Statistical analysis A two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures was used to determine the pairwise differences between the intervention group and the control group. When a significant F value was achieved, appropriate Tukey post hoc tests procedures were used to locate the difference between the means. Data are presented as mean ± SD, and statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Test-retest reliabilities for the experimental test have shown high interclass correlation (Fjørtoft et al., 2011). 738

----------- Results Table I. Pre- and post-test results for motor skill performance (mean ±SD) for training group (TG) and control group (CG). Training Group Control Group (n=21) (n=22) Test Pre Post Pre Post Jumping task (sec) 4.55 (0.89) 3.56 (0.41) # 4.55 (0.76) 4.33 (0.74) Climbing task (sec) 17.6 (2.56) 14.07 (2.64) # 16.64 (3.13) 16.65 (2.87) Throwing task (sec) 8.07 (2.73) 10.33 (3.05) # 9.45 (3.26) 9.22 (3.30) Pre- and post-tests between-groups, significant # p<0.05 At the end of the training period, the training group displayed a significant improvement in both jumping-, throwing- and climbing skills. After four weeks of training, the group average jumping skill improved significantly by 16.1 % from 4.55 (±0.41) seconds to 3.56 (±0.41) seconds, climbing skills improved significantly by 17.2 % from 17.6 (±2.56) seconds to 14.07 (±2.64), and throwing skills increased significantly by 28 % from 8.07 (±2.73) meter to 10.33 (±3.05) meters. Time (sec) Pre-test Post-test Fig. 1. Pre- and post-test for jumping distance. Gender difference. Time (sec) Pre-test Post-test Fig. 2. Pre- and post-test for climbing in wall bar. Gender difference. Distance (m) Pre-test Post-test Fig. 3. Pre- and post-test for throwing. Gender difference. 739

----------- Significant improvement was found in females jumping performance: pre 4.7 sec (±0.96), post 3.85 sec (±0.73), and in males result were: pre 4.4 sec (±0.64), post 4.05 sec (±0.69) (p<0.05). Significant improvement was found in females climbing performance: pre 17.9 sec (±2.57), post 15.5 (±3.20) and in males result were: pre 16.6 sec (±2.97), post 15.2 (±2.91) (p<0.05). Females throwing performance also showed significant improvement: pre 6.9 (±1.29), post 7.6 (±1.99) and in males result were: pre 10.5 (±3.21), post 11.8 (±2.73) (p<0.05). Gender difference in pretest performance was found in jumping and throwing skills, although not in climbing skills (p<0.05). Discussion The results of the current study showed an improvement of children s performance in jumping, climbing and distance throwing for the EG, while the CG did not improve in the motor skill tasks (Table I). The most noticeable improvement was found in overarm throwing with a 28% increase, followed by climbing with a 17.6 % increase and jumping with a 16.1% increase; not surprisingly, the biggest change was found in the individuals at the lowest start level. This is in line with previous reports, which have shown improvements in motor skills in the first four to six weeks of training (Beck et al., 2007). Thus, the hypothesis, that children who followed the task-specific program would improve their performance more than those who were engaged in ordinary PE sessions activities was verified for the above skills. Although time spent on task and practice trials was not calculated in this study, it seems reasonable to suggest that children in the EG could have spent more time on tasks and persisted for longer at tasks, obtaining many repetitions at each skill, thus helping to improve their motor skill performance. It is claimed that the key variable in determining skill improvement is the number of correct practice trials a student accumulates (Silverman et al.,1991; Siedentop et al., 2002). In the current study, the CG were probably not so focused on each of the specific motor skills, and they did not spend enough time practicing each skill to significantly improve their performance in this short-term program. Although the EG results were very positive in this study, individual differences were apparent. The task-specific approach has the advantage of dealing directly with the issues of motor skill performance, and general motor skill interventions have not clearly demonstrated gains in performance in gross motor skills (Revie & Larkin, 1993; Beck et al., 2007). Both boys and girls showed significant improvement in all tasks (Figures 1, 2, 3); interestingly, girls showed better post-test performance in jumping (Figure 1). The most significant gender difference in pre-test for the skills was in throwing, boys showed much better performance (Figure 3). This is in line with previous studies, and it has been concluded from meta-analysis that the forceful overarm throw was one of the few motor skills in which gender difference were clearly evident from the youngest ages (Thomas & French, 1985; Roberton & Konczak, 2001; van Beurden et al., 2002; Barnett et al., 2010). It has been argued that this difference between boys and girls is due to the effect of greater practice by the boys (Roberton & Konczak, 2001). It has been suggested that learning basic motor skills does need not much instruction and other feedback from teachers; the individual will find the optimal solution based on inner feedback, and that having enough time to practice many repetitions is essential (Siedentop, 2002). It is important to understand the factors influencing motor skill development and how to accommodate motor skill performance by manipulating the instructional environment and equipment, and modifying the task, so that effective instruction can be provided (Newell, 1984; Davis & Burton, 1991). Several factors can influence motor skill performance, such as the time spent in practice, intensity of exercise, quality of instructions and learning method, and it is difficult to identify specific intervention components that may contribute to success (Rietmuller et al., 2009). However, in primary schools, many teachers are not sufficient educated in PE. Thus, the PE teacher can accommodate the child s developmental level by manipulating the instructional environment and equipment and modifying the task (Siedentop et al., 2002; Beck et al., 2007). For example, distance throwing can be manipulated by the need to control the throw in order to hit a target at the appropriate height using the overarm technique. The findings from this study have educational implications, and are in line with previous school- and community-based programs delivered by physical education- or trained classroom teachers (Morgan et al., 2013; Philip et al., 2013). Results from the current study support the findings from previous reports that indicates the necessity of professional physical educators the in preschool and in primary stages in schools (Livonen et al., 2009). Another interesting observation was that the form teacher claimed that the intervention period had a positive influence on the learning environment. This is in accordance with research showing that motor skills influence concentration and social settings (Skinner & Piek, 2001). Due to the small sample size and because only three motor skills were assessed, the results from the study should be interpreted with caution. A more sophisticated modelling test battery, consisting of more tasks, would be more appropriate; however, these three tasks in the current study come from different parts of motor competence; object control skill, throwing and locomotor and balance skill, jumping and climbing. PE teachers may not be knowledgeable with regard to how to design and implement appropriate instruction. There is a need for future research to investigate the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. It is important to study 740

----------- the components of the interventions such as approach, amount of instruction time, and the content of the curriculum. Conclusion In summary, the result from this study suggest that a four-week specific training program can contribute to improved performance in gross motor skills like jumping, climbing and throwing. The teaching approach used here, with specific training in the motor skills, would probably have contributed to the gain made by the groups given intensive teaching of jumping, throwing and climbing. This is in accordance with previous research showing task-specific motor training programs to have a positive effect on motor skill performances. References Barnett, L. M., van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., Beard, J. R. (2009). Childhood motor skill proficiency as a predictor of adolescent physical activity. J Adolesc Health., 44(3), 252-259. Barnett, L. M., van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L.O., Beard, J. R. (2010). Gender differences in motor skill proficiency from childhood to adolescent: a longitudinal study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81(2), 160-170. Beck, S., Taube, W., Gruber, M., Amtage, F., Gollhofer A., Schubert, M. (2007). Task-specific changes in motor evoked potentials of lower limb muscles after different training interventions. Brain Research, 1179, 51-60. Catuzzo M.T., Henrique, R. S., Re A.H.N., de Oliveira, I. S., Melo, B. M., Moura, M. S., Araujo, R.C., Stodden, D. (2016). Motor competence and health related physical fitness in youth: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 19, 123-129. Davis, W. E., Burton, A. W. (1991) Ecological task analysis: translating movement behaviour theory into practice. Adapted Physical Activity quarterly, 8, 157-177. Erwin, H. E., Castelli, D. M. (2008). National physical education standards: a summary of student performance and its correlates. Res Q Exerc Sport, 79(4), 495-505. Fisher, A., Reilly, J. J., Kelly, L. A., Montgomery, C., Williamson, A., Paton, J. Y., Grant, S. (2005). Fundamental movement skills and habitual physical activity in young children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 37(4), 684-688. Fjørtoft, I., Pedersen, A.V., Sigmundsson, H., Verejken, B. (2011). Measuring physical fitness in children who are 5 to 12 years old with a test battery that is functional and easy to administer. Physical Therapy, 91(7), 1087-1095. Gallahue, D. L., Ozmun, J.C., Goodway, J. (2012). Understanding motor development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults. McGraw-Hill; 7 th ed. Boston. Livonen, K. S., Saakslahti, A. K., Methala, A., Villberg, J., Tammelin, J. J., Kulmala, T. H. (2013). Relationship between fundamental motor skills and physical activity in 4-year-old preschool children. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 117 (2), 627-646. Logan, S. W., Robinson, L. E., Wilson, A. E., Lucas, W. A. (2011). Getting the fundamentals of movement: a meat-analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. 2012. Child: care, health and development, 38 (3), 305-315. Logan, S. W., Webster, E. K., Getchell, N., Pfeiffer, K. A., Robinson, L. E. (2015). Relationship between fundamental motor skill competence and physical activity during childhood and adolescence: A systematic review. Kinesiology Review, 4, 416-426. Lubans, D. R., Morgan, P. J., Cliff, D. P., Barnett, L. M., Okely, A.D. (2010). Fundamental movement skills in children and adolescents. Review of associated health benefits. Sport Med, 40(129) 1019-1035. Mathisen, G. E. (2016). Motor competence and implications in primary school. 2016. Journal of Physical Education and Sport, 16(1), 206-209. Morgan, P. J., Barnett, L.M., Cliff, D. P., Okely, A. D., Scott, H. A., Cohen, K. E., Lubans, D. R. (2013). Fundamental movement skill interventions in youth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Newell, K. (1984). Physical constraints to development of motor skills. In J. Thomas (Ed), Motor development during childhood and adolescence (p. 105-120). Minneapolis, MN: Burgress. Okely, A. D., Booth, M. L., Patterson, J. W. (2001). Relationship of physical activity to fundamental movement skills among adolescents, Med Sci Sports Exerc, 33(11), 1899-904. Philip, P. J., Barnett L. M., Cliff, D. P., Okely, A. D., Scott, H. A., Cohen, K. E., Lubans, D. R. (2013). Fundamental movement skill interventions in youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 132(5), 1361-1383. Revie, G., Larkin, D. (1993). Task-specific intervention reduces movement problems. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 10, 29-41. Rietmuller, A. M., Jones, R. A., Okely, A. D. (2009). Efficacy of interventions to improve motor development in young children: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 124, 782-792. 741

----------- Roberton, M. A., Konczak, J. (2001). Predicting children s overarm throw ball velocities from their developmental level in throwing. Research Quarterly for Exercises and Sport, 72 (2): 91-103. Siedentop, D. (2002). Ecological perspectives in teaching research. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 21, 427-440. Silverman, S; Woods, A. M; Subramaniam, P. R. (1999). Feedback and practice in physical education: Interrelationship with task structures and student skill level. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 36, 203-224. Skinner, R. A., Piek, J. P. (2001). Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescent. Human Movement Science, 20, 73-94. Stodden, D. F., Goodway, J.D., Langendorfer, S. J., Stephen, J., Roberton, M. A., Rudisill, M. E., et al. (2008). A developmental perspective on the roll of motor skill competence in physical activity. An emergent relationship, Quest, 60, 290-306. Stodden, D. F., Langendorfer, S. J., Roberton, M. A., Kelbley, L. (2007). Association between motor skill competence and health-related physical fitness. Journal of Sport & Exercise Physiology, 29, 45-47 Sweeting, T., Rink, J. E. (1999). Effects of direct instruction and environmentally designed instruction on the process and product characteristics of a fundamental skill. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 18, 216-233. Thomas, J., French, K. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260-282. Valentini, N. C., Rudisill, M. E., Goodway, J. D. (1999). Mastery climate. Children in charge of their own learning. Teaching Elementary Physical Education, 10, 6-10. Van Beurden, E., Zask, A., Barnett, L. M., Dietrich, U. C. (2002). Fundamental movement skill How do primary school children perform? The Move it grove it program in rural Australia. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 5(3), 244-252. 742