Pronoun Interpretation in Direct and Indirect Speech

Similar documents
Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Individual Differences & Item Effects: How to test them, & how to test them well

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Words come in categories

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Argument structure and theta roles

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Cal s Dinner Card Deals

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Abstract. Janaka Jayalath Director / Information Systems, Tertiary and Vocational Education Commission, Sri Lanka.

Eyebrows in French talk-in-interaction

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

ROA Technical Report. Jaap Dronkers ROA-TR-2014/1. Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market ROA

STT 231 Test 1. Fill in the Letter of Your Choice to Each Question in the Scantron. Each question is worth 2 point.

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

The lab is designed to remind you how to work with scientific data (including dealing with uncertainty) and to review experimental design.

Gestures in Communication through Line Graphs

CAAP. Content Analysis Report. Sample College. Institution Code: 9011 Institution Type: 4-Year Subgroup: none Test Date: Spring 2011

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Cognition 112 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

What is related to student retention in STEM for STEM majors? Abstract:

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Instructor: Mario D. Garrett, Ph.D. Phone: Office: Hepner Hall (HH) 100

Science Fair Project Handbook

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Conversation Starters: Using Spatial Context to Initiate Dialogue in First Person Perspective Games

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Language Acquisition Chart

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

OPAC and User Perception in Law University Libraries in the Karnataka: A Study

6 Financial Aid Information

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Let's Learn English Lesson Plan

Writing a composition

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Creating Travel Advice

Student Perceptions of Reflective Learning Activities

Merbouh Zouaoui. Melouk Mohamed. Journal of Educational and Social Research MCSER Publishing, Rome-Italy. 1. Introduction

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

ECON 365 fall papers GEOS 330Z fall papers HUMN 300Z fall papers PHIL 370 fall papers

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

MA Linguistics Language and Communication

A Decision Tree Analysis of the Transfer Student Emma Gunu, MS Research Analyst Robert M Roe, PhD Executive Director of Institutional Research and

How to make successful presentations in English Part 2

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Abstractions and the Brain

Quantitative analysis with statistics (and ponies) (Some slides, pony-based examples from Blase Ur)

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

HEROIC IMAGINATION PROJECT. A new way of looking at heroism

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Functional Discourse Grammar is a functional-typological approach to language that (i) has

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Vorlesung Mensch-Maschine-Interaktion

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

LQVSumm: A Corpus of Linguistic Quality Violations in Multi-Document Summarization

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Predicting Students Performance with SimStudent: Learning Cognitive Skills from Observation

Transcription:

Pronoun Interpretation in Direct and Indirect Speech Franziska Köder (f.koder@rug.nl) Faculty of Philosophy, University of Groningen, Oude Boteringestraat 52, 9712 GL Groningen, The Netherlands Abstract This study examines how the context shift in direct speech reports influences the interpretation of personal pronouns. 108 native speakers of Dutch played an online game where they had to identify the referent of the singular pronouns I, you and he that are either presented in direct speech (e.g. Elephant said I get the football ) or indirect speech (e.g. Elephant said that I get the football). The error rate and reaction time was higher for pronoun interpretation in direct speech than in indirect speech indicating that the context shift in direct speech increases the processing load. I discuss how this can be reconciled with apparently contradicting evidence from crosslinguistic and developmental studies that suggest that direct speech is the easier speech reporting type. Keywords: Personal pronouns; direct speech; indirect speech; context shift Introduction Many languages have two different types of speech reports: direct speech (1) and indirect speech (2). (1) Direct speech: Anna zei Ik ben gelukkig. 'Anna said I am happy.' (2) Indirect speech: Anna zei dat ik gelukkig ben. 'Anna said that I am happy.' In contrast to English, there are clear syntactic and lexical cues in Dutch that distinguish direct from indirect speech. In direct speech, a reported declarative sentence has verbsecond word order. In indirect speech, the report is presented in a subordinate clause with verb-final word order and with an obligatory complementizer dat ('that'). An important semantic difference between direct and indirect speech is that the context of evaluation shifts in direct speech from the actual utterance context to the reported utterance context inducing a shift in the interpretation of indexicals (like I, tomorrow, here) (Kaplan, 1989). A result of this context shift is that the quoted first person pronoun in example (1) refers not to the actual speaker (like in example (2)), but to the reported speaker Anna. Direct vs. indirect speech The present study compares the process of pronoun interpretation in direct and indirect speech. Based on different kinds of evidence, two conflicting hypotheses about the cognitive differences can be put forward. The first hypothesis is that pronouns in direct speech are easier to interpret than in indirect speech. This is based on the following observations. First, direct speech is crosslinguistically more widespread (Li, 1986). Second, in languages where both constructions exist, direct speech is acquired before indirect speech (Ely & McCabe, 1993; Nordqvist, 2001) and in natural interactions it is used much more frequently by children and their interlocutors (Köder, 2013). Especially the fact that adults use predominantly direct speech when interacting with their children, suggests that direct speech is not only easier from a speaker s perspective, but also from a hearer s perspective. Considerations from a formal semantic perspective point in the opposite direction: Since the context of evaluation needs to be shifted in direct speech, a higher processing effort is to be expected for the interpretations of pronouns in direct speech in comparison to indirect speech. In this study, processing effort is operationalized as more mistakes and higher reaction times when identifying the referent of a personal pronoun. First and second vs. third person pronouns In addition to the comparison between direct and indirect speech, I also analyze whether there are differences between interpreting first, second and third person singular pronouns in a speech reporting environment. Lyons (1968, 1977) sees a fundamental distinction between first and second person pronouns on the one hand and third person pronouns on the other hand. While I and you refer to the primary participants of an utterance situation, i.e. speaker and hearer, third person pronouns like he, she or it are negatively defined as referring to a person or thing other than speaker and hearer. This split between first and second vs. third person pronouns is supported by studies on children s comprehension of deictic pronouns that found that the understanding of third person pronouns clearly lags behind first and second person pronouns (e.g. Brener, 1983; Charney, 1980; Legendre & Smolensky, 2012). A new contribution of this study to the field of pronoun interpretation is to investigate how a first and second person vs. third person split is affected by the context shift in direct speech in comparison to indirect speech. Method Participants 108 native monolingual speakers of Dutch (56 female, 52 male, average age: 24.4 years) participated in the study. They were recruited by university related mailing lists and the digital learning environment of the University of Groningen.

Materials The experiment is designed as an online game that the participants can access via a link. It consists of short animated scenes with three animals, a dog, a monkey and an elephant. Animals were chosen as protagonists because the experiment was originally designed for children. All three animal protagonists are male, so that gender information of the third person pronoun cannot help to identify the correct referent. Following Brener (1983), I ensured that the experimental subjects are not possible referents of pronouns themselves, but observe the scenes from the outside. This guarantees comparable conditions for the interpretation of first, second and third person pronouns. In each scene, one animal utters a sentence that indicates which of the three protagonists gets a particular object. The sentences are of a certain reporting type (no report (see example (4)), direct speech (5), indirect speech (6)) and include a pronoun of a certain type (ik ('I'), jij ('you'), hij ('he')). This leads to a total of nine possible combinations of reporting type and pronoun type. The no report condition was included as a baseline to assess participants pronoun interpretation in a non-embedded environment. All pronouns are deictic pronouns with the exception of the third person pronoun in indirect speech which is an anaphoric pronoun. Procedure The experiment consists of an introduction phase and a test phase. In the introduction phase, the three animals introduce themselves and it is checked whether the participants remember their names (Hond 'Dog', Aap 'Monkey', Olifant 'Elephant'). All animals have different male voices. Then, the 18 objects used in the experiment are presented and named. 3 practice items follow to make sure that participants are familiar with the procedure of clicking on the person who receives the object. The practice items are comparable to the no report items, but contain proper names instead of pronouns (see example (3)). (3) Practice item: Olifant krijgt het boek. 'Elephant gets the book.' The test phase is split in two parts. First the 15 no report items (5 per pronoun type) are presented in random order. At the beginning of each scene, the participants see the three protagonists and one object on the screen. Then one animal walks towards a second animal and tells him who gets the object (see figure 1b). (4) No report: Ik/ Jij/ Hij krijg(t) de voetbal. 'I/ You/ He get(s) the football.' After the end of the sentence, the protagonists are presented in their original frontal position. A basket appears in front of each animal and they are highlighted with yellow bars (see figure 1c). These cues indicate that the participants can now click on the correct recipient of the object, that is the correct referent of the pronoun. Irrespective of participants choice, the object jumps into the basket of the selected person. The program records for each participant and item the correctness of pronoun interpretation and the reaction time. In the second part of the test phase, 15 direct and 15 indirect speech items (always 5 per pronoun type) are presented in random order. The testing procedure is similar to the no report items, but includes an additional step. Now one animal whispers inaudibly for the participants into another animal s ear which of the three animals gets the object (see figure 1a). The original addressee passes the information on to the third animal using either direct (5) or indirect (6) speech. (5) Direct speech: Olifant zei Ik/ Jij/ Hij krijg(t) de voetbal. 'Elephant said, I/ You/ He get(s) the football.' (6) Indirect speech: Olifant zei dat ik/ jij/ hij de voetbal krijg(t). 'Elephant said that I/ you/ he get(s) the football.' A characteristic of the direct and indirect speech items is that two utterance contexts overlap: the original utterance context in which the whispering takes place and the actual utterance context where the sentence is reported. In each utterance context, the protagonists assume different participant-roles (speaker, addressee, other person). Cues for identifying the speaker are mouth movement and the specific vocal qualities of each protagonist. The addressee can be identified by his proximity to the speaker and his body orientation towards the speaker. The protagonist with the participant-role other person is clearly spatially separated from speaker and addressee and faces a different direction. a. Whispering (only for direct and indirect speech) b. Uttering critical sentence c. Selection phase Figure 1: Test materials

For all test items, the participant-roles of the animal protagonists, their spatial position, the sentences they utter and the objects they receive are counterbalanced. Results I used linear mixed-effects regression modeling with the software R (version 2.15.1) to explain the dependent variables error rate and log-transformed reaction time. The predictor variables are the categorical variables reporting type (no report, indirect speech, direct speech) and pronoun type (ik, jij, hij). Table 1 and 2 show how a stepwise inclusion of the predictor variables to the baseline model (including the random-effect factors) improves the goodness of fit of the models. This is indicated by the decrease of the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the results of a chisquare test. Table 1: Model for error rate AIC decrease χ2 Pr> (χ2) Random-effect factors (random intercepts per subject) + Reporting type 65.4 69.4 p<0.0001 + Pronoun type 293.6 297.5 p<0.0001 + Reporting type * Pronoun 232.6 240.6 p<0.0001 Table 2: Model for (log-transformed) reaction time AIC decrease χ2 Pr> (χ2) Random-effect factors (random intercepts and slopes per subject) + Reporting type 46.5 50.5 p<0.0001 + Pronoun type 18.9 22.9 p<0.0001 + Reporting type * Pronoun 53.9 61.9 p<0.0001 For both models, including the fixed-effect factors reporting type and pronoun type as well as the interaction between the two explains significantly more variance in the data. Error rate Figure 1 shows the mean error rate for each combination of reporting type and pronoun type. Participants make almost no mistakes in interpreting the pronouns ik (mean: 0.004) and jij (0.002) in the no report condition, but they have clearly more difficulties with the third person pronoun hij (0.319). The graph also already indicates that participants tend to make more mistakes in direct speech (ik: 0.096, jij: 0.148, hij: 0.248) than in indirect speech (ik: 0.063, jij: 0.083, hij: 0.091) and more in the interpretation of third person pronouns than of first and second person pronouns. Figure 2: Mean error rate (±1SD) A post-hoc analysis with the mcposthoc function from the 'LMERconveniencefunctions' package was performed to see which combinations of reporting type and pronoun type differ significantly. Table 3 presents all results of the pairwise, two-tailed test that are significant. Note that always pairs of combinations that include either the same pronoun or the same reporting type were compared. A positive estimate means that the element at the right-hand side of the dash in the first column has a higher error rate, a negative one that it has a lower error rate. Ik Table 3: Multiple comparisons, error rate Estimate SD z value p value No Ind 3.05 0.76 4.03 0.0001 No Dir 3.60 0.75 4.79 p<0.0001 Ind Dir 0.55 0.25 2.19 0.0289 Jij No Ind 4.10 1.05 3.92 0.0001 No Dir 4.92 1.04 4.72 p<0.0001 Ind Dir 0.82 0.22 3.71 0.0002 Hij No Ind -2.16 0.21-10.45 p<0.0001 No Dir -0.54 0.17-3.23 0.0013 Ind Dir 1.62 0.21 7.79 p<0.0001 No report Ik Hij 5.67 0.74 7.63 p<0.0001 Jij Hij 6.37 1.04 6.12 p<0.0001 Direct speech Ik Jij 0.63 0.21 2.93 0.0034 Ik Hij 1.54 0.20 7.52 p<0.0001 Jij Hij 0.91 0.19 4.90 p<0.0001 For the first and second person pronouns, a clear difference between the three reporting types was found.

Participants make significantly more mistakes in both speech reporting conditions than in the no report baseline. Moreover, they are less accurate in identifying the referents of ik, jij and hij in direct speech as compared to indirect speech. A detailed analysis of the mistakes in direct speech reveals that in 93 percent of the cases, participants incorrectly evaluate pronouns with respect to the actual instead of the original utterance context. 1 These systematic mistakes suggest that participants ignore the lexical and syntactic cues that indicate direct speech and apply an indirect speech interpretation, thereby avoiding a cognitively demanding context shift. Surprisingly, participants had the highest error rate for the interpretation of third person pronouns in the no report condition. A possible explanation for this is that it might be pragmatically dispreferred to use a third person pronoun in this context without further paralinguistic (stress) or nonlinguistic cues (eye gaze, pointing) that help to determine the correct referent. The pairwise comparison also reveals significant differences between the three pronoun types. In the no report condition, the error rate was higher for hij than for both ik and jij. In indirect speech, however, no difference between first and second as compared to third person pronouns was found. The reason for this could be that third person pronouns in indirect speech are in contrast to all other pronouns in the experiment anaphoric pronouns. This is related to a different process of referent identification. While the deictic pronouns refer to the person in the extralinguistic context that occupies a particular participant-role, the anaphoric pronoun hij refers to a linguistic antecedent, namely the proper names Aap, Hond and Olifant in the reporting clause, which in turn refer to the three animals. The results indicate that anaphoric pronoun resolution seems to be a cognitive process less prone to failure. This is consistent with evidence from child language acquisition. Charney (1980) found that children use anaphoric third person pronouns correctly before deictic ones. In direct speech, not only the error rate for first and second person pronouns in contrast to third person pronouns differed significantly, also the difference between first and second person pronouns was significant. Reaction time The mean reaction time for the no report items was 1.2s, for indirect speech 1.7s and for direct speech 2.0s. Figure 3 shows the log-transformed reaction times for every combination of reporting type and pronoun type. Figure 3: Mean log-transformed reaction time (±1SD) In table 4, all significant results of a post-hoc analysis are summarized. The test only compared combinations with either the same reporting type or the same pronoun type. A positive estimate means that the element on the right-hand side of the dash has a higher reaction time. There was no significant difference in reaction time between the no report items and the indirect speech items regardless of the type of pronoun used. Participants also did not need significantly longer to process the first person pronoun ik in direct speech than in the other two conditions. However, the high reaction times for jij and hij in direct speech clearly stand out. It takes participants significantly longer to identify a referent of these pronouns in direct speech than in indirect speech and the no report condition. Only in direct speech, a significant increase in reaction time from first person to second person to third person was found. The results of the reaction time analysis support the hypothesis that direct speech is associated with a higher processing load. The context shift does however not affect all singular pronouns equally. The first person pronoun ik is interpreted equally fast as in indirect speech. Jij Table 4: Multiple comparisons, reaction time Estimate SD t value p value No Dir 0.26 0.04 6.19 p<0.0001 Ind Dir 0.26 0.04 6.23 p<0.0001 Hij No Dir 0.36 0.04 8.70 p<0.0001 Ind Dir 0.34 0.04 8.19 p<0.0001 Direct speech Ik Jij 0.25 0.04 6.62 p<0.0001 Ik Hij 0.34 0.04 8.99 p<0.0001 Jij Hij 0.09 0.04 2.37 0.0180 1 A similar pattern was not found for the mistakes in indirect speech. In only 62 percent of the mistakes (50 percent chance level), participants wrongly selected the direct speech referent.

Discussion This study found that participants make significantly more errors and have significantly longer reaction times when interpreting pronouns embedded in direct speech than in indirect speech. This supports the hypothesis that the context shift in direct speech is associated with higher processing demands. To identify the correct referent of a pronoun in direct speech, participants must shift from the actual to the original utterance context and select the protagonist with the respective participant-role there. The high number of systematic mistakes shows that there is a strong tendency to avoid this costly context shift and to evaluate pronouns with respect to the actual utterance context, analogous to indirect speech. The results of this comprehension experiment seem to conflict with findings from cross-linguistic and developmental studies on the production of speech reports that suggest that direct speech is the cognitively less demanding speech reporting type. Direct speech occurs not only in more languages than indirect speech (Li, 1986), children also acquire it earlier and use it more frequently (Ely & McCabe, 1993; Köder, 2013; Nordqvist, 2001). One possibility to reconcile this contradicting evidence is to claim an asymmetry between the production and the comprehension of speech reports. This is to say that direct speech is easier to produce for a speaker, but more difficult to interpret for a hearer. I will however argue for an alternative explanation, namely that the cognitive complexity of a speech report is dependent on the particular discourse context in which it is used. The setup of the current experiment is in my view conducive to indirect speech. Other discourse contexts might however favor direct speech. In narratives for instance, the focus typically lies on the original utterance context while the actual context of the report (narrator telling story to audience) is back-grounded. To interpret pronouns in direct speech in this discourse context requires only a representation of the original utterance context and not a shift between actual and original utterance context. To decide between the production-comprehensionasymmetry explanation and the discourse-contextdependency explanation, one could conduct a production variant of the current study. The experimental subjects assume the role of the protagonist who reports the original speaker s utterance to another person. I predict that in this discourse context, adults will use mainly indirect speech reports because that allows them to use pronouns that refer to the participants of the currently activated utterance context. With this strategy, they can avoid to refer to themselves with quoted you, to their addressee with quoted he and to the other person with quoted I. A productioncomprehension-asymmetry explanation would however predict a preference for direct speech reports. To avoid drawing too general and therefore false conclusions, the results of this study must be restricted to discourse contexts with a highly salient actual utterance context. Acknowledgments I thank Emar Maier for helpful comments. This research is supported by the EU under FP7, ERC Starting Grant 263890-BLENDS (Emar Maier). References Brener, R. (1983). Learning the deictic meaning of third person pronouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12(3), 235 262. Charney, R. (1980). Speech roles and the development of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 7(3), 509 28. Ely, R., & McCabe, A. (1993). Remembered voices. Journal of Child Language, 20, 671 696. Kaplan, D. (1989). Demonstratives. In J. Almog, J. Perry, & H. Wettstein (Eds.), Themes from Kaplan (pp. 481 614). New York: Oxford University Press. Köder, F. (2013). How children acquire reported speech in German and Dutch: a corpus study. In B. Sonnenhauser, C. Trautmann, & P. Noel Aziz Hanna (Eds.), Perspektiven Diskussionsforum Linguistik in Bayern / Bavarian Working Papers in Linguistics 2 (pp. 15 28). München, Bamberg. Legendre, G., & Smolensky, P. (2012). On the Asymmetrical Difficulty of Acquiring Person Reference in French: Production Versus Comprehension. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 21(1), 7 30. Li, C. (1986). Direct and indirect speech: A functional study. In: F. Coulmas (Ed.), Direct and Indirect Speech (pp. 29-45). Berlin, New York, Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter. Lyons, J. (1968). Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vol. 2). Cambridge, London, New York, Melbourne: Cambridge University Press. Nordqvist, Å. (2001). Speech about speech. A developmental study on form and functions of direct and indirect speech. Göteborg: Kompendiet.