CAS LX 522 Syntax I. C-selection. C-selection ( Subcategorization ) C-selection. Feature checking. S-selection

Similar documents
Argument structure and theta roles

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

The Syntax of Coordinate Structure Complexes

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Part I. Figuring out how English works

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Control and Boundedness

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Words come in categories

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Som and Optimality Theory

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Developing Grammar in Context

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

Prediction of Maximal Projection for Semantic Role Labeling

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Update on Soar-based language processing

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Author: Fatima Lemtouni, Wayzata High School, Wayzata, MN

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

11/29/2010. Statistical Parsing. Statistical Parsing. Simple PCFG for ATIS English. Syntactic Disambiguation

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today!

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

Beyond constructions:

PROBLEMS IN ADJUNCT CARTOGRAPHY: A CASE STUDY NG PEI FANG FACULTY OF LANGUAGES AND LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA KUALA LUMPUR

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

What Can Neural Networks Teach us about Language? Graham Neubig a2-dlearn 11/18/2017

Noun incorporation in Sora: A case for incorporation as morphological merger TLS: 19 February Introduction.

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Second Language Acquisition of Korean Case by Learners with. Different First Languages

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

THE VERB ARGUMENT BROWSER

5 Minimalism and Optimality Theory

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts THE TEACHABILITY HYPOTHESIS AND CONCEPT-BASED INSTRUCTION

Compositional Semantics

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Thornhill Primary School - Grammar coverage Year 1-6

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Focusing bound pronouns

Twitter Sentiment Classification on Sanders Data using Hybrid Approach

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

More ESL Teaching Ideas

Writing a composition

The Syntax of Inner Aspect

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Transcription:

CAS LX 522 Syntax I Week 3b. Merge, feature checking 3.6-4.2 C-selection Verbs are recorded in the lexicon with the θ-roles assign as part of their aning. But, verbs are more selective than that. Pat felt a tremor. Pat felt uncomfortable. Pat felt that Chris had not perford well. Pat is the Experiencer; a tremor (noun), uncomfortable (adjective), or that well (sentence) is the The/Source. So θ-role does not determine syntactic category. And syntactic category certainly does not determine θ-role. So verbs also need to be recorded with information about the syntactic categor(y/ies) combine with. C-selection ( Subcategorization ) Kick needs a nominal object. Pat ed the pail. Kick has a [V] category feature, but also needs to have so form of [N] category feature indicating that it needs a nominal object. We don t want to risk interpreting as a noun, though. So, the [V] and [N] features must have a different status. On, the [V] feature is interpretable the [N] feature is just for use in assembling the structure, it is not interpreted hence uninterpretable. C-selection Not all transitive verbs (that take just one object) can take the sa kind of object. Sue knows [ DP the answer ] Sue knows [ CP that Bill left early ] Sue hit [ DP the ball ] *Sue hit [ CP that Bill left early] So know can take either a DP or a CP as its object argunt; hit can only take a DP as its object argunt. S-selection Verbs also exert semantic control of the kinds of argunts allow. For example, many verbs can only have a volitional (agentive) subject: Bill likes pizza. Bill ed the stone. #Pizza likes anchovies. #The stone ed Bill. We ll assu that this is not encoded in the syntactic features, but if you ss up with respect to s-selection, the interpretation is anomalous. To model this, we will say that if a syntactic object has an uninterpretable feature, it must Merge with a syntactic object that has a matching feature and once it s done, the requirent is t. The feature is checked. Specifically: Full Interpretation: The structure to which the semantic interface rules apply contains no uninterpretable features. Checking Requirent: Uninterpretable features must be checked (and once checked, are deleted) Checking (under sisterhood): An uninterpretable feature F on a syntactic object Y is checked when Y is sister to another syntactic object Z which bears a matching feature F. 1

To distinguish interpretable features from uninterpretable features, we will write uninterpretable features with a u in front of them. To distinguish interpretable features from uninterpretable features, we will write uninterpretable features with a u in front of them. D has uninterpretable feature F E has interpretable feature F. C D has uninterpretable feature F E has interpretable feature F. D [uf] E [F] If we Merge them, the uninterpretable feature can be checked (under sisterhood). D [uf] E [F] If we Merge them, the uninterpretable feature can be checked (under sisterhood). Or, for a more concrete example Or, for a more concrete example is a verb (has an interpretable V feature) and c- selects a noun (has an uninterpretable N feature). is a verb (has an interpretable V feature) and c- selects a noun (has an uninterpretable N feature). is a noun (a pronoun in fact, has an interpretable N feature, and others like accusative case, first person, singular) V is a noun (a pronoun in fact, has an interpretable N feature, and others like accusative case, first person, singular) Merging them will check the uninterpretable feature, and the structure can be interpreted. V The is the needy one. The one that had the uninterpretable feature that was checked by Merge. The combination has the features of the verb and so its distribution will be like a verb s distribution would be. Pat wants to. Pat wants to drive. I like to draw elephants. *Pat wants to elephants. *I like to draw. glance at Pat Pat [N, ] at [P, un, ] glance [V, up, ] 2

Chris glanced at Pat Pat [N, ] Chris [N, ] at [P, un, ] glanced [V, up, un, ] The idea Sentences are generated derivationally, by ans of a series of syntactic operations. A sentence that can be generated by such a procedure is grammatical. One that cannot is not grammatical. Syntactic operations operate on syntactic objects. Lexical items are syntactic objects. A derivation starts off by selecting a number of syntactic objects from the lexicon, and proceeds by performing syntactic operations on them. Syntactic operations Merge is a syntactic operation. It takes two syntactic objects and creates a new one out of them. The new syntactic object created by Merge inherits the features of one of the components (the projects its features). Merge cannot look inside a syntactic object. Syntactic objects are only combined at the root. The Extension Condition: A syntactic derivation can only be continued by applying operations to the root of ate tree. Syntactic objects have features. Lexical items (syntactic objects) are bundles of features. So features are interpretable, others are uninterpretable. By the ti the derivation is finished, there must be no uninterpretable features left (Full Interpretation). Uninterpretable features are eliminated by checking them against matching features. This happens as a result of Merge: Features of sisters can check against one another. Merge doesn t just happen. It has to happen. Heads and complents complent When Merge combines two syntactic objects, one projects its features, one does not. When a lexical item projects its features to the combined syntactic object, it is generally the, and the thing it combined with is generally the complent. A syntactic object that projects no further is a. Where X is the category, this is alternatively X max or XP. The complent is necessarily a. Heads and complents complent A syntactic object that has not projected at all (that is, a lexical item) is sotis a. Where X is the category, this is alternatively X min or X. The is a. In traditional terminology, the complent of a verb is generally the object (or direct object ). So, often, is the complent of a preposition ( object of the preposition ). 3

Linear order Merge takes two syntactic objects and combines them into a new syntactic object. Merge does not specify linear order (which of the two combined objects cos first in pronunciation). In the English, s always precede complents. But languages differ on this. The parater Languages generally have sothing like a basic word order, an order in which words co in in neutral sentences. English: SVO Akira ate an apple. Japanese: SOV John wa ringo o tabeta. John top apple acc ate John ate an apple. In our terms, this amounts to a (generally language-wide choice) as to whether s are pronounced before complents or vice-versa. English: -initial Japanese: -final Second Merge Merge occurs when there is a selectional feature that needs to be satisfied. If there is more than one such feature, Merge must happen more than once. As always, the node that projects is the one whose selectional feature was satisfied by the Merge. The sister of the (that projects) after the first Merge involving that is the complent (as above). The nonprojecting sister of a syntactic object that has already projected once from a is the. Specifiers, s, and complents A transitive verb like needs two argunts (the caller and the callee). We encode this knowledge by hypothesizing two selectional features for N. The first selectional feature will be checked by the callee. The second selectional feature will be checked by the caller. So, is Merged with. Specifiers, s, and complents So, is Merged with. V [un] complent One of the selectional features is checked off, the remaining features project to the new object. A selectional feature still remains. Merge applies again, Merging the new object with. Specifiers, s, and complents V [un] complent interdiate The second selectional feature has been eliminated. The sister to this second Merge is the. A node that does not project further is a. A node that has been projected and projects further is neither nor and is usually an interdiate. 4

Specifiers, s, and complents V [un] complent interdiate In English, s are on the left of the Unlike complents, which are on the right. As with the complent order, languages (arguably) also differ in the linear order of their s. However, Spec-initial order is overwhelmingly more common VOS order (Malagasy) Nahita ny mpianatra ny vehivavay. saw the student the woman The woman saw the student. Historical note: X -theory In the 70s and 80s, these ideas went by the na X -theory. Every XP has exactly one: (a lexical item) interdiate complent (another XP) (another XP) for any X (N, V, A, P, I, etc.) XP YP X X ZP complent Merge vs. X -theory The system of selectional features and Merge is preferable because it gets this structure without stipulating the template. The structure assigned to sentences is generally the sa except that for us, there no interdiate or s unless are needed. XP YP X X interdiate ZP complent Adjuncts *Pat put the book. Pat put the book on the shelf. Pat put the book on the shelf dramatically. Pat put the book on the shelf dramatically on Tuesday. Pat put the book on the shelf dramatically on Tuesday before several witnesses. So things are required. So things are not. Argunts get θ-roles and are required. Adjuncts are modificational and are optional. Adjuncts and distribution Adjuncts are relatively transparent having an adjunct does not seem to change the distributional characteristics. Pat wants to eat (quickly). Pat wants to dine. *I like to draw eat (quickly). I like to draw (happy) elephants. *Pat wants to (happy) elephants. Idea: A verb (phrase) with an adjunct is still a verb (phrase), just as if it didn t have an adjunct. Adjoin The operations Merge and Adjoin are two different ways to combine two objects from the workbench. Merge takes two objects and creates a new object (with the label/features inherited from one of them). Adjoin attaches one object to the top of another one. The linear order of adjuncts does not appear to be set paratrically, so can either before or after the object attach to. eat quickly eat eat quickly 5

The luxury of adjunction The luxury of adjunction We will also assu that Adjoin only applies to s. That is: If a syntactic object still has a selectional feature, Adjoin cannot attach sothing to it. Merge must happen first. Once all of the things that need to happen are taken care of, then you have the luxury of adjunction. Pat ate V quickly Any number of adjuncts can be added, and generally in any order. Adjuncts co in many different categories adjunct is not a category, but rather a structural description. Colonel Mustard killed V Mr. Boddy PP in the PP study with the candlestick PP before tea A phrase Complents vs. adjuncts So, a full phrase can have all of these pieces (plus perhaps so additional adjuncts) XP [X, ] XP X adjunct complent interdiate PPs seem to be freely reorderable when are adjuncts. I ate on Tuesday at Taco Bell with Pat I ate on Tuesday with Pat at Taco Bell I ate with Pat on Tuesday at Taco Bell I ate on Tuesday with Pat at Taco Bell etc But consider glance at Chris. I glanced at Chris on Tuesday *I glanced on Tuesday at Chris Ok: Why? 6