Pointing to an invisible object behind a wall: Comprehension of pointing with a bent index finger

Similar documents
A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

PART C: ENERGIZERS & TEAM-BUILDING ACTIVITIES TO SUPPORT YOUTH-ADULT PARTNERSHIPS

Cognition 112 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Robot manipulations and development of spatial imagery

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

For international students wishing to study Japanese language at the Japanese Language Education Center in Term 1 and/or Term 2, 2017

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

Unit: Human Impact Differentiated (Tiered) Task How Does Human Activity Impact Soil Erosion?

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams

ECON 365 fall papers GEOS 330Z fall papers HUMN 300Z fall papers PHIL 370 fall papers

INDEPENDENT STUDY PROGRAM

Gestures in Communication through Line Graphs

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

Welcome Prep

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

Airplane Rescue: Social Studies. LEGO, the LEGO logo, and WEDO are trademarks of the LEGO Group The LEGO Group.

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Model of Lesson Study Approach during Micro Teaching

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

The Pragmatics of Imperative and Declarative Pointing 1

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

Friction Stops Motion

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Evaluation of Hybrid Online Instruction in Sport Management

Meaning and Motor Action

S H E A D AV I S C O L U M B U S S C H O O L F O R G I R L S

Welcome to ACT Brain Boot Camp

Increasing Student Engagement

How to make successful presentations in English Part 2

Client Psychology and Motivation for Personal Trainers

Algebra 2- Semester 2 Review

Primary Teachers Perceptions of Their Knowledge and Understanding of Measurement

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Teachers Guide Chair Study

Mental Models and the Meaning of Connectives: A Study on Children, Adolescents and Adults

Good Enough Language Processing: A Satisficing Approach

Hardhatting in a Geo-World

What s in a Step? Toward General, Abstract Representations of Tutoring System Log Data

LEt s GO! Workshop Creativity with Mockups of Locations

Cognitive Apprenticeship Statewide Campus System, Michigan State School of Osteopathic Medicine 2011

Prototype Development of Integrated Class Assistance Application Using Smart Phone

In how many ways can one junior and one senior be selected from a group of 8 juniors and 6 seniors?

Human Factors Engineering Design and Evaluation Checklist

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Eliciting Language in the Classroom. Presented by: Dionne Ramey, SBCUSD SLP Amanda Drake, SBCUSD Special Ed. Program Specialist

A Metacognitive Approach to Support Heuristic Solution of Mathematical Problems

Function Number 1 Work as part of a team. Thorough knowledge of theoretical procedures and ability to integrate knowledge and performance into

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

Mathematics process categories

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

2.B.4 Balancing Crane. The Engineering Design Process in the classroom. Summary

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

Word learning as Bayesian inference

Characterizing Diagrams Produced by Individuals and Dyads

Urban Analysis Exercise: GIS, Residential Development and Service Availability in Hillsborough County, Florida

An Evaluation of E-Resources in Academic Libraries in Tamil Nadu

Adult Education and Learning Theories Georgios Giannoukos, Georgios Besas

Constructing a support system for self-learning playing the piano at the beginning stage

Examining Action Effects in the Execution of a Skilled Soccer Kick by Using Erroneous Feedback

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Infant Behavior and Development

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

Learning Lesson Study Course

Title: Improving information retrieval with dialogue mapping and concept mapping

Do students benefit from drawing productive diagrams themselves while solving introductory physics problems? The case of two electrostatic problems

with The Grouchy Ladybug

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Module 9: Performing HIV Rapid Tests (Demo and Practice)

Sound Beginnings. Questions & Answers About Teaching Children to Read

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Functional Skills Mathematics Level 2 assessment

EL RODEO SCHOOL VOLUNTEER HANDBOOK

Using Blackboard.com Software to Reach Beyond the Classroom: Intermediate

Reinforcement Learning by Comparing Immediate Reward

Piano Safari Sight Reading & Rhythm Cards for Book 1

Elizabeth R. Crais, Ph.D., CCC-SLP

Concept mapping instrumental support for problem solving

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW

COMPUTER-ASSISTED INDEPENDENT STUDY IN MULTIVARIATE CALCULUS

LESSON PLANS: AUSTRALIA Year 6: Patterns and Algebra Patterns 50 MINS 10 MINS. Introduction to Lesson. powered by

This scope and sequence assumes 160 days for instruction, divided among 15 units.

Job Hunting Skills: Interview Process

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

The Foundations of Interpersonal Communication

Workshop 5 Teaching Writing as a Process

Ergonomics of translation: methodological, practical and educational implications

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Modeling Dialogue Building Highly Responsive Conversational Agents

ONE TEACHER S ROLE IN PROMOTING UNDERSTANDING IN MENTAL COMPUTATION

Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis

Transcription:

Pointing to an invisible object behind a wall: Comprehension of pointing with a bent index finger Harumi KOBAYASHI (h-koba@mail.dendai.ac.jp) Division of Information System Design, Tokyo Denki University, Ishizaka Hatoyama, Saitama, 350-0394 Japan Tetsuya YASUDA (t-yasuda@jumonji-u.ac.jp) Department of Human Developmental Psychology, Jumonji University Sugasawa Niiza, Saitama, 352-8510 Japan Abstract Pointing is a gesture that people use to specify and convey information about objects in the environment. Previous research has mostly explored peoples comprehension and production of others straight pointing gestures, that is, pointing at an object with arm and index finger kept straight while aiming at a visible object. However, we seem to use various types of pointing in addition to typical straight pointing to denote both visible and invisible objects. This study examined comprehension of pointing with a bent index finger at an invisible object behind a wall. The experimenter pointed either at an object in front of a wall or one behind a wall with a straight index finger or a bent index finger, and the participants guessed which object was being denoted. The results were that when the participants looked at straight pointing, they thought objects in front of the wall were being denoted. However, when they looked at bent pointing, they thought objects behind the wall could be denoted. The study suggested that people have common ground in terms of interpretation of different types of pointing. Keywords: gesture; declarative pointing; common ground; non-linguistic information Introduction When we communicate with others, we often draw their attention to objects about which we wish to communicate (Tomasello, 2008; 2014). Pointing is a gesture that people use to specify and convey information about referents. For example, when one asks a friend about a landmark in the city, she will point at the landmark using her index finger or her hand. Clark (2003) discussed use of attention-getting gestures in various cases. He noted that pointing at a referent and placing a referent are both useful ways to convey information about referents, but that people index objects differently. In pointing, a person directs the addressee s attention to the referent object; for example, a customer may point at a package of a medicine that is difficult for her to reach but is easy for the clerk. In placing, a person puts a referent object in the area of an addressee s attention; for example, a customer may place a package of medicine on the checkout counter where a clerk waits. These communications are possible without saying any words. In order to communicate smoothly, people must share mutual understanding of pointing at referents and placing referents in different situations. Clark, Schreuder, & Buttrick (1983) and Clark (1996) proposed that people use common ground as implicit mutual knowledge in human communication. Common ground can include a variety of information: how people convey information using words and sentences, knowledge about the history and culture of the speakers and addressees, mutually shared knowledge about specific people and events, and knowledge of what is going on in the current communication. Thus, common ground includes both general knowledge about the world and specific knowledge of the specific task that the conversation partners are conducting. Previous research has focused mostly on language and verbally describable information included in common ground. Non-verbal information such as gestures must also be comprehended using common ground as to how people use gestures in different situations; however, usage of gestures as common ground has not yet been thoroughly explored. Some research has explored peoples comprehension and production of pointing gestures when they use some language such as demonstratives such as this or that while indicating objects in the environment (Bangerter, 2004; Coventry, Valdés, Castillo, & Guijarro-Fuentes, 2008; Coventry, Griffiths, & Hamilton, 2014). Most of pointing studies have examined typical pointing gestures, we named it straight pointing because pointing at an object is done with the addresser s arm and index finger kept relatively straight while aiming at a visible object in the environment (Coventry et al., 2008; Doherty, Anderson, & Howieson, 2004; Jaswal & Hansen, 2006; Kobayashi, 2007). In this situation, the addresser can easily share information about visible objects, using visual joint attention and common ground. How, then, can we point at invisible objects such as objects behind obstacles? In the authors lab, we observed a person pointing at a magnet pin that was attached on the other side of a steel board. The addresser bent his index finger when he pointed at the invisible, but known, magnet pin. We observers immediately understood the meaning of his pointing gesture, although pointing with a bent index finger seems relatively unusual. We might have common ground with regard to non-verbal gestures, or at least 477

knowledge about how we should interpret others various, occasionally unusual, gestures. In the case of referring to invisible objects, sharing information about the referent may be more difficult for both the addresser and the addressee because visual joint attention is difficult to establish. The role of common ground in human non-verbal gesturing seems to be more important when people refer to invisible objects. We examined people s common ground regarding pointing gestures. This study focused on comprehension of pointing with a bent index finger at an invisible object behind a panel. Because this is the first study to examine the role of a bent index finger, we did not examine the possibility that the general posture of the arm and the index finger as a whole may have a role in this study. The experimenter pointed at an object in front of a panel or behind a panel with a straight index finger or a bent index finger. There were objects either in front of or behind the panel, and the participants guessed which object was being indicated. We expected that if the experimenter used a bent index finger in pointing, the participant would interpret this as referring to the object behind the panel, but if he used a straight index finger in pointing, the participant would interpret this as referring to the object in front of the panel. The reason is that the bent index finger seems to suggest that the pointing trajectory (imaginary trajectory of pointing gesture) can go over the panel. of the table between object #2 and object #3. Participants were randomly assigned to all conditions. Fig. 1: Examples of the two types of pointing gestures: a denotes the shape of the straight pointing; b denotes the shape of the bent pointing. Method Participants Twenty Japanese undergraduate university students (M age = 21.2 years; 3 females) participated. The experiment was conducted in accordance with Tokyo Denki University s code of ethics. Procedure The experimental conditions consisted of two types of pointing (straight vs. bent) and obstacle placement (with vs. without). With regard to pointing condition, straight pointing was when the experimenter pointed at the referent with her arm extended horizontally and her index finger kept straight (Fig. 1a), Bent pointing was when the experimenter pointed at the referent with her arm extended slantwise and her index finger kept somewhat bent (Fig. 1b). The with-obstacle condition was when there was a small black opaque panel (W: 25 cm x H: 40 cm) on the table. Without-obstacle was when there was no panel on the table. Fig.2 shows the experimental setup. On the table, there were 4 small bottles (W: 2.3 cm H: 8 cm) designated 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively, on a label of each bottle. The experimenter sat on one side of the table, wearing black sunglasses during the experiment so that participants could not see the experimenter s gaze direction. The participant sat at the table at a right angle to the experimenter. In the with-obstacle condition, the panel was placed in the middle Fig. 2: Experimental setup in the with-obstacle condition. Each object was placed 10 cm apart from the adjacent bottle. First, the experimenter and the participant looked at all the bottles placed on the table. Each bottle was placed 10 cm apart from the adjacent bottle. Bottle #1 was placed 30 cm away from the edge of the side of the table where the experimenter sat. Then, the participant sat on the experimenter s chair and looked at the table. Then, the experimenter put the panel in between bottles #2 and #3, and the participant again looked at the table. Thus, the participant experienced the experimenter s view in both with the obstacle and without the obstacle conditions (Fig. 3). Then, in the straight pointing with the obstacle condition, the experimenter put the panel in between bottles #2 and #3 and said to the participant, I cannot see bottles #3 and #4. Now, I will point at one of the four bottles. Then, the experimenter pointed at bottles using either with the straight index finger or the bent index finger. With each pointing gesture, the experimenter said, Now I am pointing at something. Which bottle would you guess I am pointing at? Please answer with the number of the bottle. The participant responded orally using the bottle number. The bottle number corresponded to the distance from the edge of the table: Bottle #1 s distance was 30 cm; #2, 40 cm; #3, 50 cm; and #4, 60 cm, respectively. In addition, the bottle numbers corresponded to visible or invisible status within the with-obstacle condition: bottles #1 and #2 were visible, 478

Fig. 3: Experimenter s view during the experiment in the without-obstacle condition (left) and in the with-obstacle condition (right). Before the experiment, each participant first sat on the experimenter s chair and looked at the table with and without the obstacle to know the experimenter s views of the both conditions. and bottles #3 and #4 were invisible. Consequently, the participants answered using a scale of 1, 2, 3, and 4. In the without-obstacle condition, all four bottles were visible. Because the experimenter wore sunglasses, the participant could not see the experimenter s eye gaze. In the bent-pointing with-obstacle condition, the procedure was the same as with the straight pointing withobstacle condition except that bent pointing was used. In the without-obstacle conditions, after both the experimenter and the participant looked at the four bottles, the experimenter pointed at a bottle in random order, and the participant guessed which bottle was being pointed at. There were two pointing conditions (straight and bent), and in each pointing condition, there were two obstacle conditions (with obstacle, without obstacle); in each pointing and obstacle combination, there were four bottle (distance) trials. There were 4 blocks in the order of pointing, straight-pointing and with-obstacle, straightpointing and without-obstacle, bent-pointing and withobstacle, bent-pointing and without-obstacle. In each block, the order of the bottle was random, and there were totally 16 trials. Overall, the order of these blocks was counterbalanced between the participants. The experimenter was trained to show the same pointing gesture in either the bent or the straight conditions in the aspects of speed of the movement, the height of the wrist, and the distance from the participant s body. Results Fig. 4 shows the participant s responses when the experimenter pointed at each object in each condition. A 2 (Pointing: straight, bent) 2 (Obstacle: with, without) 4 (Referent: #1, #2, #3, #4) ANOVA was performed with the number of the bottle that the participant responded as the dependent measure. There were significant main effects of Pointing, F(1,19) = 78.042, p <.001, η p 2 = 0.804; Obstacle, F(1,19) = 6.163, p <.01, η p 2 = 0.245; and Distance, F(3,57) = 160.457, p <.001, η p 2 = 0.894. There were also significant interactions of Pointing Obstacle, F(1,19) = 6.935, p <.005, η p 2 = 0.300, and Pointing Distance, F(3,57) = 6.935, p <.005, η p 2 = 0.148. To explore the significant Pointing Obstacle interaction, the simple main effects of Pointing within each Obstacle condition and the simple main effects of Obstacle within each Pointing condition were analyzed. Pointing differences in pointing with the obstacle (F(1,38) = 34.139, p <.001, η p 2 = 0.473.) and without the obstacle (F(1,38) = 80.265, p <.001, η p 2 = 0.679.) were significant. Obstacle differences in the straight pointing were significant, F(1,39) = 13.023, p <.001, η p 2 =.255. To explore the significant Obstacle Distance interaction, the simple main effects of Pointing within each Obstacle condition and the simple main effects of Obstacle within each Pointing condition were analyzed. Obstacle difference was significant for object #4, F(1,76) = 12.552, p <.001, η p 2 = 0.142. Distance differences in the straight and the bent pointing conditions were significant, except when the experimenter pointed at objects #3 and #4 in the without-obstacle condition (p <.05). Discussion This study examined how people interpret the experimenter s pointing with a bent index finger at an invisible object behind a panel. The experimenter pointed at bottles that were placed either in front of the panel or behind the panel using a straight index finger or a bent index finger, and the participants guessed which object was being indicated. The results were that in the with-obstacle condition, straight pointing tended to be interpreted as referring to objects #1 and #2, whereas in the withoutobstacle condition, straight pointing tended to be interpreted as referring to all objects, including objects #3 and #4. However, interestingly, the situation was different when bent pointing was used. In the with-obstacle condition, bent pointing tended to be interpreted as referring to all the 479

Fig. 4: Participant s responses when the experimenter pointed at each object in each condition. Here, a denotes the straight pointing condition, and b denotes the bent pointing condition. X-axis shows the bottle number that corresponds to the distance from the edge of the table: Bottle 1 s distance was 30 cm; Bottle 2, 40 cm; Bottle 3, 50 cm; and Bottle 4, 60 cm, respectively. The bottle numbers also correspond to visible or invisible status in the with-obstacle condition: bottles #1 and #2 were visible, and bottles #3 and #4 were invisible in this condition. In the without-obstacle condition, all four bottles were visible. Y-axis shows the number of the bottle the participant responded. objects, including #3 and #4, and there was no difference between the with-obstacle condition and the withoutobstacle condition. Thus, the results indicate that participants interpret the straight pointing as referring to all objects when the panel was not present and the objects in front of the panel when the panel was present. In contrast, they tend to think the bent pointing as referring to all objects irrespective of the presence or absence of the panel. The current experiment did not disentangle if the effect could be due to the bent index finger or to the general posture of the arm and the index finger as a whole. In future research, the roles of the bent index finger per se and it and the arm as a whole must be examined. In addition, the current experiment did not perfectly control the speed of the pointing gesture. The speed of pointing may have an effect in estimating the distance of the imaginary trajectory. Future research must examine this issue. In conclusion, the study showed that people could interpret pointing at an invisible object when bent index finger was used in pointing. It suggests that people know the meaning of the bent index finger based on common ground in their interpretation of different types of pointing. Acknowledgments We would like to thank all participants who participated in our experiment. We thank Kota Kuwayama for data collection. This study was partially supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS): Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) #24530793 (H.K.), and Grant-in- Aid for Young Scientists (B) # 26870549 (T.Y.). References Bangerter, A. (2004). Using pointing and describing to achieve joint focus of attention in dialogue. Psychological Science, 15, 415-419. Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Clark, H. H. (2003). Pointing and placing. In S. Kita (Ed.). Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 243-268). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Clark, H. H., Schreuder, R., & Buttrick, S. (1983). Common ground at the understanding of demonstrative reference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 245-258. 480

Coventry, K. R., Griffiths, D., & Hamilton, C. (2014). Spatial demonstratives and perceptual space: Describing and remembering object location. Cognitive Psychology, 69, 46-70 Coventry, K. R., Valdés, B., Castillo, A., & Guijarro- Fuentes, P. (2008). Language within your reach. Nearfar perceptual space and spatial demonstratives. Cognition, 108, 889-895. Doherty, M. J., Anderson, J. R., & Howieson, L. (2009). The rapid development of explicit gaze judgment ability at 3 years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 296-312. Jaswal, V. K. & Hansen, M. B. (2006). Learning words: children disregard some pragmatic information that conflicts with mutual exclusivity. Developmental Science, 9, 158 165. Kobayashi, H. (2007). The effect of touching object parts on learning novel object part names among young children and adults. Studies in Language Sciences 6, 61-76. Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge: MIT press. Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural History of Human Thinking. Harvard University Press. 481