Drawing Diagrams and Solving Word Problems: A Study of a Sample of Bruneian Primary

Similar documents
Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

The Role of Problems to Enhance Pedagogical Practices in the Singapore Mathematics Classroom.

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

Evaluating the Effectiveness of the Strategy Draw a Diagram as a Cognitive Tool for Problem Solving

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

A Metacognitive Approach to Support Heuristic Solution of Mathematical Problems

Answer Key For The California Mathematics Standards Grade 1

Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

CAN PICTORIAL REPRESENTATIONS SUPPORT PROPORTIONAL REASONING? THE CASE OF A MIXING PAINT PROBLEM

FROM QUASI-VARIABLE THINKING TO ALGEBRAIC THINKING: A STUDY WITH GRADE 4 STUDENTS 1

Students Understanding of Graphical Vector Addition in One and Two Dimensions

Classify: by elimination Road signs

Page 1 of 11. Curriculum Map: Grade 4 Math Course: Math 4 Sub-topic: General. Grade(s): None specified

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Save Children. Can Math Recovery. before They Fail?

Do students benefit from drawing productive diagrams themselves while solving introductory physics problems? The case of two electrostatic problems

This scope and sequence assumes 160 days for instruction, divided among 15 units.

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Success Factors for Creativity Workshops in RE

First Grade Standards

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Alignment of Australian Curriculum Year Levels to the Scope and Sequence of Math-U-See Program

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Using Realistic Mathematics Education with low to middle attaining pupils in secondary schools

MODULE FRAMEWORK AND ASSESSMENT SHEET

DMA CLUSTER CALCULATIONS POLICY

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

Aviation English Training: How long Does it Take?

Rendezvous with Comet Halley Next Generation of Science Standards

Remainder Rules. 3. Ask students: How many carnations can you order and what size bunches do you make to take five carnations home?

What's My Value? Using "Manipulatives" and Writing to Explain Place Value. by Amanda Donovan, 2016 CTI Fellow David Cox Road Elementary School

Let s think about how to multiply and divide fractions by fractions!

Mathematics process categories

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

SPATIAL SENSE : TRANSLATING CURRICULUM INNOVATION INTO CLASSROOM PRACTICE

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

The Curriculum in Primary Schools

MERGA 20 - Aotearoa

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

Standard 1: Number and Computation

Paper 2. Mathematics test. Calculator allowed. First name. Last name. School KEY STAGE TIER

Faculty Schedule Preference Survey Results

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

After your registration is complete and your proctor has been approved, you may take the Credit by Examination for MATH 6A.

ONE TEACHER S ROLE IN PROMOTING UNDERSTANDING IN MENTAL COMPUTATION

End-of-Module Assessment Task

Mathematics subject curriculum

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

NUMBERS AND OPERATIONS

Mathematics Success Grade 7

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

Math 121 Fundamentals of Mathematics I

Introducing the New Iowa Assessments Mathematics Levels 12 14

Pre-Algebra A. Syllabus. Course Overview. Course Goals. General Skills. Credit Value

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful?

FEEDBACK & MARKING POLICY. Little Digmoor Primary School

TASK 2: INSTRUCTION COMMENTARY

Contents. Foreword... 5

What effect does science club have on pupil attitudes, engagement and attainment? Dr S.J. Nolan, The Perse School, June 2014

HOW DO PUPILS ExPERIENCE SETTING IN PRIMARY MATHEMATICS?

evans_pt01.qxd 7/30/2003 3:57 PM Page 1 Putting the Domain Model to Work

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

Case study Norway case 1

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role

Problem of the Month: Movin n Groovin

Appendix L: Online Testing Highlights and Script

Sample Problems for MATH 5001, University of Georgia

Evaluating Statements About Probability

Characteristics of Functions

South Carolina English Language Arts

Cognitive Thinking Style Sample Report

Centre for Evaluation & Monitoring SOSCA. Feedback Information

LITERACY ACROSS THE CURRICULUM POLICY

Marketing Management MBA 706 Mondays 2:00-4:50

No Child Left Behind Bill Signing Address. delivered 8 January 2002, Hamilton, Ohio

Arizona s College and Career Ready Standards Mathematics

Coping with Crisis Helping Children With Special Needs

Chapter 4 - Fractions

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C

Curriculum Policy. November Independent Boarding and Day School for Boys and Girls. Royal Hospital School. ISI reference.

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Developing Effective Teachers of Mathematics: Factors Contributing to Development in Mathematics Education for Primary School Teachers

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

Mathematics Program Assessment Plan

HOW DO WE TEACH CHILDREN TO BE NUMERATE? Mike Askew and Margaret Brown King s College London

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology

PREVIEW LEADER S GUIDE IT S ABOUT RESPECT CONTENTS. Recognizing Harassment in a Diverse Workplace

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Transcription:

Drawing Diagrams and Solving Word Problems: A Study of a Sample of Bruneian Primary a~d Secondary School Children Palanisamy K. Veloo University of Brunei Darussalam Francis Lopez Real University of Hong Kong Abstract: Drawing diagrams is often recommended as a strategy for problem solving. The mathematics education literature suggests that teachers use diagrams as a problem solving strategy but provides little guidance on the kinds of diagrams teachers should draw and how teachers could teach pupils to generate their own diagrams. The findings from two separate studies, one with primary and the other with secondary school children, on the nature and use of diagrams in solving problems are described. The paper discusses the effectiveness of diagrams in helping children solve word problems, and implications for classroom instruction. Introduction Solving word problems in mathematics is a difficult topic to teach in both primary and secondary schools. Research into children's performance in public examinations in Brunei reveals that only about 1 % of the pupils attempted any kind of diagram in solving word problems (Lopez-Real, Veloo & Maawiah, 1992). Although drawing of diagrams as a problem solving strategy has long been advocated by mathematics educators, (polya,1957; Schoenfeld, 1985; Hyde.A.A. et al. 1991),, empirical evidence on its effectiveness has been scarce. Children have been found to perform better in mathematical problem solving situations when diagrams are used by teachers to elicit appropriate mental imag~s Driscoll, 1979; Riedesel 1969). (Yancey,V. et al,1989; However, little has been written about how pupils should be taught to generate their own diagrams (Moses, 1982; Nelson 1983). The extent to which children use this strategy in problem solving, or how spontaneously they ul\.e this strategy has not been fully investigated. This paper discusses the results from two studies in primary and secondary schools in Brunei Darussalam on children's use of diagrams in solving word problems. Methodology The sample in the first study (Lopez-Real & Veloo,1993) consisted of 96 children from two primary schools in Bandar Seri Begawan, the capital of Brunei Darussalam. Each child was given a set of ten word problems to solve. The Newman interview procedure which classifies students' errors on written mathematical tasks as either reading, comprehension, transformation, process skills or encoding (Newman, 1977) was used to study unsuccessful instances. Errors that were identified as comprehension or transformation were followed up with the following procedure. First, the pupil was asked to draw a

668 diagram of the problem and to include any important information from the problem into the diagram and then to try to solve the problem again. No help or assistance was given by the interviewer. If the pupil was unable to draw a diagram or was still unsuccessful after having drawn one, he or she was then presented with a diagram drawn by the authors and asked to try again using this diagram. The results are as follows. Table 1: Successful and unsuccessful Cases after Drawing Diagrams Able to draw Diagram Yes No 107 19 If Diagram Drawn Successful Unsuccessful 41 66 If Authors' Diagram Used Successful Unsuccessful S2 26 The striking feature in Table 1 is that 38% (or 41 out of 107) of the cases in which pupils had previously failed to obtain a correct solution, they were now successful with no further assistance or help other than the suggestion to represent the problem pictorially. The responses to several of the problems used in the study are now discussed in more detail. Discussion of selected diagrams Problem 1: A teacher brought a bunch of bananas to be shared equally among 12 boys. 8 of the bananas were bad and were thrown away. After this each boy received 4 bananas. How many bananas were in the bunch at the slart? Figurel l'iunela The diagrams in Fig.1 show that once a matching has been established between boys and bananas, the pupils were able to determine with ease the critical step 12 x 4. Izzudin's example is the most common type; Termizi shows a slight variation. Hasnal drew 4 bananas but did not proceed beyond the second pupil; this was sufficient to enable him to establish the matching between pupils and bananas. Pamela's diagram even shows a waste bin for the bad bananas but the representation of the twelve pupils enabled her to obtain 4x12. Rebecca's diagram shows a literal representation of the major elements of the problem but no matching of boys and bananas is evident..

669 Problem 2. A red box weighs 3 times lis much lis a blue box. The two boxes logether weigh 36 kg. lwuzl is the weight of the blue box. -.'!'Cd. '*' s- la.,., \"... bl,.a& ~ '1"'1 ~boo< '2.'... Figure 2 -.~~ ~ Rozie Ri. ~ 'ti31. f~~~. /. ~ 1Cd_~ "'\Ma..~ ~ 1Gi 'Wa:~~.. "-~ ~t~... 1 11 I I I 1,1 "~, ~t.\ The most common diagram observed for this question is similar to that drawn by Rozie. The diagram expresses clearly the relationship between the red box and, the blue box. A concrete representation of the problem is given in the diagram drawn by Hogan. He drew three balances the first with the pointer at 3, the second has the pointer at 1 and the third has the pointer at 4. He then successfully wrote down the step 36+4. Izyan's diagram shows a line, marked out with 36 parts to represent the total weight 36kg. He counted along the line and marked a longer division on 9 and said that the weight of the blue box is 9kg. He then proceeded to calculate the weight of the red box as 36-9. Probkm 7. A while stick is 3 times longer Ihan a black Slick. The difference in Iheir lengths is l2cm. lwuzt is Ihe length oflhe black slick? Figure 3 Kartika made two atsempts at drawing without any prompting from the interviewer. Having completed her first drawing, she immediately shook her head and said "No, that's not right". She proceeded to draw a second diagram and then wrote down the answer 6cm without hesitation. Marcus used a totally different strategy. His representation was in terms of "match sticks" and he used a trial-and-error approach. Secondary school study The secondary school study was a follow-up study to the primary school study described above. In particular, we wanted to investigate (a) the extent to. which diagrams drawn by pupils at different levels of secondary schooling were concrete, semi-concrete or symbolic representations of the given problems, and (b) whetherthe differences in the types of representation were related to levels of schooling. Two hundred

670 pupils from secondary 1 to secondary 4 of two secondary schools in Bandar Seri Begawan were each given a set of ten word problems to solve. These problems did not involve any knowledge of mathematical concepts or techniques beyond the secondary 1 level. The pupils were instructed to read each problem carefully and then to draw a diagram that they felt would help them solve the problem. They were also instructed to include all important information in their diagrams. Despite the instruction which was verbally emphasised when conducting the test, to draw diagrams for each problem, the distribution of cases for which there was no diagram varied from 5% to 64.5%. The number of children who actually drew diagrams against those who did not for each question is given in Table 2 below. Table 2 : Distribution of cases with and without diagrams Ql Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Diagram drawn 165 171 180 190 96 161 No diagram.35 29 20 10 104 39. Table 3 shows the success rate for each question. Q7 Q8 Q9 134 71 107 66 129 93 QIO 115 85 Table 3: Distribution of cases with correct and incorrect solutions Ql Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Q7 Q8 Q9 QI0 Correct 126 186 151 154 21 181 83 36 85 16 Wrong 74 14 49 46 179 19 117 164 115 184. With regard to the type of representation, a diagram is classified as Concrete when students attempt to give a physical or literal representation of the objects in the problem. At the other end" ~f the scale, where the representation is purely symbolic, it is classified as Symbolic. However, 'where the representation appears to lie somew~ere between the two extremes it is categorised as Semi-Concrete. Examples of these are shown below. Examples of diag~ drawn by secondary school pupils The following are selected examples of diagrams drawn by secondary school pupils. Probkm 3: Figure 4 A box colllains 12 oranges and 9 apples. One-third of all the fruits is bad. if 114 of the oranges are bad, how many of the apples are bad? Concrete Semi-concrete Symbolic

671 Probkm 6:..4 large number o/books were received by the Ministry o/education as a gift. The Ministry kept back 100 booksfor its own library and the rest were shared equally among 25 schools. If each school received 80 books, how many books were in the gift 10 the Ministry? FigureS <OD ~O, " ~o 50 _I> 1t0 rei ~ (>I.-lOO) '/ffchllll:.:!~~.. sq;8 ~a!!o.a,llalba.l!. B [!'A - x.,oo'u---+ Ir\. --. ~o.\cs 100,0 /. Ib lio to eo I I I I I I /J I I t. /Ill 11 ~ 1/ ~ 1/1 /!Ill CD 1IJ 111 alld m lij ru III lb fj 1111' In 6J IIJ 4/ m /11 co vo... f~ co ~./ ~(P l~ 100,.. 10 f CO Cb (0 io fo ("~~ ~PJ/e. ;< '"...".!lssl"'-=ij!i,';.':, 1IJ111. 15 scj...ol4 1-l.l.o.1 " io ; b. :;.oco t lid....'--,j ::? I 00 M,,4«. 1'0""""..-IoW 1'10.08 -, ot/.c.c.. Concrete Semi-concrete SymbolIc Probkm 8: In a certain class 3/5 o/ihe pupils are girls. If the number o/boys is doubled and 6 more girls join the class, there Figure 6 would be an equal number of boys and girls. How many pupils were in the class at the start? Concrete Semi-concrete Symbolic Table 4 below shows the breakdown of results for each question with the. diagrams classified under the categories Concrete, Semi-concrete or Symbolic. Table 4: Distribution of Concrete, Semi-concrete and Symbolic diagrams Ql Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Q7 Q8 Q9 QI0 No diagram 35 29 20 10 104 39 66 129 93 85 Concrete 72 63 69 6 60 33 29 14 SI 44 Semi -concrete 67 75 76 23 19 55 60 20 22 44 Symbolit\, 26 33 35 161 17 73 45 37 34 27 In this study no interviews were conducted and so we can only speculate on the reasons for children not having drawn a diagram. The fact that the results in Table 2 vary enormously from question to question suggests that the nature of the problem itself is an important factor. A geometric problem will readily lend itself to a visual representation and it is therefore no surprise that this question (Table 4) has the highest frequency for diagrams drawn. This was the only problem that referred to a geometric figure. Omitting this case, two extreme cases, are Problem 3 with 90% drawn and Problem 8 with only 35.5% drawn. Both problems involve fractions and neither involve large numbers, so why the difference? It can be argued that if pupils immediately recognise how to tackle a problem and feel confident about solving it then they will consider the instruction to draw a diagram quite pointless and simply ignore it. This is a reasonable

672 argument, but the results in Table 3 showing the success rate for each question, certainly belie this argument. Comparing success in solving the problem and diagrams drawn for each question we find that the proportion of pupils who successfully solved a problem was higher among those who drew diagrams than those who did not. Table 4 shows the breakdown of results for each question with the diagrams classified under three categories described earlier. The nature and difficulty of the problem has a strong bearing on the type of diagram drawn by pupils. Our analysis shows pupils in Secondary 1 and 2 tend to draw diagrams that are more of the concrete type than pupils in Secondary 3 and 4. In order to determine whether there was a change in the type of diagrams drawn by pupils at different levels, a X2 test was used to analyse the responses for each question. Contingency tables involving levels of schooling (Secondary 1&2 and 3&4) against type of diagram drawn were obtained for all the 10 questions. Table 5 gives a summary of the X2 analysis. Table 5: Distribution of the level of significance for each question Significance at p<o.oo5 Ql Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Q7 Q8 Q9 QIO No Yes Yes No No Yes Yes' Yes Yes Yes In most cases where there was a significant difference, the direction of change 'Was that the Secondary 3 and 4 pupils drew more diagrams at the symbolic end of the continuum and less at the concrete end than the Forms 1 and 2 pupils. Discussion of results Cues and mis-cues One of the most co,mmon behaviours exhibited by primary school children prior to being asked to draw a diagram was to perform an operation initiated by a cue word in the problem. The word "times" in Problems 2 and 7 often resulted in operations like 36x3 and 12x3. Stuart's comment while trying Problem J highlights this type of behaviour: "It says thrown away so it's got to be a subtract somewhere!" Such cue words act as mis-cues almost as frequently as they cue the appropriate operation. We suspect that the strategy of looking for cue words is actively encouraged by many textbooks and teachers. The speed with which children launched into operating on the numbers (without apparent reference to their meaning) was very striking. Typically, 12x8 and 12-8 were frequent attempts at Problem J, while 36+3 and 12+3 were even more common for Problems 2 and 7 respectively.

673 Function of Diagrams. Given that nearly 38% of the primary school pupils interviewed were successful in solving the problems after simply drawing a diagram, can we identify what function the diagram is playing in these cases? Two elements ~e involved in comprehending (or understanding) a problem. The first arises from the Newman instruction "Tell me what the question is asking you to do." This is effectively asking whether the child understands the structure of the problem. It does not probe any understanding of the second element which involves understanding of the concepts.it was clear that some children understood the "sticks" problem in terms of knowing that they had to find the length of the shorter stick but did not have an understanding of the "difference" concept. From our observations, it appears that the very act of drawing a diagram focuses the child's attention on what the numbers represent rather than the numbers as abstract entities. In this sense, the function of a diagram may be to act" as an alternative form of "expressing the problem in one's own words". Having focused on the meaning of the numbers and their relationships within the problem, it then appears that the diagram can act as a key aid in the transformation stage. Helpful Features of Diagrams Two elements featured regularly in diagrams that led to successful outcomes. First, the representation of relationships in a clear, visual form and second, the incorporation of numerical information from the" problem into the diagram itself. Irrespective of whether the diagram drawn wa~ concrete, semi-concrete or \ l symbolic, it was clearly evident that the fea~re in diagrams that led to successful ".outcomes was the representation of relationships involved. Stages in diagram development A sizable proportion of the pupils were unable to succeed even with the authors' diagrams in the primary school study. The authors' diagrams were all at the symbolic end of the continuum described earlier. (The author's diagram for~roblem 1 is shown below.) It could well be the case that some children were not ready for such representations and yet might have been able to interpret a more physical or literal version. This leads us to conjecture that children may need to progress through stages of development in the drawing and interpretation of diagrams for problem-solving. They need to move from a concrete form of representation to a more symbolic form. Conclusion Figure 7 AUDanan.. 12 Pupil. " bananas each Thrown from here away Most children in the primary school study showed a general willingness to use the strategy of drawing diagrams. There are strong indications in the first study that drawing diagrams can be an important strategy

674 in problem solving but one that is not exploited in the classroom. The desire to push children too quickly into abstract manipulation and algorithms may be one reason for this. And possibly teachers may have the perception (which is passed on to the children) that drawing diagrams is somehow less mathematically respectable than algebraic manipulation. In our investigation involving secondary school pupils we have found that the type of diagrams drawn by children increases in sophistication as they mature. This has implications for teachers intending to use diagrams as a problem solving strategy. Young children need to be initiated into the technique of drawing diagrams beginning with a concrete representation of the problem and then moving to semi-concrete and later into more abstract forms of representations as they become more mature. The results of the two studies strongly suggest that diagrams drawn by pupils which represent relational aspects of the problem provide a definite aid to a successful solution of a p(oblem. In these two studies we have found that there is a lack of spontaneity, often bordering on resistance on the part of the pupils, in drawing diagrams to solve problems. There are strong indications that children both in primary and secondary schools are not introduced to this technique of problem solving, which is a matter of concern. We have shown here that drawing diagrams to solve problems is a useful strategy. However, if this strategy is to be taken seriously it must be taught. A rich diversity of examples and techniques should be introduced to pupils. We feel that there is a rich field here for further research both in identifying the salient features of helpful diagrams and in developing suitable\curriculum materials. (We would like to acknowledge with thanks the valuable comments and suggestions from Dr. P.S. Khoo of University of Brunei Darussalam) References: Driscoll, Mark 1.(1979) Mathematical Problem-Solving: Not lust a Matter of Words. Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Education. Hyde A.A.&Hyde R.f.,(1991).Mathwise-Teaching Mathematical Thinking alld Problem Solvillg. Portsmouth: Heinemann. Lopez-Real, Veloo,P.K., & Maawiah(l991). Analysis of Pupil's Perfonnance in the Brunei Darussalam PCE(1989) Mathematics Paper I and 11. University of Brunei Darussalam, Brunei Darussalam. Lopez-Real,F&Veloo,P.K.(1993) Children's Use of Diagrams as a Problem-Solving Strategy. Proceedings of PME XVII, Tsukuba,Japan. Moses, Barbara.(1982) "Visualization: A Different Approach to Problem Solving." School Science and Mathematics 82 : 141-147. Nelson, Dennis W.(1983) "Math Is Not a Problem... When You Know How to Visualize It." Illstructor 93 : 54-55 Newman, M.A.(1977). An analysis of sixth-grade pupils' errors on written mathematical tasks. Victorian IlIStitute for Educational Research Bulletin, 39, 31-43. Polya,G.,(1957). How to Solve it.(2n edition), New York, Doubleday. Riedesel, C. Allen.(1969) "Problem Solving: Some Suggestionsfrom Research." Arithmetic Teacher 16: 54-55 Schoenfeld,A.H.(1985). Mathematical Problem Solving. New York: Academic Press. Yancey, Anna V et al.(1989)"children Must Learn to Draw Diagrams." Arithmetic Teacher 36(7), 15-19.