CiNsenga Anaphora Sketch - Version 1.1. Ken Safir and Silvester Ron Simango

Similar documents
Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Words come in categories

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Control and Boundedness

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Language contact in East Nusantara

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Developing Grammar in Context

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Writing a composition

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Advanced Grammar in Use

On the Notion Determiner

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

IS THERE A PASSIVE IN DHOLUO?

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Course Syllabus Advanced-Intermediate Grammar ESOL 0352

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

SHEEO State Authorization Inventory. Nevada Last Updated: October 2011

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Direct and Indirect Passives in East Asian. C.-T. James Huang Harvard University

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Appendix D IMPORTANT WRITING TIPS FOR GRADUATE STUDENTS

Course Outline for Honors Spanish II Mrs. Sharon Koller

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Interactive Corpus Annotation of Anaphor Using NLP Algorithms

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

A Grammar for Battle Management Language

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

5 th Grade Language Arts Curriculum Map

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Beyond constructions:

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Independent Pronouns in Blackfoot 1. Martina Wiltschko, Valerie Marshall, Andy Matheson, Audra Vincent. University of British Columbia

Tagged for Deletion: A Typological Approach to VP Ellipsis in Tag Questions

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

CORPUS ANALYSIS CORPUS ANALYSIS QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Transcription:

CiNsenga Anaphora Sketch - Version 1.1 Ken Safir and Silvester Ron Simango CiNsenga has an anaphora pattern similar at its core to many other Bantu anaphora systems, though there are some fine details that suggest interesting lines of comparative research. That core consists of four main strategies for representing clausemate anaphora: the zi strategy, the null object strategy, the an strategy, and the pronoun strategy, and there are two auxiliary strategies, the mwinicozi and eka strategies, that mark emphasis or other restrictions on the anaphorically interpreted argument or on the nature of the event. Another anaphoric strategy is restricted to kinship relations and will be treated in a section that includes related constructions of note, including some constructions where possessor anaphora can be interpreted as possessor raising in some cases, perhaps as applicative in others. Long distance anaphora is achieved by the pronoun strategy. The zi strategy, or Strategy A as it is called in the AQ response (AQR), is the most generally employed strategy to achieve reflexive readings. The zi affix is morphologically instantiated by a marker on the verb, one that competes with object markers (for pronominal readings that are not understood reflexively) in the same slot (left adjacent to the verb stem, after subject agreement and tense). Noncanonical objects can become objects when applicative applies (see, for example, AQR A3g, C4bd) and then the zi strategy is used, but if no applicative is possible to promote a prepositional object, then the zi strategy is not possible (e.g., AQR C7a-c). The subject is always the antecedent of the reflexive reading when the zi strategy is used. The zi marker is invariant for all persons and numbers, showing no agreement relation. The nonreflexive object marker varies according to number and person as reported in the grammar sketch. It appears that if the zi strategy is available, then it must be the one used, unless the other option is the null object strategy, described below. There are cases where the zi strategy is used for the possessor of a direct object - see the section on possessor raising and applicative below. The null object strategy is lexically restricted to verbs such as those of grooming, for example, or for reciprocals, verbs of conflict, like fight. The direct object is missing, there is no object marker on the verb, and no morphology on the verb marks reflexivity. In those cases where the same verb can either use the null object strategy or the zi strategy, only the zi strategy permits proxy readings (compare AQR 3.8 for null object and zi strategies). The -an strategy is used for reciprocal readings. Unlike the reflexive prefix zi, the affix an is suffixed to the verb stem (preceding a final vowel) and is not inflected for person or number. It cannot cooccur with overt objects or object markers. In cases like AQR A12c,d there appears to be a form of doubling with possessors) and for AQR C29a a reciprocal relation is permitted between the subject and the object of a with PP by means of an an suffix on the verb, although this relation is not possible if there is an 1

overt direct object in VP (AQR C29a ). The eka strategy can be used with the -an strategy to render it emphatic and exclusive (i.e., and no one else, e.g. AQR 3.1 for the EKA strategy). We expect to explore -an in more detail in future follow-up work. The pronoun strategy. CiNsenga has both affixal and argument position pronouns (see AQR 2.2.2). The object affixal pronouns (object markers) are in complementary distribution with direct objects (but see the eka-strategy), but the subject affixes, which are more clearly like agreement morphemes, occur with any sort of overt subject, though independent subject pronouns are typically dropped, as in most pro-drop languages (see AQR 2.2.1). Object markers are normally used for direct pronominalization, not an argument position pronoun. As usual, a pronoun is considered independent if it does not require a sentence-internal antecedent, and in this sense of independent both argument position and affixal pronouns are independent. The pronoun strategy is used for all non-clausemate anaphora and in some cases of clausemate anaphora. Pronouns cannot be used for an anaphoric reading when the zi strategy is possible, which is any case where the dependent argument is the direct object (or is the direct object as a result of applicative morphology). It appears that pronouns are not normally used to refer to a clausemate non-subject either, but this is not impossible (but see AQR A3d,e). Thus the use of pronouns for clausemate anaphora is generally limited to prepositional object position (sometimes with an appropriate clitic-like form, as in AQR C10c), just in cases where the preposition cannot be incorporated into the verb (by a version of the applicative alternation). Subject pronouns can be argument forms for long distance coreference construals, and object markers are favored for objects coconstrued with a nonclausemate antecedent (see AQR D11). Pronouns allow split antecedents, but not when the antecedents are clausemates. Backwards coreference is generally disfavored in CiNsenga, but it is possible with pronouns, such that a pronoun embedded in a subject can be coconstrued with a direct object it does not c-command, although this is sensitive to predicate type (see the contrasts in AQR C22e,f, and Z22e,f). Principle C works as expected, although the effects may be overdetermined when the antecedent is a pronoun due to limits on backwards coreference. There is no morphological marking that enforces logophoric or de se interpretation in this language - both de se and non-de se readings use the same independent pronouns, and these pronouns do not require an anaphoric reading with any logophoric antecedent. The mwinicozi strategy appears to be an emphatic form, occurring adjacent to an independent pronoun, or else in the position where an independent pronoun would be (e.g., when the subject is pro-dropped) and its presence appears to make the pronoun a more likely candidate to be construed with an antecedent in the sentence (see AQR A3b,c), but if this is not necessary to retrieve the antecedent (as in 1 st person plural), a pronoun can be used alone (AQR A3d). This effect is not limited to clausemates, since the emphatic usage can be used for a complement subject (see AQR Y2 of 3.8 for the zi strategy), but apparently not for a complement object (AQR 2.1.4, Y1e). This strategy appears to be more necessary with clausemate arguments that cannot participate in the 2

zi strategy. Mwinicozi cannot achieve a reflexive reading without being associated with a pronoun and it is generally prohibited from attaching to a direct object, even in its emphatic use when attached to a name (AQR 2.1.4, Y1b). In isolation, mwinicozi is used to mean owner, as in (AQR 2.1.4, Y1f) and certain characteristics of its distribution suggest a parallel with English own. See AQR 2.1.4, Y1c for a discussion of its pragmatic force in relation to null argument subjects. The eka strategy, where this means something like alone and it occurs with a pronoun that agrees with that of the subject of the clause, and when the null object reflexive strategy is employed there is an agreement morpheme on eka that matches the subject. The eka strategy must cooccur with either the zi strategy, the an strategy (see AQR 2.3.2, 4.1.3.1 C18') or the null object strategy, which suggests that it is not quite a reflexive or reciprocal strategy on its own. As it is stated in the AQR, The function of eka is somewhat subtle when used in combination with Strategy A (as in A7d). As noted, eka is optional since Strategy A by itself suffices to express reflexivity. In this particular case, however, eka adds an emphatic meaning to the sentence by asserting that the activity in question was not induced (or initiated) by some outside forces. Thus the interpretation of A7d is that the children out of their own volition help themselves and (also that) they don t help anyone else. One can envisage a different scenario in which someone advises or instructs the children to help themselves: if the children wind up helping themselves, then the appropriate construction would be one in which eka is omitted. Another context in which eka would be omitted is where the children help someone else in addition to helping themselves - here eka restricts the bounds to which the help was extended. The eka strategy bounds the participants in the event sharply, and in conjunction with the reflexive it appears that the event did not have any causer other than the antecedent. When it is omitted, as in AQR 2.1.6, 2.3.2, it is possible to understand the event described to have an external cause. There are instances where eka ensures an anaphoric reading (AQR 4.1.1.2, see the variations on C3b). As mentioned in the section below, the zi strategy and the null object strategy can be employed to insure anaphoric readings for missing objects of verbs with gestural, grooming interpretations. For these verbs, it is also possible for the verb to be marked by zi and then the missing possessor of the overt direct object can be understood as possessed by the subject. When these verbs are not marked by zi in this construction, the anaphoric interpretation for the possessor is still possible, but not required. However, the presence of eka then ensures an anaphoric reading for the missing possessor. The eka strategy can also insure emphatic readings of pronouns. For example, an object pronoun can be compatible with the presence of an object marker only if the pronoun is accompanied by the eka strategy (AQR 2.2.2, Fb). This is an interesting morpheme that deserves more study. It can also be used to mean alone in the more conventional sense, without accompaniment or just x and no more (see also the comment in AQR 2.3.2). Notice that for AQR C18, inanimate subjects with reflexive readings require eka, and this may be because the volition of 3

agents is missing (stressing that the event is not externally caused). Another interesting feature of eka is that examples that permit proxy readings with the zi strategy cannot support the proxy reading with the eka strategy (see AQR 3.8 for the eka strategy). Some interesting constructions Possessor raising and/or applicative - Cases like AQR A15c,d where there is a possessor raising interpretation are interesting and deserves more study. In such cases, the complementarity between objects and the zi reflexive marker breaks down, presumably because the direct object is not being treated as a direct object, because applicative affixation has made a new direct object, or because the possessum has the status as an adjunct on the real direct object. Also, reflexivity of an inalienably possessed body part is more typically expressed by the null possessor strategy, which in this case would mean a null possessor and no morphology on the verb for an object (i.e., the possessum behaves as an object). If, however, the possessor of the direct object is not an inalienable one, as in AQR A15d, then the zi strategy must be used. It is not possible for possessor raising to be achieved with a non-anaphoric object marker (i.e., an object affixal pronoun), so the question of how apparent possessor raising interpretations arise is strictly for cases where the interpretation of the possessor is anaphoric to the subject. The status of the possessum as an argument of the verb is not clear in these cases, since it is not treated as a direct object, which led Simango (1995) to treat it as an adjunct on an object and not as an applicative construction (see also AQR C13'a and C13bi where an object marker can correspond to the body part, or even alienable part/whole relations as in C13'b, and an apparently extraneous direct object appears to function as an adjunct). Notice also with the kinship anaphor X s mother as the apparent object, the zi marker is not possible (as in AQR X3a of section 4) unless the anaphoric argument appears to be the object of an applicative structure AQR X3b, such that the possessor argument has an emphatic reading similar to a promoted-toobject benefactive. Kinship anaphora - The CiNsenga locution X s mother and X s father behave somewhat like anaphors, in that they respect some locality conditions. These terms are somewhat lexicalized: My mother, your mother, and his mother are rendered, amama, anyoko, and anyina, respectively, while my father, your (singular) father, and his father are rendered atata, awuso, and awisi, respectively. The third person form is used for all plurals, but then the kinship term is accompanied by a pronoun or name and does not have to be locally anaphoric (see AQR 4.0, X3d). There are no specialized anaphoric terms for other kinship relationships. As shown in 4.1.2.6 local kinship anaphora is unambiguously bound by its local antecedent, a nonlocal antecedent is not possible, unless as in AQR C13a the possessive morpheme is introduced, in which case there is ambiguity, even to a third person retrievable from the discourse context, though a local interpretation is preferred. There are some interesting interactions with the zi strategy (see AQR 4.0) where zi coexist with a kinship anaphor if the verb bears an applicative marker (as above). Thus while it is accurate to say that wherever pronoun s mother/father might be expected to be found there is 4

morphologically quirky spellout of the sequence his mother/father, but the behavior of these forms shows certain locality restrictions reminiscent of anaphoric domains. It would appear that the locality properties of these lexicalized kinship terms deserve more study. Some Theoretical Issues There are some theoretical and comparative issues that might be profitably explored based on the CiNsenga pattern. Many Bantu languages employ cognates of the -an- and zi affixes and some of the clausemate boundary issues can be explored comparatively, concerning, for example, the sorts of prepositions or adjunct arguments that block possessor-raising or the zi strategies, on the one hand, and the sorts of prepositions that permit the -an prefix to be doubled inside a PP. It may be significant that an is a suffix with respect to the verb stem, as are passive, causative and applicative, whereas pronouns are prefixes with respect to the verb stem. The eka strategy suggests that morphemes meaning alone might need to be added to the lexical atoms that can be anaphoric atoms (see Safir, 1996, 2004), if the eka strategy is local, though clearly not all languages use their alone morpheme this way - English and French don t, for example. The necessity of the eka strategy when the antecedent is inanimate is also interesting from the perspective of how thematic roles affect argument structure. Both the eka strategy and the mwinicozi strategy deserve more study with respect to the relationship between emphatic markers and anaphoric readings (see, for example, Koenig and Siemand, 2000). The contrast between the behavior of null anaphora and the zi and pronoun strategies with respect to proxy readings (see AQR 3.8) is of particular interest (as well as the fact that the presence of eka excludes a proxy reading even for the zi strategy) and it is explored in a comparative context in Technical Report #1 on this site. The only locality issues that appear to be of interest here concern clausemate relations and some interactions with grammatical function changing affixes like causative which permit non-coarguments to enter into the zi construction. On further study, there may be some interesting locality effects with respect to the mwinicozi strategy and kinship anaphora. 5