Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Similar documents
Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Control and Boundedness

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Writing a composition

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Developing Grammar in Context

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Words come in categories

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

Argument structure and theta roles

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Advanced Grammar in Use

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

On the Notion Determiner

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

EAGLE: an Error-Annotated Corpus of Beginning Learner German

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Som and Optimality Theory

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Interdisciplinary Research - Challenges and Opportunities for Actuarial Profession. Aldona Skučaitė, lecturer Vilnius university

Structure-Preserving Extraction without Traces

Feature-Based Grammar

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Comprehension Recognize plot features of fairy tales, folk tales, fables, and myths.

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

cmp-lg/ Jul 1995

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

THE VERB ARGUMENT BROWSER

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Interactive Corpus Annotation of Anaphor Using NLP Algorithms

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Programma di Inglese

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

LQVSumm: A Corpus of Linguistic Quality Violations in Multi-Document Summarization

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

Second Language Acquisition of Complex Structures: The Case of English Restrictive Relative Clauses

Word Formation is Syntactic: Raising in Nominalizations

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

SAMPLE. Chapter 1: Background. A. Basic Introduction. B. Why It s Important to Teach/Learn Grammar in the First Place

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark

Pye, C The Focus Antipassive in Quiché Mayan, Kansas University Working Papers in Linguistics.

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

The building blocks of HPSG grammars. Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) HPSG grammars from a linguistic perspective

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

LFG Semantics via Constraints

Compositional Semantics

INTRODUCTION TO MORPHOLOGY Mark C. Baker and Jonathan David Bobaljik. Rutgers and McGill. Draft 6 INFLECTION

Intervention in Tough Constructions * Jeremy Hartman. Massachusetts Institute of Technology

ON THE SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS

Appendix D IMPORTANT WRITING TIPS FOR GRADUATE STUDENTS

FOCUS MARKING IN GREEK: SYNTAX OR PHONOLOGY? Michalis Georgiafentis University of Athens

Transcription:

0 Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider Sentences Brian D. Joseph The Ohio State University Abbreviated Title Grammatical Relations in Greek consider Sentences Brian D. Joseph Department of Linguistics, Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio 43210-1298 +1-614-292-4981 +1-614-292-8833 joseph.1@osu.edu Word Count: 2691 Word Equivalents: 2691

1 Chapter 12 Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider Sentences Brian D. Joseph 12.1 Introduction A hallmark of Relational Grammar (RG) has always been that it states syntactic generalizations directly in terms of grammatical relations rather than by reference to word order or to other sorts of configurational representations. Moreover, within RG, it has been a basic tenet that this reference to grammatical relations need not be restricted just to the superficial relations one can observe in the actual production of a sentence; rather reference to relations at different levels of syntactic analysis (known as strata ) is often needed. In this tribute to David, I offer a brief analysis of some facts from Modern Greek in which reference to nonsuperficial grammatical relations, i.e. grammatical relations at a level other than the final stratum (roughly, the surface structure), is needed. Moreover, this is so even in a sentence that appears to be monostratal in its syntax, i.e. to have a rather flat syntactic structure with the syntax essentially read off of the surface, a sentence that seemingly can be generated just by a simple phrase structure rule. The sentence type in question is illustrated in (1): 1 (1) (eγo) θeoro ton jani eksipno I/NOM consider/1sg the John/ACC smart/acc.masc.sg

2 I consider John smart. Such a sentence, with a verb θeoro consider, an accusative NP object, ton jani John and an adjective eksipno smart predicated of the object, might be argued to be a relatively flat structure with ton jani governed by the verb and eksipno as an adjunct modifying jani; indeed, accusative case-marking on ton jani is expected for the object of a verb and masculine accusative singular is the expected form of an adjective modifying a noun of that sort. Admittedly a small clause analysis could be entertained for such a sentence, but even that is not fully biclausal. There are two fuller sentence-types that (1) seems to be related to, with a complete clausal complement; these are given in (2): (2) a. θeoro pos o janis ine eksipnos consider/1sg COMP the John/NOM is/3sg smart/nom.masc.sg I consider that John is smart b. θeoro ton jani pos ine eksipnos consider/1sg the John/ACC COMP is/3sg smart/nom.masc.sg I consider John to be smart (literally: I consider John that (he) is smart ) Such sentences also contribute to the impression that (1) is a monostratal and basically flat (S => (NP) V NP) structure, since it is clear in (1) that there is no other

3 verb besides θeoro and that moreover complement clauses with verbs are possible with θeoro. The English parallel to (1), given in (3): (3) I consider John smart while the object of considerable discussion in the literature (especially regarding a small-clause, e.g. Stowell 1981 and 1983, versus a predication analysis, e.g. Williams 1980 and 1983), offers some evidence showing it to be monostratal. 2 In particular, English seems to allow tough-movement on direct objects only with nonderived ( thematic ) objects, as argued by Berman (1973), 3 and as demonstrated by the sentences in (4): (4) a. *Mary is easy to give e presents (compare: Mary is easy to give presents to e / Presents are easy to give e to Mary) b. *John is hard to believe e to have committed that crime Importantly, tough-movement on John in (3) is perfectly grammatical, as in (5): (5) John is easy to consider e smart

4 a fact which suggests that John in that sentence is a thematic, i.e. underlying, object of consider, and that English sentences like (3) therefore are monostratal and thus relatively flat in structure. A consideration of some additional facts, however, reveals that the Greek sentence in (1) is different from its apparent English counterpart in (3) with respect to monostratalness and flatness of structure. That is, it turns out that there is a process in the language one type of reflexivization for which crucial reference must be made to the status of ton jani in (1) in terms of the grammatical relation it bears at different levels of analysis ( strata ); in this reference to multiple levels, ton jani must be specified as a superficial object that is underlyingly a subject, in RG terms, a final stratum 2 (object) that is also an initial stratum 1 (subject). The structure of (1) is therefore more complex than its superficial would suggest and more so too than the English (3). 12.2 Reflexivization in Greek Greek has two types of reflexivization. There is a syntactic construction that makes use of the reflexive nominal ton eafto the self with a possessive pronoun indicating the coreferent nominal in the reflexivization, as in (6): (6) i maria xtipai ton eafto tis the Mary/NOM hit/3sg.act the self/acc her Mary is hitting herself.

5 In addition, there is a morphological reflexive in which the coreferential linking is expressed through so-called nonactive verbal morphology, also known as mediopassive or middle voice forms; 4 in the case of hit, the equivalent to (6) using this morphological strategy would be (7a) and some other such reflexives are given in (7b) and (7c): (7) a. i maria xtipjete the Mary/NOM hit/3sg.non-act Mary is hitting herself. b. i maria plenete the Mary/NOM wash/3sg.non-act Mary is washing herself. c. i maria kitazete s ton kaθrefti the Mary/NOM look-at/3sg.non-act in the mirror/acc Mary is looking at herself in the mirror. While the syntactic reflexive is rather free in terms of what sorts of coreferential linkages can be expressed, Modern Greek morphological reflexivization is constrained such that only underlying, i.e. thematic, direct objects can be linked with coreferent subjects. In RG terms, this constraint would mean that only final level 2 that is also (simultaneously) 5 an initial 2 can be linked with a subject in this reflexivization strategy. The evidence for this constraint comes from the

6 reflexivization possibilities in two constructions that have a surface (final level) direct object which is a not an initial direct object and cannot be linked to a subject in a nonactive voice reflexive construction. Although presented in Joseph 2000, the relevant evidence is briefly recapitulated here. First, in the Greek Dative Shift construction, illustrated in (8), the notional indirect object, corresponding to the prepositional phrase in (8a), occurs as an final level direct object, a 2 in RG terms, marked with accusative case, as in (8b); however, this accusative-marked final 2, corresponding as it does to a semantic indirect object, is a noninitial 2, and, as (8c) demonstrates, it cannot be linked with the subject via the morphological reflexivization strategy (8c) has only a passive reading and not a reflexive reading: (8) a. i asko γramatiki s ton jani teach/1sg.act grammar/acc to the John/ACC I teach grammar to John b. i asko ton jani γ ramatiki teach/1sg.act the John/ACC grammar/acc I teach John grammar c. o janis i askete γ ramatiki the John/NOM teach/3sg.non-act grammar/acc John is taught grammar / * John teaches himself grammar.

7 Second, the full complement structure with θeoro consider, given in (2) above, admits of an analysis whereby (2a) reflects the underlying structure more or less directly and (2b) is a derived structure, in which the surface accusative NP, the final level 2, is a noninitial (i.e. nonthematic) object, taking on final 2 status as the result of what has elsewhere been called Subject-to-Object Raising (see Joseph 1976, 1990, 1992). Important for the argument here is the fact that this final (and noninitial) object cannot feed into the morphological reflexive strategy, as shown by the unavailability of a reflexive reading for (9b), where only a passive sense is possible for θeorite: (9) a. θeoro ton jani pos ine jeneos consider/1sg.act the John/ACC COMP is/3sg brave/nom.sg I consider John to be brave b. o janis θeorite pos ine jeneos the John/NOM consider/3sg.non-act COMP is/3sg brave/nom.sg John is considered to be brave / * John considers himself to be brave Furthermore, there is an instructive contrast between (9) and similar-appearing structures with piθo persuade, as given in (10). With piθo there is a post-verbal accusative object which is an initial object (in (10a)), and in the nonactive voice (in (10b)), while a passive reading is possible, as with θeorite in (9b), so too is a reflexive reading:

8 (10) a. episa ton jani pos ine jeneos persuaded/1sg.act the John/ACC COMP is/3sg brave/nom.sg I persuaded John that he is brave b. o janis pistike pos ine jeneos the John/NOM persuaded/1sg.non-act COMP is/3sg brave/nom.sg John was persuaded that he is brave / John persuaded himself that he is brave. The reflexive possibility in (10b) is consistent with the constraint on nonactive voice reflexivization, because unlike ton jani in (9a), ton jani in (10a) is an initial (and final) 2. 12.3 Reflexivization in consider Sentences With this constraint, it is now possible to test for the type of object that ton jani in (1) is. As (11) shows, the morphological reflexivization strategy based on the structure of (1) is ungrammatical; that is, in (11): (11) o janis θeorite eksipnos the John/NOM consider/3sg.non-act smart/nom.sg

9 only a passive interpretation John is considered smart is possible and not a reflexive reading, *John considers himself smart. This fact means that ton jani in (1), even though clearly a final stratum 2 (surface direct object) is not an initial 2, being rather a nonthematic object. 6 What its initial grammatical relation is perhaps is not clear, but it could well be an initial 1 (a subject), if sentence (1) above is taken to be a reduction in some way from the structures indicated in (2). In any case, though, given the constraint on the morphological reflexive, (11) fits in with a pattern of reflexivization possibilities in Greek focusing on final stratum objects that are also initial stratum objects; the morphological reflexive strategy is not possible with a final object that is a different initial stratum grammatical relation, as in (8c), (9b), and (11). It is important to note that the problem with reflexivization in (12) is not a semantic problem since the syntactic reflexive can be employed to provide a reflexive reading: (12) θeoro ton eafto mu eksipno consider/1sg the self/acc my smart/acc.sg I consider myself smart Moreover, it is not a morphological problem since the nonactive form θeorite does occur, but only in a passive sense, not a reflexive sense.

10 12.4 Conclusion The result of this discussion is that reference to multiple levels of grammatical relations is an essential part of the statement of nonactive voice reflexivization in Greek it operates with final 2s (roughly, surface direct objects) that are simultaneously initial 2s (underlying direct objects). No other combination of grammatical relations allows for this reflexivization strategy. While one could explore the possibility of stating this on a semantic basis in terms of the thematicity of the direct object to be linked with a coreferent subject, it is not clear that theme or affected entity is a coherent semantic notion; the entity hit or washed in (7ab) clearly is affected in some way but is the entity persuaded in (10b) affected in the same way, or is the entity viewed in (7c) even affected at all? Most likely not, making a purely semantic characterization less compelling. Moreover, restricting the morphological reflexive to a layer of lexical derivation could produce the desired results, but the basic fact remains that there is a syntactic dimension to reflexivity in Greek in that the realization of argument structure in nonactive verbs is different from that seen with active verbs; furthermore, reflexivization participates in that argument reduction. Consequently, one way or another, reference to grammatical relations at different levels of analysis must be recognized to account for the full range of reflexivization facts in Greek.

13 This paper was completed while I was a Special Visiting Fellow at the Research Centre for Linguistic Typology at La Trobe University in the Melbourne, Australia area. I would like to thank its director, Bob Dixon, and its associate director, Sasha Aikhenvald, for kindly inviting me to spend some time at the Centre in the summer of 2006. It was a fitting place to write this tribute to David Perlmutter, as he always emphasized to me that linguistic theory was about accounting for the ways in which all languages are similar and the ways in which they are different, and this is a basic goal of linguistic typology as well. What I present here is an application of an analysis that David helped me with years ago having to do with the interaction of raising and reflexivization in Modern Greek (some of which were published in the Appendix to Joseph 1990). I happily dedicate this present piece to him. 1. Here is a key to the abbreviations used in glossing the examples: NOM = nominative, SG = singular, ACC = accusative, MASC = masculine, COMP = complementizer, ACT = active, NON-ACT = nonactive 2. These facts from English and other aspects of the Greek sentence-type here are discussed in greater detail and with a different goal in Joseph 2000. 3. Berman stated the constraint as follows: Tough movement may move a noun phrase only from its position in underlying structure, and this can be interpreted as given here in terms of thematicity of the direct object. 4. These forms can have passive or, with plurals, reciprocal, meanings, as well as other functions all of which are irrelevant to the matter at hand here.

14 5. RG is a nonderivational framework (hence the scare quotes around terminology below like derived object, which are intended in a metaphorical sense), so it is not the case that an initial relation turns into a final one, but rather a given nominal has properties from its initial status and its final (and other level) status at all points in the generation/interpretation of a sentence. 6. As discussed in Joseph 2000, this is different from the ostensibly parallel English sentence-type, to judge from the facts in (4) and (5), but it seems one has to simply take each language on its own terms.

15 References Berman, Arlene. 1973. A constraint on tough-movement. Papers from the Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society 9:34 43. Joseph, Brian D. 1976. Raising in Modern Greek: A copying process? 290r*: Harvard Studies in Syntax and Semantics Vol. II:241 278. Joseph, Brian D. 1990. Morphology and universals in syntactic change: Evidence from Medieval and Modern Greek (Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics Series). New York: Garland Publishers. (Expanded and updated version of 1978 Harvard University doctoral dissertation.) Joseph, Brian D. 1992. Diachronic perspectives on control. In Richard Larson, Sabine Iatridou, Utpal Lahiri, and James Higginbotham, eds., Control and grammatical theory, 195 234. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Joseph, Brian D. 2000. consider sentences re-considered in the light of Greek evidence. In Katerina Nicolaidis and Marina Mattheoudakis, eds., Proceedings of the 13th International Symposium on Theoretical and Applied Linguistics April 1999 (Festschrift for Prof. Athanasios Kakouriotis), 81 88. Thessaloniki: Department of English, Aristotle University. Stowell, Tim. 1981. Origins of phrase structure. Doctoral dissertation, MIT. Stowell, Tim. 1983. Subjects across categories. The Linguistic Review 2:285 312. Williams, Edwin. 1980. Predication. Linguistic Inquiry 11:203 238.

Williams, Edwin. 1983. Against small clauses. Linguistic Inquiry 14:287 308. 16