THE THREE RELATIVE CONSTRUCTIONS IN SWAHILI (KISANIFU)

Similar documents
Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Writing a composition

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

On the final vowel in Kikae

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Words come in categories

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Part I. Figuring out how English works

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

On the Notion Determiner

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

Handout #8. Neutralization

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Developing Grammar in Context

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

15 The syntax of overmarking and kes in child Korean

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

Primary English Curriculum Framework

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Language contact in East Nusantara

National University of Singapore Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences Centre for Language Studies Academic Year 2014/2015 Semester 2

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

2006 Mississippi Language Arts Framework-Revised Grade 12

Year 4 National Curriculum requirements

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

INSTANT VOCABULARY 6-10

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

Proposed syllabi of Foundation Course in French New Session FIRST SEMESTER FFR 100 (Grammar,Comprehension &Paragraph writing)

The Internet as a Normative Corpus: Grammar Checking with a Search Engine

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark

Pearson Longman Keystone Book D 2013

Introduction to Swahili Language and East African Tribal Communities SFS 2060

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Som and Optimality Theory

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

The Bulgarian Reportative as a Conventional Implicature Chronos 10. Dimka Atanassov University of Pennsylvania

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

ELP in whole-school use. Case study Norway. Anita Nyberg

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

Individual Component Checklist L I S T E N I N G. for use with ONE task ENGLISH VERSION

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Reading Horizons. Organizing Reading Material into Thought Units to Enhance Comprehension. Kathleen C. Stevens APRIL 1983

ENGLISH. Progression Chart YEAR 8

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Course Syllabus Advanced-Intermediate Grammar ESOL 0352

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

IS THERE A PASSIVE IN DHOLUO?

TA Script of Student Test Directions

EAGLE: an Error-Annotated Corpus of Beginning Learner German

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

GRAMMATICAL MORPHEME ACQUISITION: AN ANALYSIS OF AN EFL LEARNER S LANGUAGE SAMPLES *

Advanced Grammar in Use

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

APA Basics. APA Formatting. Title Page. APA Sections. Title Page. Title Page

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Student Name: OSIS#: DOB: / / School: Grade:

Transcription:

THE THREE RELATIVE CONSTRUCTIONS IN SWAHILI (KISANIFU) Thilo C. SCHADEBERG 1. INTRODUCTION A. Swahili has three morphologically distinct relative verb constructions. subject subject _ verb stem na PRES li PAST taka FUT si NEG relative relative _ verbe stem C. amba relative (1) A. (wa-tu) wa-sema-o kiswahili (people) SC speak RC 2 2 2 SENTENCE (those) who speak Swahili B. (wa-tu) wa-na-o-sema kiswahili (those) who are speaking Swahili C. (wa-tu) amba-o (ha-wa-ta-sema) kiswahili (those) who will not speak Swahili 2. INTERNAL RECONSTRUCTION Construction B is historically derived from construction A. The clearest indication for this is the form of the Future Tense Marker -takà-, with stress on its final syllabe. Since stress, in Swahili, regularly falls on the penultimate syllabe, these verb forms are clearly marked as compounds with an (internai) word boundary after the Relative Concord. The fïrst part of these compounds constitutes an A-type relative construction of the verb -taka «want» (The non-relative Future Tense Marker -ta- is, of course, derived from the same auxiliary, the syllabe ka being lost in unstressed position). A similar development is hypothesized for type B constructions with the Tense Markers -na-, -li-, and -si-. However, the symmetry is not complete. These three morphèmes do not function as regulär verbs in Swahili, and there is no reason to believe they did in pre-swahili.

34 T.C. Schadeberg Rather, they occur each in its own peculiar set of copulative constructions, amongst which, notably, are type A forms : (2a) wa-na-cho (kitabu) they have it (the book) (2b) (kitabu) wa-li-cho na-cho (the book) which they have (2c) (watu) wa-li-o na watoto (those) who have children (2d) (watu) wa-si-o na watoto (those) who have no children Note that the RC following na, äs in (2a) and (2b), does not mark thé clause as being relative; it simply is the proper morphological form of the bound anaphoric pronoun to be used in this environment, i.e. after na. The fact remains that na, li, and si all occur in the environment SC--RC, i.e. in type A constructions. 1 suspect further that -na-, -M-, and -si- carry stress in thé same way as -takä- when used in type B relative constructions. I have noted, that a frequent déviation from standard spelling is to make a break after the RC, e.g. wanao andika/walio andika/wasio andika «they who write/wrote/don't write» / The same applies to Swahili written in Arabic script. We may say, therefore, that all four type B constructions consist of a type A construction plus a verb stem (which may be preceded by an object ). The verb stem, in turn, can be seen to be derived from an infmitive, of which the nominal prefix ku has been deleted when unstressed. (3a) (wa-tu) wa -li -o -ya-nunüa (ma-tunda) (those) who bought them (the fruits) NP-people SC-PAST RC -OCbuy NP-fruits 2 2 2 6 6 (3b) (wa-tu) wa-li-o-yé-la (those) who ate them (3c) (wa-tu) wa-li-o-ku-la ma-tunda (those) who ate fruits The inference that type B relative constructions developed out of type A constructions is obvious. The type C relative construction, too, is nothing but a special case of type A. The verb stem -amba is used in modern Swahili mainly in the infmitive kw-amba which functions as a complementizer, but the applicative verb -ambia «teil» is very common. This alone is enough to hypothesize an older verb -amba «say». The «relative marker» amba-rc is very likely to have developed out of the type A construction by loss of the subject. The paraphrase would have been «of which/ of whom one says...», «duquel on dit...»'. 3. COMPARATIVE RECONSTRUCTION Internai reconstruction leads to the hypothesis that type A relative constructions are thé oldest; it does not, as far as I can see, provide arguments bearing on thé relative chronology of type B and type C. Still, type C is often assumed to be the most recent one, cf. Perrott (1951 : 64) : «Good Swahili got on for years without these forms, but they are likely to become more common as time goes on» (See also Givón 1972 : 191). Swahili written tradition should belong 1. An alternative hypothesis, proposée by Dammann (1966), dérives amba-rc from an imperative followed by thé RC functioning as an anaphoric object pronoun. Such an imperative construction is, however, quite unknown in Swahili, in northeastern Bantu, and in Bantu in général.

The three relative constructions in Swahili 35 enough to evaluate this claim. If we substitute «Zanzibar Swahili» for Perrott's «good Swahili», Steere (1906 : 121f) confïrms that type C is «not used in Zanzibar». Likewise, Sacleux (1909a, b) does not treat type C in his two Swahili grammars, though the construction is documented in his dictionary (1939). Outside Zanzibar, type C constructions probably have existed for centuries, cf. Miehe's (1979) study of the language of the older Swahili poetry. I would like to interpret the more restricted geographical spread of type C within Swahili as an indication of its more recent origin, as compared with type B constructions. This is confirmed by comparison of Swahili with its closest relatives in northeastern Bantu. According to Nsuka (1982 : 31f.; map 2), the only Bantu languages employing type A and/or type B relative constructions are all geographically and linguistically close to Swahili : Pokomo, Nyika, Digo, Shambala, Bondei, Hehe (attestation doubtful). The fact that almost all these languages employ both type A and type B constructions, whereas grammars make no mention of type C, can again be taken as an indication of type B's chronological priority over type C. It would be interesting, but leading too far away from Swahili, to try and trace the development of type A in a comparative Bantu perspective. 4. RESTRICTIONS ON TENSE MARKERS (TM) Type B constructions occur with four different Tense Markers : -na- PRESENT, -li- PAST, -taka- FUTURE, and -si- NEGATIVE. The question is : why are there no relative tenses (type B) with other Tense Markers, such as -a- AORIST, -ka- SUBSECUTIVE, -ki- SITUATIVE, -nge-, -ngali CONDITIONAL, and -me- PERFECT. The answer is not the same in each case. The TMs -a- and -ka- are very old; they have, in fact, been reconstructed for proto-bantu. At the time when type A constructions of the copular «verbids» and the future auxiliary verb were compounded into type B constructions, -a- and -ka- had long since ceased to be auxiliaries. The TM -ki- is only usable in subordinated clauses; a SITUATIVE clause is semantically not capable of forming a relative clause on its own. One might even say that -ki- clauses are syntactically embedded just like relative clauses. A similar explanation probably holds for the CONDITIONAL clauses, though the TM's -nge- and -ngali- are used in the conditional as well as in the main clause. (4a) ningekuwa na mali ningemwoa If I had money I would marry her A relative embedding of this sentence is semantically possible, but no type B construction exists. Type C, however, is acceptable. (4b) msichana ambaye ningekuwa na mali ningemwoa The girl which... 2 Thus, there seems to be a restriction on Type A and B relatives that excludes such complex sentences, just as relative subjunctives are excluded. The non-occurrence of thé TM -me- in type B constructions demands a différent explanation. Semantically, nothing inhibits perfective relative clauses, and -me- is a relatively 2. Some speakers would accept (or admit using) type B relatives with the TM -ngali- (but not -nge-); e.g. msichana ningaliyemwoa ningalikuwa na mali «thé girl which I would hâve married if I had had the money». The reason for this near-acceptability seems to be that -ngali- is a complex TM, and that the RC in such forms follows the formative -li-.

36 T.C. Schadeberg recent TM which developed from the verb -mala «finish» used as an auxiliary. Miehe (1979 225-28) documents three paths along which this verb became an auxiliary : Basic stem Perfective stem with palatalization (-mala) -maa- -ma- -mazie- -mezie- -mezewith imbrication -mele- -mee- -me- All these forms are regulär in the sensé that they are attested for other verbs, too; compare -kazie and -kalie (no imbrication, with and without palatalization) from -kaa «sit», -pete from -pata «get», -yee from -yaa «be füll», etc. It appears that in old (northern, poetic) Swahili type A relatives could freely be formed from basic as well as from perfective sterns, and from these we would expect the development of type B relatives whith the various PERFECT TMs. They do, in fact, occur, but with the notable exception of -me-o-. (The less morphème -o-frequently replaces the RC in old/northern/poetic Swahili). (5a) nayo ndiyo ipeseo this is what is proper (5b) ndiye amezookoka it is hè who is redeemed (5c) sayo nimayotamka this is what I have said (Cited from Miehe 1979 : 113). There is évidence that -me- is the most recent PERFECT TM in poetic Swahili, and that it spread from the south to the north (cf. Miehe 1979 : 229-30). Possibly, the type B relative paradigm was complete at the time when -me- spread all over Swahili and pushed out other competing forms. 5. ACCESSIBILITY All three constructions (A, B and C) can be employed to form subject and object relatives. In simple object relatives, the use of an object (OC) is (almost) obligatory. (6) A. m w-a ka g-ja-p m-tu ni-m-pokea-ye B. mw-aka u-li-o-pita m-tu wa-li-ye-m-pokea C. kj-tabu amba-cho ha-kj-ja-fika ki-tabu amba-cho hu-ja-ki-soma 3 next year («year which-comes») the man whom I receive last year («year which-passed») the man whom they received the book which has not yet arrived the book which you didn't read yet 3. Object relatives of types A and B have the order (ANTECEDENT) VERB/REL SUBJECT when the subject is a noun. In type C, word order may be either (ANTECEDENT) amba- RC VERB SUBJECT or... SUBJECT VERB; the order VS déviâtes from the dominant SV(O) order of swahili. Givón (1972 :191-92) invokes the «principle of pronoun attraction» as an explanation. Dik (1980 : 188-90) proposes another hypothesis : hè believes that Bantu originally had P1VSO word order, where PI represents a special position for Topic or Focus. Accordingly the order in an object relative clauses hould have been Relative Marker VS, and our patterns A and B explained by Dik as the results of an incorporation of RM into the verb. The fact that the Relative Marker (RC) occurs originally (type A) in postfinal position, i.e. after the verb, réfutes Dik's hypothesis, at least as far as Swahili is concerned. An original SVO pattern plus Givón's «pronoun attraction» adequately account for the facts of word and morphème order with which we are here concerned.

The three relative constructions in Swahili 37 In all three constructions, the RC may also refer to an entity which is neither subject nor (direct) object of the relative clause. In these cases, naturally, the RC and the OC - if present - have different referents. (7) A. ny-imbo ni-mw-andikia-zo the songs I write for him B. ma-neno ni-li-yp-ku-ambia the words I told you Such syntactic constructions are particularly common with RCs of the «autonomous» classes 8 (vyo- MANNER) and 16 (-po- TIME/CONDITION). (8) ni-li-vyo-ku-onyesha as I showed you wa-na-po-imba when/if they sing u-ja-po-m w-a m bia hasikii even though you teil him, hè does not listen Relative clauses of all three types may also refer to arguments that are morphologically embedded within the relative clause. Such morphological embeddings are the Connective («genitive»), the Instrumental phrase preceded by kwa-, and the Associative/Agentive phrase preceded by na-. In such constructions, we find a RC within the verb form as well as a kind of «resumptive pronoun» with the appropriate morphological marker. (9a) ndiy.e ni-li-ye-ku-zungumzia habari za-ke (9b) mp-neno a-li-yp-wa-danganya wenziwe kwa-y,o (9c) w-enziwe a-li-o-zungumza na-o it is hè whose stories I told you the words with which hè deceived his companions her friends with whom she talked Locatives of classes 16, 17 and 18 (marked by a non-class spécifie suffix -ni) deserve special attention in this connection. The RC has to refer to the class of the head noun. Thus, examples (b) and (c) below are ungrammatical. (loa) nyumba-n.i ni-na-mp-kaa (lob) *nyymba^j]ijna-rno-kaa (10e) *nyumba-m^va : taka-yp-i-bomoa in-the-house in which I live the house in which I live in-the-house which they are going to pull down Sentences (b) and (c) are rectified by using analytic (often : connective) locative constructions. (lob') ny-umba ni-na-yo-kaa ndani ya-ke (10c') ndani ya ny-umba wa-taka-yo-i-bomoa (10c") katika ny-umba wa-taka-yo-i-bomoa Even greater syntactic accessibility is shown in the following example where a type B relative refers to the object of an embedded sentence. Note that the RC again appears within the main verb to which it has no direct syntactic relation. (11) ny-imbo ni-li-zo-wa-sikia wa-zee wa-o wa-ki-zi-imba i i i 1 the songs which I heard their elders sing (them) In terms of frequency or stylistic préférence, however, it is probably true that the more complex the syntactic structure of the relative clause and the function of the relativized argument in it, the more likely becomes the use of type C constructions. Thus, Ashton (1944 : 310) describes (12a) as normal, and (12b) as «very heavy Swahili». (12a) (masanduku) ambayo sisi watu wawili au watatu hatuwezi kuyainua two or even three of us could not lift (12b) (masanduku) tusiyoweza sisi watu wawili au watatu kuyainua. (boxes) which Ashton furthermore observes that type C constructions are required for non-restrictive relative clauses (1944 : 310). I have not been able to find out how strict a rule this is.

38 T.C. Schadeberg (13) Hapo kale nchi ya Uganda haikuwa na watu ila mmoja, ambaye aliitwa Kintu Long ago the country of Uganda had no inhabitants except one, who was called Kintu Finally, a type C relative construction is the only possible choice when «tenses» other than the simple AOR/PRES/PAST/FUT/NEG are desired (see also (4b) and (6/C) above). (14a) (vitabu) ambavyo sitavisoma (14b) (vitabu) ambavyo u(si)visome (books) which I am not going to read (books) which you should (not) read We may conclude that the spread of type C relative constructions is less due to superior syntactic accessibility than has sometimes been assumed. lts main advantages lie in its morphological and word order versatility. REFERENCES ASHTON, E.O., 1944, Swahili Grammar. London : Longmans. DAMMANN, Ernst, 1966, Das Determinativum im Suaheli. In Neue Afrikanistische Studien (Hamburger Beiträge zur Afrika-Kunde, 5), ed. J. Lukas, pp. 71-78. Hamburg : Deutsches Institut für Afrika- Forschung. DIK, Simon C., 1980, Studies in Functional Grammar. London : Academie press. GIVON, Talmy, 1972, Pronoun attraction and subject postposing in Bantu. In The Chicago Witch Hunt, ed. P.M. Peranteau, J.N. Levi, and G.C. Phares, pp. 190-97. Chicago : Chicago Linguistic Society. MIEHE, Gudrun, 1979, Die Sprache der älteren Swahili-Dichtung. (Marburger Studien zur Afrika- und Asienkunde, Serie A : Afrika, 18). Berlin : Dietrich Reimer. NSUKA NKUTSI, F., 1982, Les structures fondamentales du relatif dans les langues bantoues. (Annales, 1O8 [erroneously numbered 107]). Tervuren : Musée Royal de l'afrique Centrale. PERROTT, D.V., 1951, Teach Jourself Swahili. London : The English University Press. SACLEUX, Ch., 1909a, Grammaire swahilie. Paris : Procure des PP. du Saint-Esprit. SACLEUX, Ch., 1909b, Grammaire des dialectes swahilis. Paris : Procure des PP. du Saint-Esprit. SACLEUX, Ch., 1939, Dictionnaire Swahili-Français. (Travaux et Mémoires de l'institut d'ethnologie, Université de Paris, 36-37). Paris : Institut d'ethnologie, Musée de l'homme. STEERE, Edward, 1906, A Handbook of the Swahili Language, As Spoken at Zanzibar. Rev. by A.C. Madan, 7th ed. London : Society for promoting Christian Knowledge. CONTRIBUTION TO THE DISCUSSION I would like to profit from thé discussion which followed thé présentation of this paper (and continued outside thé officiai session) and add thé following remarks : (1) The non-occurence of type C relative constructions in 19th Century Kiunguja is supported by thé absence of such forms from the autobiography of Tippu Tip (Gudrun Miehe, personal communication). Various other though sometimes conflicting views on use, frequency, and chronology of type C relative constructions are assembled and quoted in Van 't Veld 1966. (2) I think thé stress on -nâ-, -li-, -si- and -takà- in type B constructions should not be described as «secondary stress», and thus there is no need to assign Swahili différent degrees

The three relative constructions in Swahili 39 of distinctive stress. There is only a général rule which reduces non-phrase final stress ; the two examples below are identical as far as stress (levels) are concerned : a.na.sô.ma ki.tâ.bu he is reading a book a.nà.ye.a.ndi.ka he who writes (3) The attestation of type C relative constructions in old Swahili poetry rules out any possibility of their being a borrowing from English. Generally, type C constructions hâve more syllables than those of type A or B, thus the former are unlikely to be modem substitutes for older (type A or B) constructions in poetic texts. However, it is not at all unlikely that the recent spread of type C has been favoured by its structural similarity to the relative construction in English. (4) The suggestion was made that the form amba-rc might never have had a subject which would mean that it did not develop out of the type A construction. The suggested «parallel» (from Sanga L. 35) did not convince me because it concerned a complementizer, not a «relative pronoun». On the other hand, the loss of a subject is attested in Swahili by forms such as (i)japo «although». Even more direct évidence has since corne to my attention : Van 't Veld (1966 : 97) cites Giryama forms (from Westen, 1903) where the subject is still present : (mimi) nambaye, (kitu) chambacho. If such forms are indeed the source of type C relative constructions, the original paraphrase would have been «(I) who say», «(the thing) which says», etc. (5) An interesting hypothesis was put forward concerning the (pre-swahili) origin of type A relative constructions. In the following two examples the formative PP-o (PP = pronominal prefix) appears in postfinal position in non-relative verb forms. Ngazija (example provided by M. LAPON) : tsi-m-nika-so I give it to him SC OC give RC Isg 1 7 Tikuu (example from D. NURSE, 1982 : 101) : v - enda - vo they are going SC go RC 2 2 The Ngazija example shows that thé «relative» is but a spécial instance of the more général «referential» (RC), which is also true for Standard Swahili (Kisanifu); see, for instance, its use in démonstratives and after -ingine-. The suggestion now is that either one of thèse two constructions could somehow hâve transformed («il y avait un passage») into Swahili type A relative constructions. I think this is very unlikely. The Ngazija example and parallels could be cited from a wide range of Bantu languages shows thé use of thé RC (PP-o) as an anaphoric (second) object pronoun. It can only occur with (bi-)transitive verbs, and how exactly it could hâve changed into a général relative remains unclear. The Tikuu example looks more promising because it is not limited to transitive verbs, and it appears to be a construction particular for this spécifie northeastern Bantu area. The closest parallel I know of exists in Rirai (or : Nyaturu) where certain non relative verb forms hâve a tone-copy of the subject in postfinal position (Schadeberg, 1978/79). There is good comparative évidence that thèse and similar verb forms were originally relative verb forms (cf. Nsuka, 1982). I therefore suggest that thé Tikuu form above (as well as équivalent forms in other Northern Swahili dialects) hâve their historical source in relative verb forms, and that they changed into absolute forms by way of topicalized cleft sentences such as «it is they who go».

40 T.C. Schadeberg REFERENCES NURSE, Derek, 1982, The Swahili dialects of Somalia and thé northern Kenya coast. in Etudes sur le bantu oriental : langues des Comores, de Somalie et du Kenya. (LACITO - documents, Afrique, 4), ed. M.-F. Rombi, pp. 73-146. Paris. SELAF. SCHADEBERG, Thilo C., 1978/79. Über die Töne der verbalen Formen im Rimi. Afrika und Übersee 62 : j 288-313. l VAN 'T VELD, H., 1966, The relative verbal in Swahili. Swahili : Journal ofthe Institute of Swahili Research 36, l : 94-99.