CAUSATIVE VERB FORMATION IN BODO LANGUAGE

Similar documents
Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Words come in categories

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

Developing Grammar in Context

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

THE VERB ARGUMENT BROWSER

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

June 15, 1962 in Shillong, Meghalaya, India. Address: Civil Dept, Assam Engineering College, Guwahati

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

Noun incorporation in Sora: A case for incorporation as morphological merger TLS: 19 February Introduction.

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Comprehension Recognize plot features of fairy tales, folk tales, fables, and myths.

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Argument structure and theta roles

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Dissertation Summaries. Headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics: Evidence from Italiot and Cypriot (University of Patras, 2014)*

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Underlying Representations

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Control and Boundedness

PolicePrep Comprehensive Guide to Canadian Police Officer Exams

DESIGNING NARRATIVE LEARNING MATERIAL AS A GUIDANCE FOR JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS IN LEARNING NARRATIVE TEXT

Test Blueprint. Grade 3 Reading English Standards of Learning

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Basic concepts: words and morphemes. LING 481 Winter 2011

A Simple Surface Realization Engine for Telugu

ScienceDirect. Malayalam question answering system

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Child Causatives: Acquisition of Bi-clausal Structures in Japanese. Reiko Okabe UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. Los Angeles

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

On the Notion Determiner

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Handout #8. Neutralization

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

UNIT 1. Unit 1. I m only human

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Advanced Grammar in Use

Syntactic types of Russian expressive suffixes

About this unit. Lesson one

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Primary English Curriculum Framework

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

1 Nonapriorism vs. apriorism

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Rhode Island College

Alignment of Iowa Assessments, Form E to the Common Core State Standards Levels 5 6/Kindergarten. Standard

Writing a composition

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Transcription:

International Research Journal of Humanities, Language and Literature Vol. 4, Issue 2, February 2017 Impact Factor- 5.401 ISSN: (2394-1642) Associated Asia Research Foundation (AARF) Publication Website: www.aarf.asia Email : editor@aarf.asia, editoraarf@gmail.com CAUSATIVE VERB FORMATION IN BODO LANGUAGE Dr. Daimalu Brahma Centre for Endangered Languages, Tezpur University, India. ABSTRACT The present paper is to describe causative verb formation of the Bodo language. It belongs to the Bodo-Garo sub-group of Tibeto-Burman sub family of languages (Benedict, 1972). Bodo has many monosyllabic intransitive verbs which form causative verbs with the help of adding prefixes. The paper will focus how the intransitive and transitive verb forms the causative verb in Bodo language. Bodo uses causative prefix and suffix to form causative verbs and some of the causative verbs are formed by the process of suppletion. The paper will discuss causativization by affixation and causativization by suppletion. It will be noticed here direct and indirect causativization in Bodo language. The paper tries to focus on causative construction of the Bodo language. Most of the data of this paper are collected from the primary sources i.e. standard native speakers in different age, groups and sex during my PhD research work (2011-2016). Standard Bodo speakers are found mainly in Kokrajhar and Chirang districts of Assam. KEYWORDS - Bodo Language, Causative Formation, Affixation, Suppletion Direct- Indirect Causativization 1. THE LANGUAGE The Bodo is one of the major tribes of Northeastern India. The people who speak this language calls Bodo language. It is one of the Tibeto-Burman languages. G. A. Grierson (1903), Benedict, (1972), Joseph and Burling (2006) describe that Bodo belongs to the Bodo- Garo sub-branch of the Tibeto-Burman language family. The language closely connects with the cognate languages i.e. Dimasa, Kokoborok, Bru (Reang), Garo, Rabha and Tiwa. It is a recognized language in the 8 th Schedule of the Indian Constitution. Genetically, Bodo 1 P a g e

belongs to the Mongoloid stock of the Indo-Mongoloids. According to P. C. Bhattacharya (1977:1) the Bodo word is first applied by Hodgson. The Bodo is known as Bodo or Boro, Kachary, Kirata, Mech etc. in different places and at different times. In course of time, they came to be known as simply Bodo or Boro. The Bodo speakers are mainly found in Assam as well as some adjacent areas of West Bengal, Meghalaya, Arunachal Pradesh, Nagaland and some parts of Nepal and Bhutan. According to 2001 census of India the total population of Bodo speakers in Assam are 13, 52,771 having 61.3% literary rate. Bodo has some dialects but also they are mutual intelligibility viz. Kokrajhar and Chirang area s dialect that is Standard Bodo language, West-Bengal area s dialect that is called Mech or Mes, Nepal area s dialect that is called Meche or Mesi, Gowalpara dialect, Udalguri area s dialect that is called Sanzari, Bengtol area s dialect that is called Bwrdwn. 2. DEFINITION OF CAUSATIVE FORMATION Causative verbs are found in many languages. Thus, studies on the causative formation become an important area in Linguistics. Comrie (1981) points out that causative construction have played an important role in the recent history of Linguistics, not only from a typological viewpoint, and also represent an important area of convergence between Linguistics and such adjacent disciplines as Philosophy and Cognitive Anthropology. According to him, any causative situation involves two component situations, the cause and its effect, for example in English John caused me to be late: here, the effect is clearly that I was late and in that sentence John is the instance of cause. According to Crystal (1980), causative refers to the causal relationship between alternative versions of a sentence. For example, the pair of sentence the cat killed the mouse and the mouse died are related, in that the transitive kill can be seen as a causative version of the intransitive die. Comrie (1985) Dixon (1979), Blake (1987) and Austin (2005) clearly mention that transitive verbs can be different from the intransitive verbs in many languages. Intransitive verbs may be converted into the transitive verbs by regular morphological process known as causativization or causative formation. Comrie (1981) states there are three types of causatives i.e. morphological causative, lexical causative and periphrastic causatives. Morphological causative forms by adding affixes to the verbs, lexical causatives are formed by suppletion. There is no morphological similarity between the base verb and the causative verb in lexical causatives. Periphrastic causative indicates causation with the help of verb. For example in English Ram made the child cry here, the verb make is expressing the causative action along 2 P a g e

with the verb eat. The present paper intends to provide a detailed description of the causative verb formation of the Bodo language. 3. TYPES OF CAUSATIVE FORMATION Generally, causative verbs are formed by adding affixes in Bodo-Garo languages. Kok- Borok, uses both causative prefix and suffix e.g. ran be dry p h i -ran make dry, munui laugh munui-ri cause to laugh (Debbarma 2014: 137-138). Tiwa has causative prefix e.g. nu see p h u-nu show (Baishya & Muchahary 2015). Dimasa has both causative prefix and suffix e.g. nu see p h u-nu show (Barman 2012: 64) and nai look nai-ri cause to look (Longmailai, elicited data). Morphologically, the Bodo language has also both causative prefix and suffix use to form causative verbs. There are three types of causative verb formation processes found in the language. The processes are namely prefixation, suffixation and suppletion. These are discussed below with the help of examples. 3.1. CAUSATIVIZATION BY PREFIXATION Bodo has more causative prefixes than suffix. Most of the intransitive verbs are converted into causative verbs by prefixing the causative prefixes /p h ɯ-/, /p h o-/, /p h a-/, /p h u-/, /p h e-/, /p h i-/, /sɯ-/, /si-/and /so-/. Except for /p h ɯ-/ and /sɯ-/, all other prefixes having /p h V-/ and /sv-/ can be assimilated by vowel phoneme of the verbal base. The vowel phoneme /ɯ/ cannot be influenced by other vowel phonemes in Bodo language. Hence, the prefixes /p h ɯ-/ and /sɯ-/ are added to the verbal base without modifying in causative formation. The following examples (1a) to (1g) have shown a vowel assimilation of the causative prefixes and the examples (1h) to (1k) have shown without vowel assimilation of the causative prefixes. Intransitive verb Causative verb 1a. /zɯb/ complete /p h ɯ-zɯb/ cause to complete b. /dob/ to bend /p h o-dob/ cause to bend c. /ham/ be good /p h a-ham/ make good d. /ruŋ/ be loose /p h u-ruŋ/ make loose e. /der/ to be big /p h e-der/ make large/ nourish f. /gidiŋ/ to move /p h i-gdiŋ/ or /p h i-diŋ/ cause to move g. /gó/ to out /so-gó/ or /so-k h ó/ make out/ make lose 3 P a g e

h. /rán/ to dry /p h ɯ-rán/ make dry i. /lao/ to be long /p h ɯ-lao/ make long j. /máo/ to jerk /sɯ-máo/ cause to jerk/ shack k. /gáb/ cry /sɯ-gáb/ cause to cry A causative formation indicates that a subject causes someone to perform an action or one cause a change in state of a non-volitional event. The following sentence examples (2a) to (2e) have shown the action of difference between intransitive verb and causative verbs in Bodo language. Table 1: Intransitive verbs and causative verbs Intransitive verbs 2a. /k h amani-ya zɯb-bai/ work- NOM complete-pfrf Work has completed b. /ɯuwa-ya dob-bai/ bamboo-nom bend- PERF. Bamboo has curved. 2c. /got h o-wa der-bai/ child-nom. be big- PERF. The child has become young. d. /bi-yɯ gab-dɯŋ/ he- NOM. cry- RLS. He is crying e. /zwuse raŋ-a gó-bai/ hundred rupees- NOM. lose- PERF. The hundred rupees has lost /Ram-a Causative verbs k h amani-k h ɯu p h ɯ-zɯb-bai/ Ram- NOM. work- ACC CAUS-complete-PERF Ram has made the work completed /bi-yɯ ɯuwa-k h ɯu. p h o-dob-bai/ he-nom. bamboo-acc CAUS -bend- PERF. He/she has made the bamboo curved /bima-ya got h o-k h ɯu p h e-der-bai/ Mother-NOM. child- ACC Mother has made the child young. CAUS-be big- PERF. /aŋ-ɯ bi-k h ɯu sɯ-gab-dɯŋ-mɯn/ I- NOM. he- ACC CAUS-cry- RLS-PAST I made him cry /bi-yɯ zwuse raŋ-k h ɯu so-k h ó-bai/ he- NOM. hundred rupees-nom. CAUS-lose- PERF. He has made the hundred rupees lost In Bodo, some of the transitive verbs are also converted into causative verbs by adding prefixes /p h ɯ-/ and /si-/. The examples from (3a) to (3b) have shown how the transitive verbs are converted into causative verbs in this language. 4 P a g e

Table 2: Transitive verbs and causative verbs Transitive verbs 3a. /bi-yɯ p h ulis-k h ɯu gi-yw/ he- NOM. police- ACC. fear-hab. He fears the police b. /zi-ya bisina-k h ɯu lɯm-ɯ/ cloth- NOM. bed- ACC. cover-hab. The cloth full covers the bed. Causative verbs /p h ulis-a bi-k h ɯu si-gi-yw/ police- NOM. he- ACC. CAUS.-fear- HAB. Police causes to fear him or The police frighten him /aŋ-ɯ bisina-k h ɯu p h ɯ-lɯm-ɯ/ I- NOM. bed- ACC. CAUS-cover- HAB. I make the bed full cover. 3.2. CAUSATIVIZATION BY SUFFIXATION There is one causative suffix found in Bodo i.e. /-hɯ/. It expresses causative action by someone while it is added to the transitive verbs. It can derive causative verb by adding to the transitive and intransitive verbs. The examples (4a) to (4c) have shown causative verbs derive from transitive verb and examples (4d) to (4f) have shown causative verbs from intransitive verbs. Transitive verb Causative verb 4a. /lir/ write /lir-hɯ/ cause to write (throw someone) b. /lɯŋ/ drink /lɯŋ-hɯ/ cause to drink (through someone) c. /nai/ look /nai-hɯ/ cause to look (through someone) Intransitive verb Causative verb 4d. /zó/ sit /zó-hɯ/ cause to sit e. /duŋ/ to be hot /duŋ-hɯ/ cause to make hot f. /gáb/ cry /gáb-hɯ/ cause to cry There is a difference between derived causative verbs from the transitive and intransitive verbs. If the /-hɯ/ is added to the transitive verb it shows that action is done by the subject with the help of someone or somebody. If the /-hɯ/ is added to the intransitive verb it shows that action is done by the subject itself, in this case subject is the core participant agent and the action is done by subject without taking help. 5 P a g e

3.3. Causativization by Suppletion There are some causative verbs found in Bodo which are not similarity between the basic verbs and its causative counterpart. Theoretically (Comrie & Dixon) it can be called lexical causatives. The lexical causatives are formed by the process of suppletion. According to Crystal (1980) suppletion is a term used in morphology to refer to cases where it is not possible to show a relationship between morphemes through a general rule, because the forms involved have different roots. It is to be noticed here that if the non-causative basic verbs begin with unaspirated stop velar sound /g/, it is replaced by the aspirated stop velar sound /k h / in causative verb formation. The following examples (5a) to (5f) have shown how the /g/ is replaced by /k h /, i.e., from voiced unaspirated to voiceless aspirated, during causative formation. Non-causative Verb Causative Verb 5a. /gɯlɯi/ fall down /k h ɯlɯi/ cause to make down b. /gau/ to split /k h au/ make split c. /gu/ to be loose /k h u/ make loose d. /gɯma/ to lose /k h ɯma/ make lost e. /geŋ/ to open (window) /k h eŋ/ make open f. /geu/ to loose (bundle) /k h eu/ make loose The examples (5g) to (5l) have shown another type of suppletion. Here, causative verbs are totally different from the non-causative verbs which are derived from different roots. 5g. /za/ eat /dɯu/ cause to eat/ feed h. /hab/ to enter /sɯ/ cause to enter i. /k h am/ to burn /sau/ cause to burn j. /zo/ to section /so/ make section k. /undu/ sleep /p h ut h u/ make sleep l. /t h ɯi/ die /sit h ar/ kill 3.4. DIRECT AND INDIRECT CAUSATIVIZATION The direct causativization is trying to focus a direct participant of the subject that means causative action is directly done by the subject itself and indirect causativization is trying to 6 P a g e

highlight here indirectly participant of the subject. In Bodo, there are two kinds of causative action found i.e. directly participant by the agent (subject) and indirectly participant by the agent. When an agent directly participants the causative action then causative prefixes are attached to the intransitive verb. If the agent indirectly participants causative action then the causative suffix /-hɯ/ is attached to the transitive verbs. Morphologically, the direct causativization process in Bodo is Causative Prefix + Intransitive Verb > Direct Causativization. Some of the direct causatives are derived by the process of suppletion (see the examples (5a) to (5l)). The following examples (6a) to (6d) consider as direct and indirect causativization from the intransitive verb. Intransitive Verb Direct Causative Indirect Causative 6a. /hai/ /p h a-hai/ /p h a-hai-hɯ/ be down make down make down (through someone) b. /suŋ/ /p h u-suŋ/ /p h u-suŋ-hɯ/ be short make short make short (through someone) c. /t h aŋ/ /p h ɯ-t h aŋ/ /p h ɯ-t h aŋ-hɯ/ preserve make alive make alive (through someone) d. /máo/ /sɯ-máo/ /sɯ-máo-hɯ/ to jerk cause to jerk/ shack cause to jerk (through someone) The order of causativization in the set of examples of indirect causative from (6a) to (6d) occurs as Causative Prefix + Intransitive Verb + Causative Suffix > Indirect Causativization. The sentence examples (7a) to (7e) have shown the difference between direct and indirect causativization in Bodo language. Table 3: Direct and Indirect causativization Direct causativization 7a. /aŋ bi-ni k h oro-k h ɯu I he- GEN. head-acc. p h a-hai-bai/ CAUS-down- PERF Indirect causativization /aŋ Dabla-nɯ bi-ni k h oro-k h ɯu I Dabla-DAT. he-gen. head-acc. p h a-hai-hɯ-bai/ CAUS.-down- CAUS.-PERF. 7 P a g e

I have made his head down I have made his head down throw Dabla b. /bi-yɯ duruŋ-k h ɯu he-nom. rope-acc. p h u-suŋ-bai/ CAUS.-be short-perf. He has made the rope short. c. /dakt h ar-a rugi-k h ɯu Doctor-NOM. patient- ACC. p h ɯ-t h aŋ-dɯŋ-mɯn/ CAUS.-preserve-RLS-PAST Doctor made the patient alive. d. /Ram-a k h unt h ia-k h ɯu Ram-NOM. post-acc. sɯ-máo-dɯŋ/ CAUS.-jerk-RLS Ram made the post jerk. e. /aŋ got h o-nɯ ɯŋk h am dɯu-dɯŋ/ I child-dat. rice feed-rls. I fed the rice to the baby. /bi-yɯ Gobla-nɯ duruŋ-k h ɯu He-NOM. Gobla-DAT. rope-acc. p h u-suŋ-hɯ-bai/ CAUS.-be short- CAUS.-PERF. He has made the Gobla shorten the rope. /aŋ-ɯ dakt h ar-nɯ rugi-k h ɯu I-NOM. Doctor-DAT. patient-acc p h ɯ-t h aŋ-hɯ-dɯŋ-mɯn/ CAUS.-preserve- CAUS.-RLS-PAST I made the Doctor preserve the patient alive. /Ram-a Gobla-nɯ k h unt h ia-k h ɯu Ram-NOM Gobla-ACC. post-acc sɯ-máo-hɯ-dɯŋ/ CAUS.-jerk- CAUS.- RLS. Ram made the Gobla jerk the post /aŋ got h o-nɯ bima-k h ɯu ɯŋk h am dɯu-hɯdɯŋ/ I child-dat mother-acc rice feed- CAUS-RLS I made the baby eat the rice through mother. 4. CONCLUSION On the basis of analysis undertaken the following conclusions on Bodo causative verbs formation can be drawn. a. There are three type causative verb formation process found in Bodo i.e. prefixation, suffixation and suppletion. b. Causative prefixes are found more than suffix in Bodo. The prefixes are /p h ɯ-/, /p h o-/, /p h a-/, /p h u-/, /p h e-/, /p h i-/, /sɯ-/, /si-/and /so-/. Except for /p h ɯ-/ and /sɯ-/, all other prefixes can be assimilated by vowel phoneme of the verbal base (see (1a) to (lk)). c. A few transitive verbs can be converted into causative verbs by adding prefixes (see (3a) to (3b)). 8 P a g e

d. Bodo has one causative suffix i.e. /-hɯ/. It can be added to the transitive and intransitive verbs. If it is added to the transitive verb it indicates the action is done by subject with the help of someone. If it is added to the intransitive verb it shows the action is done by the subject itself (see (4a) to (4f)). e. The lexical causatives are formed by the process of suppletion in Bodo. When noncausative basic verbs begin with unaspirated stop velar voiced sound /g/, it is replaced by the aspirated stop velar voiceless sound /k h / in causative verb formation. f. According subject participant in the sentence, there are direct and indirect causativization found in Bodo (see (7a) to (7e)). The direct causativization process in Bodo is Causative Prefix + Intransitive Verb > Direct Causativization and the indirect causativization process is Causative Prefix + Intransitive Verb + Causative Suffix > Indirect Causativization. ABBREVIATIONS High Tone = Level Tone = unmark Low Tone = Accusative Case = ACC. Causative = CAUS Dative Case = DAT. Habitual = HAB. Genitive Case = GEN Nominative Case = NOM. Perfective = PERF. REFERENCES Adams, V. (1973). An Introduction to Modern English Word-Formation. Longman Group Ltd., London. Aronoff, M. (Reprinted 1985). Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.... (1994) Morphology by Itself Stems and Inflectional Classes. MIIT Press, Massachusetts, USA. Austin, P. K. (2005). Causative and applicative constructions in Australian Aboriginal Languages. SOAS, University of London. Baishya, A. K. (2008). Word Formation in Dimasa. International Journal of Dravidian Linguistics Vol. 37, No. 1. Baishya, A. K. & M. Laheram. (2015). Word Formation Processes in Tiwa. International 9 P a g e

Journal of Dravidian Linguistics, Vol. 44, No. 2. Baker, Mark C. (2004). Lexical Categories Verb, Nouns, and Adjectives. Cambridge University Press. Barman, B. Language Maintenance Among Linguistic Minorities: A case study of Dimasas of Barak Valley. (Unpublished PhD Thesis), Assam University, Silchar, 2012. Baro, M. R. (1990). Structure of Boro Language. Guwahati: N. L. Publication. Barua, B. K. (1967). A Cultural History of Assam. Vol.-1 (Early Period). Lawyer Book Stall, Gauhati, Assam. Basumatary, Phukan. (2005). An Introduction to the Boro Language. New Delhi: Mittal Publications. Bauer, L. (1983). English Word Formation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Beck, D. (1996). Transitivity and causation in Lushootseed Morphology. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 41, No. 2. Benedict, P. K. (1972). Sino-Tibetan. A Conspectus. Contributor editor, James A. Matisoff, Cambridge University Press, New York. Bhattacharya, P. C. (1977). A Descriptive Analysis of the Boro Language. Guwahati: Gauhati University, (Reprinted 2007). Blake, B. (1987). Australian Aboriginal Grammar. London: Croom Helm. Booij, G. (2005). The Grammar of Words. Oxford University Press. Boro, A. (2 nd Ed. 2010). Folk Literature of Bodos. N. L. Publications. Brahma, D. (2014). Inflectional Processes of the Tense and Aspect in Bodo, available at: http://www.languageinindia.com/jan2014/tenseaspectbodo.pdf (January 2014)... (2015). The Inflectional Affixes in Bodo, in Mishra, A. K., Saikia, S. & Mishra, S. K. (Eds.), Studies in Language, Literature and Culture of Northeast India (pp. 57-77). Lakshi Publishers & Distributors, New Delhi. (2016). Adverb formation process of the Bodo Language, available at: http://www.languageinindia.com/feb2016/daimalubodoadverbformation.pdf (February 2016). Brahma, K. (2009). A study in Cultural Heritage of the Boros. Guwahati, Bina Library. Burling, R. (1961). A Garo Grammar. Pune, Deccan Collage. Burling, R. (2003). The Language of the Modhupur Mandi (Garo). Published by 10 P a g e

University of Michigan. Chatterji, S. K. (2 nd Ed. Reprinted 2011). Kirata Jana Kriti. The Asiatic Society, Kolkata. Comrie, B. (1981). Language Universals and Linguistics Typology: Syntax and Morphology. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Comrie, B. (1985). Causative verb formation and other verb-deriving morphology. Language typology and syntactic description. Crystal, D. (4 th Ed. Reprinted 2000). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. Blackwell Publishers. Debbarma, S. (2014). The Structure of Kokborok. (Unpublished PhD Thesis), Assam University, Silchar. Dixon, R. M. (1979). Ergativity. Language, 59-138. Endle, R. S. (2 nd Ed. 2012). The Kacháris. Guwahati: Bina Library. Gait, S. E. (Reprinted 2008) A History of Assam. Eastern Book House, Panbazar Guwahati, Assam. Grierson, G.A. (1903). Linguistics Survey of India. Vol.-III, Part-II, Delhi: D.K. Publishers Distributors P. Ltd. Haokip, P. (2014). Word Formation in Thadou. Himalayan Linguistics. Vol. 11(2). Harley, H. (2006). English Words: A Linguistics Introduction. Blackwell Publishing. Katamba, F. (1993). Morphology. Houndsmill: The Macmillian Press. Kiryu, K. (2008). An Out Line of the Meche Language grammar, text and glossary. A Project Report of the Mimasaka University, Japan. Lieber, R. (2009). Introducing Morphology. Cambridge University Press. Lyons, J. (Reprinted 2009). Language and Linguistics: An Introduction. Cambridge University Press. Matthews, P. H. (1974). Morphology: An Introduction to the Theory of Word-Structure. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.... (1997). The Concise Oxford Dictionary of Linguistics. Oxford University press. Plag, I. (2003). Word Formation in English. Cambridge University Press. Robins, R.H. (4th Ed. 1989). General Linguistics: An Introductory Survey. Longman, London and New York. Saha, S. B. (1988). Origin & Structure of Kakbarak: A Tribal Language. Rupali Publishing, Pyaribabur Bagan, Agartala. 11 P a g e

Shopen, T. (2 nd Ed. 2007). Language Typology and Syntactic Description.Vol. I: Clause Structure. Cambridge University Press.... (2 nd Ed. Reprinted 2008). Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Vol. II, Cambridge University Press.... (2 nd Ed. Reprinted 2008). Language Typology and Syntactic Description, Vol. III. Cambridge University Press. Singh, R. A. (1982). An Introduction to Lexicography. Central Institute of Indian Languages, Mysore. Singha, Kh. D. The Structure of Dimasa: A Typological Study. (Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis), Assam University, Silchar, 2002 Stekauer, P., Valera, S. & Kortvelyessy, L. (2012). Word- Formation in the World s Languages: A Typological Survey. Cambridge University Press. 12 P a g e