DIFFERENTIATION OF HOLISTIC PROCESSING IN THE TIME COURSE OF LETTER RECOGNITION

Similar documents
Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

13 Automaticity and Second Languages

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

MD, USA Published online: 03 Jan 2014.

Meaning and Motor Action

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

Feature-oriented vs. Needs-oriented Product Access for Non-Expert Online Shoppers

Dublin City Schools Mathematics Graded Course of Study GRADE 4

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

Levels-of-Processing Effects on a Variety of Memory Tasks: New Findings and Theoretical Implications

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Probabilistic principles in unsupervised learning of visual structure: human data and a model

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Classify: by elimination Road signs

Evaluating the Effectiveness of the Strategy Draw a Diagram as a Cognitive Tool for Problem Solving

UPPER SECONDARY CURRICULUM OPTIONS AND LABOR MARKET PERFORMANCE: EVIDENCE FROM A GRADUATES SURVEY IN GREECE

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Policy Taverham and Drayton Cluster

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

QuickStroke: An Incremental On-line Chinese Handwriting Recognition System

Proposal of Pattern Recognition as a necessary and sufficient principle to Cognitive Science

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Ontologies vs. classification systems

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

A Context-Driven Use Case Creation Process for Specifying Automotive Driver Assistance Systems

Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

Relating Math to the Real World: A Study of Platonic Solids and Tessellations

Merry-Go-Round. Science and Technology Grade 4: Understanding Structures and Mechanisms Pulleys and Gears. Language Grades 4-5: Oral Communication

1. Programme title and designation International Management N/A

Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children. With and Without Language Impairments. Franklin R. Manis. University of Southern California.

Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital. Guidance and Information for Teachers

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

COMPETENCY-BASED STATISTICS COURSES WITH FLEXIBLE LEARNING MATERIALS

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Special Education Services Program/Service Descriptions

Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model

Retrieval in cued recall

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Appendix L: Online Testing Highlights and Script

Learning Methods for Fuzzy Systems

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

Encoding. Retrieval. Forgetting. Physiology of Memory. Systems and Types of Memory

An Introduction to Simio for Beginners

Longitudinal Analysis of the Effectiveness of DCPS Teachers

Seminar - Organic Computing

A Guide to Adequate Yearly Progress Analyses in Nevada 2007 Nevada Department of Education

A GENERIC SPLIT PROCESS MODEL FOR ASSET MANAGEMENT DECISION-MAKING

Identifying Students with Specific Learning Disabilities Part 3: Referral & Evaluation Process; Documentation Requirements

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Modeling user preferences and norms in context-aware systems

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Page 1 of 11. Curriculum Map: Grade 4 Math Course: Math 4 Sub-topic: General. Grade(s): None specified

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Chunk Formation in Immediate Memory and How It Relates to Data Compression

Pre-AP Geometry Course Syllabus Page 1

Professional Learning Suite Framework Edition Domain 3 Course Index

Orthographic coding in illiterates and literates. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language (BCBL); Donostia, Spain

International Variations in Divergent Creativity and the Impact on Teaching Entrepreneurship

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

TabletClass Math Geometry Course Guidebook

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

QUESTIONS and Answers from Chad Rice?

Interactions often promote greater learning, as evidenced by the advantage of working

Deploying Agile Practices in Organizations: A Case Study

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

Success Factors for Creativity Workshops in RE

Transcription:

RT (ms) DIFFERENTIATION OF HOLISTIC PROCESSING IN THE TIME COURSE OF LETTER RECOGNITION Thomas Lachmann and Cees van Leeuwen lachmann@rhrk.uni-kl.de Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Germany Laboratory for perceptual Dynamics, Braun Science Institute, Riken, Japan Abstract Pairs of letters, pseudo-letters, and geometrical shapes were presented in a sequential same different task, in which the time between the first and second items was varied. The second item was either presented in isolation or surrounded by an irrelevant geometrical shape that could be congruent or incongruent to the target. Congruence effects were obtained for shapes and pseudo-letters, but not for letters if the interval between the first and second items was short. Absence of congruence effects was interpreted as categorical influence on early visual integration processes; letters are processed less holistically than non-letter shapes. The present result indicates that categorical influence of letters depends on the time course of stimulus processing. As a highly automatized process, it is effective for stimuli appearing at a relatively fast rate, whereas, a slower rate of stimulus presentation eliminates task-irrelevant categorical influences. We investigated whether higher-order semantic knowledge influences the degree of perceptual feature integration. An important semantically informed categorical distinction in visual information is letters vs. non-letters (e.g., Burgund, Schlaggar & Petersen, 2006). Such a distinction is not easily translated into visual features. For instance, the capital A and the non-letter in Fig. 1 are similar in shape, and yet they belong to distinct semantic categories. For this reason, it is unlikely that dissociation in early processing is driven solely by pre-categorical visual features. If these distinctions, nevertheless, play a role at the level of visual feature integration processes, these might lead to differences in congruence effects (Bavelier, Deruelle & Proksch, 2000). 490 480 470 460 isolated 450 congruent 440 letters shapes incongruent Fig. 1. Letters and non-letter shapes in congruent and incongruent surroundings (left) and reaction time data (right) from van Leeuwen and Lachmann (2004).

We tested this prediction by comparing congruence effects for letters and non-letters for different task demands (Lachmann & van Leeuwen, 2004, 2008 a,b; van Leeuwen & Lachmann, 2004). Letter vs. Shape categorization In van Leeuwen and Lachmann (2004) we performed six experiments in which a speeded binary classification task was used, letter and nonletter (geometrical shapes, pseudoletters, or rotated letters) targets were presented either in isolation or surrounded by a geometrical shape. The surrounding shape was irrelevant for the task. It could be congruent or incongruent with the target regarding its geometrical form (for example see Fig. 1 left side: first column = congruent surrounded letter and shape; second column = incongruent surrounded letter and shape). When the classification required from the participants a distinction between letters versus nonletters, either explicitly (Experiments 1 3) or implicitly (Experiment 4), a negative congruence effect was obtained for letters, contrasting with a regular, positive congruence effect for nonletters (see Fig. 1 right, and Fig. 2 left). Fig. 2. Response categories used in Experiments 4 and 5 in van Leeuwen & Lachmann (2004). The conditions displayed left led to negative congruence effects for letters, whereas for conditions shown on the right side positive congruence effects were obtained. Note that in the right conditions letters and non-letters of one category are similar in shape.

In Experiments 5 and 6, in contrast, no distinction between letters and nonletters was required. Here, response categories were varied in a way allowing participants to solve the task based on a purely visual strategy. For instance, the task was to classify two letters as one and two other letters as the other category. As a consequence, letters and nonletters invariably showed a positive congruence effect. Thus, we concluded that between Experiments 1 4 and Experiments 5-6, the occurrence of negative or positive congruence effects for the same stimuli depended on the task (see Fig. 2). Feature interaction, target selection, and response competition explanations were tested against a feature integration approach. The results were explained in terms of different feature integration strategies for letters and nonletters. Fig. 3. Data from Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2004): Mean reaction times form and 95% confidence intervals (vertical bars) of same pairs of letters (lele) and nonletters (pseudo-letter, psps), and of different pairs of letter/pseudo-letter (leps) and pseudo-letters/letters (psle), for congruent and incongruent conditions, respectively. The results are displayed separately for the Dissimilar (left) and the Similar group. In Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2004) we used the introduced material in a successive same-different task, in which the material classes letters versus shapes were mixed within a pair (different responses). Again, the direction of congruence effects when the second item was a letter depended on whether distracter shape (S1) was visually similar or dissimilar to the letter configuration (S2). For illustration see Figure 3. The graph at the right side displays a condition in which implicitly a categorization requires a distinction between letter and non-letter shape (in the conditions leps and psle; e.g., between A and triangle ; implicit, because the instruction was not to distinguish between the letters and non-letters categories). This leads to a negative congruence effect for letters (leps and psle) and a positive congruence effect for pairs where both items were either shapes (psps) or letters (lele). The negative congruence effect for letters is similar to that displayed in Figure 1 (right side), for a task where explicitly a distinction between letters and shapes was required. In contrast, for the condition displayed in the left side of Fig.3, where the categorization can be managed by a visual strategy, without distinction between letter and shapes, positive effects occur for both letters and shapes.

Congruence effects depend on the time course of letter recognition The difference in congruence effects between letters and shapes were interpreted in terms of a more holistic processing of shapes (Kimchi, 1992), and a more analytic processing of letters (Lachmann and van Leeuwen, 2008 a). In the latter study we investigated congruence effects of letters and shapes in same-different task in which the first and the second item always belonged to the same category, i.e., either two letters or two shapes, same or different, respectively. The dependency of congruence effects on the time course of processing was studied by varying the presentation time of the first item (S1) and the interstimulus-interval (ISI). If the ISI was short, congruence effects were obtained for non-letters, but not for letters (see Fig. 4). The absence of congruence effects was interpreted, in accordance with the findings described before, as categorical influence on early visual integration processes; letters are processed less holistically than non-letter shapes. These results indicate that categorical influence of letters depends on the time course of stimulus processing. As a highly automatized process, it is effective for stimuli appearing at a relatively fast rate, whereas, a slower rate of stimulus presentation eliminates task-irrelevant categorical influences. Fig. 4. Mean reaction times (RT in ms) for pairs of letters, pseudo-letters, and shapes (Material). The second item of the pair was surrounded by a congruent or incongruent shape. Upper left: short presentation time of the first item (80 ms) and short inter-stimulus-interval between the first and the second item (ISI = 320 ms); upper right: long presentation of the first item (300 ms) and short inter-stimulus-interval (ISI = 320 ms); lower left: short presentation time of the first item (80 ms) and long inter-stimulus-interval (ISI = 2920 ms); lower right: long presentation of the first item (300 ms) and long inter-stimulus-interval (ISI = 2920 ms). Error bars indicate 5% confidence intervals.

Fig. 5. Congruence effects, RT(incongruent) RT(congruent), for data from Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2008a) and for four subgroups from Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2008 b): normally reading adults, children, and frequent-word reading impaired (FWRI) and non-word reading impaired (NWRI) dyslexics. Applied aspects Recently, the development of letter-specific processing was investigated (Burgund & Abernathy, 2008; Burgund, Schlaggar & Petersen, 2006). We (Lachmann and van Leeuwen, 2008 b) used the paradigm introduced above to investigate the letter processing strategies in subgroups of children with developmental dyslexia as compared to normally reading children and adults. All groups performed the successive same different task with pairs of letters and nonletters (pseudoletters or geometrical shapes), as in the study introduced above. Adults showed congruence effects with nonletters but not with letters, and children with both types of stimuli (see Fig. 5). Frequent-word reading-impaired dyslexics in addition showed dramatically slower overall reaction times. Nonword reading-impaired dyslexics showed congruence effects with nonletters but negative congruence effects with letters (see Fig. 5). The results support the notion that normal readers have established a special visual processing strategy for letters. Processing speed rather than reading expertise seems crucial for this strategy to emerge. The contrasting effects between subgroups of dyslexics reveal specific underlying deficits. Discussion We found that congruence effects differ between letters and shapes if the task requires a distinction between the categories letter versus shape or if the task requires a phonological coding (Interestingly, the direction of the congruence effect differs also between Kana versus

Kanji processing in Japanese, cf. Jincho, Lachmann & van Leeuwen, 2008). This effect arises very early (we generally proposed that interference occurs very early, e.g., Boenke et al., 2009) and could be seen as an argument against traditional serial letter processing models (Posner & Mitchell, 1967). The effect, however, depends on the time course of information processing. In the latter same-different task (Lachmann and van Leeuwen, 2009) congruence effects became increasingly positive when the period between the onset of first and second stimuli was increased. This result appeared to be independent on whether the first stimulus was visually present for a long period during the Interval (S1). This suggests that these effects are mediated by phonological encodings and/or letter name codes (Posner, 1969). They may become effective gradually and slowly by activating top down pattern recognition routines for letters. This top down activity, according to our hypothesis, has the effect of rendering the letter-specific visual representation of the second stimulus task irrelevant, so that the letterspecific processing mode can be abandoned and a holistic feature integration mode uniformly adopted for letters as well as for non-letter shapes. References Bavelier, D., Deruelle, C., & Proksch, J. (2000). Positive and negative compatibility effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 100 112. Boenke, L., Ohl, F., Nikolaev, A., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2009). Different time courses of Stroop and Garner effects in perception - an Event-Related Potentials Study. NeuroImage, 45, 1272-1288. Burgund, E. D., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2006). Development of letterspecific processing: The effect of reading ability. Acta Psychologica, 122, 99 108. Burgund, E. D., & Abernathy, A. E. (2008). Letter-specific processing in children and adults matched for reading level. Acta Psychologica, 129, 66-71. Jincho, N., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Dissociating congruence effects in letters versus shapes: Kanji and kana. Acta Psychologica, 129, 138-146. Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of holistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24 38. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2004). Negative congruence effects in letter and pseudoletter recognition: The role of similarity and response conflict. Cognitive Processing, 5, 239 248. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008 a). Differentiation of holistic processing in the time course of letter recognition. Acta Psychologica, 129, 121-129. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008 b). Different letter-processing strategies in diagnostic subgroups of developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 730-744. Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392 409. Posner, M. I. (1969). Abstraction and the process of recognition. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Advances in research & theory III (pp. 44 96). New York: Academic Press. van Leeuwen, C., & Lachmann, T. (2004). Negative and positive congruence effects in letters and shapes. Perception and Psychophysics, 6, 908 925.