An investigation into the sensory properties of materials

Similar documents
Function Number 1 Work as part of a team. Thorough knowledge of theoretical procedures and ability to integrate knowledge and performance into

Friction Stops Motion

5.1 Sound & Light Unit Overview

How Does It Feel? Sensory Processing, Brain Functioning and Behavior. Agenda. Acknowledgements. Presented by Gerry Morgan, M.S. Behavior Specialist

Mathematics subject curriculum

THE WEB 2.0 AS A PLATFORM FOR THE ACQUISITION OF SKILLS, IMPROVE ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE AND DESIGNER CAREER PROMOTION IN THE UNIVERSITY

Here are some helpful steps to guide you in completing the Contributor s Form below:

Inquiry Learning Methodologies and the Disposition to Energy Systems Problem Solving

Lower and Upper Secondary

South Carolina English Language Arts

Client Psychology and Motivation for Personal Trainers

Sensory evaluation. Teachers guide (primary)

Simulation in Maritime Education and Training

Learning and Teaching

What is Research? A Reconstruction from 15 Snapshots. Charlie Van Loan

Physical Features of Humans

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

ARTS IMPACT INSTITUTE LESSON PLAN Core Program Year 1 Arts Foundations VISUAL ARTS LESSON Unity and Variety in a Textural Collage

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

Strategy Study on Primary School English Game Teaching

SPATIAL SENSE : TRANSLATING CURRICULUM INNOVATION INTO CLASSROOM PRACTICE

The Political Engagement Activity Student Guide

Lecturing Module

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Grade 3 Science Life Unit (3.L.2)

What is Thinking (Cognition)?

Changing User Attitudes to Reduce Spreadsheet Risk

Guidelines for Writing an Internship Report

B. How to write a research paper

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

KS1 Transport Objectives

ADDIE MODEL THROUGH THE TASK LEARNING APPROACH IN TEXTILE KNOWLEDGE COURSE IN DRESS-MAKING EDUCATION STUDY PROGRAM OF STATE UNIVERSITY OF MEDAN

A 3D SIMULATION GAME TO PRESENT CURTAIN WALL SYSTEMS IN ARCHITECTURAL EDUCATION

Starting primary school

Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching. Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta

Designing a Rubric to Assess the Modelling Phase of Student Design Projects in Upper Year Engineering Courses

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Special Educational Needs & Disabilities (SEND) Policy

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Evaluation of Learning Management System software. Part II of LMS Evaluation

Playwriting KICK- START. Sample Pages. by Lindsay Price

EQuIP Review Feedback

Creating Coherent Inquiry Projects to Support Student Cognition and Collaboration in Physics

Utilizing Soft System Methodology to Increase Productivity of Shell Fabrication Sushant Sudheer Takekar 1 Dr. D.N. Raut 2

ACTION LEARNING: AN INTRODUCTION AND SOME METHODS INTRODUCTION TO ACTION LEARNING

Twitter Sentiment Classification on Sanders Data using Hybrid Approach

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

Proposal of Pattern Recognition as a necessary and sufficient principle to Cognitive Science

Timeline. Recommendations

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

Spatial Strengths Betty Maxwell, M.A.

Linking the Ohio State Assessments to NWEA MAP Growth Tests *

E C C. American Heart Association. Basic Life Support Instructor Course. Updated Written Exams. February 2016

7. Stepping Back. 7.1 Related Work Systems that Generate Folding Nets. The problem of unfolding three-dimensional models is not a new one (c.f.

Taste And Sight Anatomy Study Guide

Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: GRADE 1

November 2012 MUET (800)

Problems of practice-based Doctorates in Art and Design: a viewpoint from Finland

MATH Study Skills Workshop

Relating Math to the Real World: A Study of Platonic Solids and Tessellations

Lesson 1 Taking chances with the Sun

Metadiscourse in Knowledge Building: A question about written or verbal metadiscourse

CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT

LINKING LIBRARY SPACES TO USERS NEEDS AND PREFERENCES

Inside the mind of a learner

Learning Methods for Fuzzy Systems

Measurement. When Smaller Is Better. Activity:

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Word Segmentation of Off-line Handwritten Documents

Bachelor of Science in Mechanical Engineering with Co-op

Strategy for teaching communication skills in dentistry

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS

Communication around Interactive Tables

Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C

Monitoring and Evaluating Curriculum Implementation Final Evaluation Report on the Implementation of The New Zealand Curriculum Report to

Executive Summary. Osan High School

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Facing our Fears: Reading and Writing about Characters in Literary Text

P. Belsis, C. Sgouropoulou, K. Sfikas, G. Pantziou, C. Skourlas, J. Varnas

Lesson Plan Art: Painting Techniques

Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Policy Taverham and Drayton Cluster

Course Syllabus. Instructor Information. Course Description. Prerequisites/Corequisites. OCIs. Course Objectives

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Whole School Evaluation REPORT. Tigh Nan Dooley Special School Carraroe, County Galway Roll Number: 20329B

Introduction to Communication Essentials

THINKING TOOLS: Differentiating the Content. Nanci Cole, Michelle Wikle, and Sacha Bennett - TOSAs Sandi Ishii, Supervisor of Gifted Education

Language Acquisition Chart

Lesson objective: Year: 5/6 Resources: 1a, 1b, 1c, 1d, 1e, 1f, Examples of newspaper orientations.

Science Fair Rules and Requirements

10.2. Behavior models

The Ontario Curriculum

Software Maintenance

Characteristics of Functions

Intensive Writing Class

This Performance Standards include four major components. They are

Python Machine Learning

Transcription:

An investigation into the sensory properties of materials H Zuo a, T Hope a, P Castle b, M Jones c, a Design & Advanced Technology Research Centre, Faculty of Technology, Southampton Institute, Southampton SO14 RD, UK b Psychology group, Faculty of Social Science, Southampton Institute, Southampton SO14 YN, UK c Product design group, Faculty of Technology, Southampton Institute, Southampton SO14 RD, UK Abstract An initial matrix of material representation was established for studying human perception and response to different materials. A conceptual discrimination was clarified between texture and texture perception. Analysis of the preliminary results from the psychological tests have enabled four character s of texture description to be proposed: geometrical ; physical-chemical ; emotional ; and associative. Lexicons within these s were found to be slightly different in responsive sensitiveness under different conditions (material surface finish, sensory conditions, subject groups etc.). A comparison was made between visual touch and blindfold touch. With the proposed -lexicon system, people could subjectively describe the material texture in a common way. keywords materials, sensory perception, tactual texture, product design 1. Introduction Our physiological, psychological and cultural responses and expectation are key elements of the way we perceive the products we see and use, and should be considered much more in the design strategy for new manufactured products[1]. When consumers are faced with a product for the first time, they will formulate a perception of the product partly based on the materials sensory properties (colour, texture, sound, smell, taste ). Selecting materials with particular sensory properties will enhance the product s total image and the market's perception of its value. Although industrial product designers are very aware of the importance of material as the medium through which products are interpreted, they do not have an in-depth body of research information to use, about the sensory properties of materials. This investigation is concerned with our perception of the inherent and perceived properties of materials within a holistic environment. This will be referred to as 'material representation'. It is defined as: the perceived images, properties, meanings, and values of a material in the human-product interface under a specific set of environmental conditions. The term emphasises: 1. a human-material dialogue; 2.sensory perception; 3. the environmental context. Information regarding this material representation would be a very important aid in material selection for today s product designer. This research programme aims to investigate material representation within a framework or matrix (such as the one outlined in Fig.1 be), identify all the elements or variables in the whole matrix, and then carry out an indepth systematic investigation into the relationships between these elements or variables under controlled experimental conditions (materials as samples) and contextual conditions (materials applied in manufactured products), respectively. The theoretical analysis and the experimental findings will contribute to the development of a new database integrating the research with other evolving new knowledge about the subjective-objective dialogue. This innovative database is expected to make it possible for designers, artists, and engineers (through innovative treatment and application of existing and emerging materials) to be able to create artefacts more effectively, matching human perceptual, sensory and emotional expectation. This paper reports the first-stage results from this research program. Research was focused on tactual texture of materials, being carried out through theoretical, empirical, and controlled experimental methods.

Fig.1 Diagram of materials representation 2 Theoretical research on texture 2.1 Texture and texture perception Originally, the word texture was a textile term, a quality of fabrics appraised and appreciated through the sense of touch. Then gradually the concept was expanded to a wider range from a philosophical and cognitive angle of view, and also was transplanted to a more specific meaning in the materials science and engineering fields. In the field of materials science and engineering, texture, as a specific term, means a particular orientation distribution in microstructure. When a piece of metal is deformed by some directional process such as wiredrawing or rolling, the constituent crystal grains acquire a preferred orientation. This orientation is called texture[2]. The orientation can result in changes of material properties, but because of its micro-scale magnitude (the crystallitic grain size is usually from 1µm to 1µm or even smaller from 1nm to 1nm(nano-materials)), the texture is difficult for a human to perceive directly by sensation. This texture can only be tested and analysed by means of specialist equipment such as an X-ray diffraction device etc. So, essentially, this could be called micro-texture. On the other hand, cognitive scientists have recognised texture as a visual 1 cue[3] that plays a significant role in a variety of cognitive tasks. A common use is in describing and differentiating between different kinds of objects, for example, wallpaper, furniture, carpets, sand, and grass. A working definition of texture in this context is the surface markings on an object or the 2-D appearance of a surface[3]. Obviously, here the texture is not micro- but macro- as it can be perceived by sensation. However, surface markings include something more geometrical like isotropy, roughness etc. and partly physical, e.g. colour. This definition seems suitable for visual texture but not for tactual texture. Tactual texture should go beyond merely surface markings, and should concern other characteristics such as the moisture retention level, thermal conduction, temperature etc. By integrating the content of tactual texture into visual texture, we propose a definition of texture: the geometrical configuration and physical-chemical attributes of the surface or the bulk of materials/ 1 in fact, not only visual but also tactual.

objects). In this definition, the content of texture includes two aspects: geometrical configuration the spatial constructive elements and their shape, size, orientation and distribution which will be perceived as, e.g., fine, granular, linear, regular etc.; and physical-chemical attributes the dynamic characters which need energy exchange (such as mechanical, thermal, optical, etc.) with environment and is time-related, and usually perceived as warm, cold, hard, soft, shining, moist, sticky, etc. We emphasise that it is necessary to discriminate between two concepts. One is texture, the other is perceived texture or texture perception. The former is objective, the latter is subjective. As a comparison, we also propose a definition of perceived texture: a synthesis of physiological and psychological response and impression to the geometrical configuration and physical-chemical attributes of the surface or the bulk of materials/objects. In this definition, the synthesis means it is not simply A plus B but A fusing with B, therefore the subjective responses to A (geometrical configuration) and B (physical-chemical attributes) would interact. Although under certain conditions (e.g. by vision), the response to geometrical characteristics may be dominant over physical-chemical attributes of texture, or the inverse, under other conditions (e.g., by blindfold touch). What s more, the subjective response will possibly go beyond these two aspects as can be found in the experimental research (section 3). It is the very understanding of the correlation between the objective texture parameters and the subjective texture perception which will provide the framework for creating suitable, aesthetic material textures. 2.2 Subjective description of texture In parallel to identifying the geometrical, physical-chemical properties for texture from the objective side, it is also necessary to understand how people subjectively perceive texture. Researchers have investigated material texture description although the reports have proved to be very limited. Ohno R[4] in his study of visual perception of texture of building materials, used six pairs of adjectives to describe the visual texture of different materials: 1). rich-poor; 2). rough-smooth; 3). bright-dark; 4). hard-soft; 5). heavy-light; 6). warm-cold. Ozawa et al.[5] extracted three s for the identification of visual texture of wood surface under normal lighting conditions (incandescent light 6 lux, fluorescent light 6 lux, and natural light): 1). comfortable; 2). tense/nervous; 3). tasty/tastefulness, while under the condition of strong intensity of lighting (3 lux, 7 lux), the impression of visual texture was dominated mainly by the two s: comfortable and tasty/tastefulness. Therefore it seems that the s of texture identification tend to be influenced by the environmental context. Tanaka Minami [6], in an experiment of texture evaluation of handmade straw paper, obtained other s for describing the texture of paper. They are: 1). calmlively; 2). comfortable-uncomfortable; 3). warm-cold. Terauchi Fumio et al[7] in a study of evaluation of a gathered leather seat, extracted five s for constructing the texture evaluation of leather materials. The five s are: 1). boldness; 2). regularity; 3). softness; 4). elegance; 5). tension. Aoki H[8] in his early research on material sensory properties, used four s for tactual texture evaluation of several different materials such as aluminium, iron, glass, plastic. The four s are: 1). warm-cold (thermal character); 2). wet-dry (moist character); 3). rough-smooth (surface contour); 4). loosedense (denseness). A fifth, preference, was added as a general impression evaluation especially related to materials selection. These s were analysed both for visual tactual and blindfold tactual conditions in Aoki s experiment, but whether there was difference in s under these two conditions was not indicated. In another study [9], Aoki used 25 s for the subjective evaluation of leather texture by vision and by touch. The 25 s are: 1). isotropic anisotropic; 2). warm cold; 3). with wet absorption without wet absorption; 4). with wet transmission without wet transmission; 5). smooth rough; 6). dense loose; 7). with air transmission without air transmission; 8). glossy non glossy; 9). regular irregular; 1). light weighty; 11). flexible unflexible; 12). elastic non-elastic; 13). good bad; 14). attracting disgusting; 15). applicable non-applicable; 16). good tone bad tone; 17). lightness lightness; 18). cold colour warm colour; 19). colourful colourful; 2). compress-resistance compress-resistance; 21). compress-recovery compress-recovery; 22). bend-resistance bend-resistance; 23). bend-recovery bend-recovery; 24). extend-resistance extendresistance; 25). extend-recovery extend-recovery. Generally, it seems that all the above character s (words or word pairs) of texture description are different between researchers and show a lack of a systematic classification. For this purpose, we carried out a preliminary study to identify suitable character s.

3 Experimental research 3.1 Experiment method The controlled experiments were conducted to assess a person s response to different material surfaces. Information was obtained through both visual touch and blindfold touch on the surface of various material samples. In the experiment, 21 pieces of material in a specific size from four categories (aluminium, plastic, ceramic, rubber) were adopted as samples which were coded as aluminium: AL1(rolled), AL2, AL3, AL4(sandblasted with different grain size), AL5(mirror-polished), AL8, AL9, AL1(PVD with different-sized patterns); plastic: ABS1~ABS1(moulded with different patterns); ceramic: C1(polished with pattern), C2(natural); rubber: R1, R2, R3(different patterns). 37 Psychology students (average age 24 years) and 24 Design students (average age 21 years) served as participants in the tests. Both the Design and Psychology participants were divided into two groups: one group for visual touch tests, the other for blindfold touch tests. The environmental condition was set as: temperature 2 C; humidity 5%; and under fluorescent light. The material samples were presented to the participants in a random order. Participants were then asked to select words from a vocabulary list or go beyond the list by using more words chosen by themselves to describe the material surface they touched. The vocabulary list (see Table 1) was set up by the authors through unifying all the most common words of texture description used in existing reports of other researchers. Photographs of test participants are shown in Fig.2 and Fig.3. Table 1 the texture vocabulary list presented to the participants in the test coarse, fine, smooth, rough, bold, bumpy, plain, isotropic, anisotropic, repetitive, nonrepetitive, linear, radial, regular, irregular, dot-scattered, line-scattered, granular, arbitrary, simple, complex, moist, clammy, dry, arid, warm, cold, dense, loose, hard, soft, light, heavy, clear, vague, dim, bright, dark, shining, glossy, unshining, gloomy, sticky, unsticky, viscoid, non-viscoid, viscoelastic, non-viscoelastic, comfortable, uncomfortable, flabby, tense, nervous, modern, traditional, calm, lively, showy, depressing, dull, cheerful, safe, relaxed, tedious, elegant, ugly, tasteful, tasteless. Fig.2 blindfold touch test Fig.3 visual touch test 3.2 Results and discussion 3.2.1 Dimensions of texture perception By grouping the -frequency words and combining them into pairs which have bipolar meanings, e.g. warm cold, (except the associative description words), we classified these word pairs, which here we called lexicons, into four s: geometrical, physical-chemical, emotional, and associative.

Geometrical : this describes the subjective response to the geometrical configuration of a material surface. High-frequency lexicons used in this include such as: smooth rough, fine coarse, plain bumpy, regular irregular, linear nonlinear, etc. Physical-chemical : this describes the subjective response to the physical and/or chemical attributes of a material surface. High-frequency lexicons used in this include such as: warm cold, hard soft, moist dry, shining unshining, sticky unsticky, etc. Emotional : this describes the hedonic, valuable, affective feelings which are evoked by touching the material surface. High-frequency lexicons in this include such as: comfortable uncomfortable, lively/cheerful dull, elegant ugly, modern traditional, etc. Associative : this describes the subjective association from the material, that is: to what existing things in the perceiver s experience can the texture be compared? This description is beyond the description of geometrical and physical-chemical characteristics, and is much more individualdependent. Therefore the lexicons in this are random, and have frequency, but they are rich, such as plastic-like (the material in fact may not be plastic), matt-like, rubber-like, treebark-like, animalskin-like, honeycomb-like, dimples-like, ice-like, etc. We obtained a minimum collection of lexicons by looking at where the lexicons intersected in terms of different material categories, different surface finish, different sensory conditions, different subject groups. This minimum lexicon collection {Dimensions-Lexicons} min which we called the primary texture lexicons collection was shown in Fig.4. Similarly, by unifying all the lexicons in terms of the above different conditions, a maximum collection of lexicons {Dimensions-Lexicons} max which we called secondary texture lexicons collection was obtained and shown in Fig.5. {Dimension-Lexicon} min Frequency of being used Geometrical Smooth Rough Plain/flat bumpy Fine coarse Frequency of being used Physical-chemical Warm Cold Moist/clammy Dry Hard Soft Frequency of being used Emotional Comfortable Uncomfortable Lively/cheerful Dull Associative (No shared lexicon, dependent upon material categories, surface treatment, sensory conditions, subject characters etc. ) Fig.4 {Dimension-Lexicon} min the primary lexicons collection of material tactual texture With this Dimension-Lexicon system, the subjective perception of material texture would hopefully be described consistently. Only the detailed lexicons within each tend to be slightly different in the number of lexicons and responsive sensitiveness by subjects under different conditions. We have compared the details between visual touch and blindfold touch. 3.2.2 Texture lexicons in terms of sensory conditions The lexicons within each in terms of visual touch and blindfold touch was compared in Fig.6 From Fig.6, it can be seen that between visual touch and blindfold touch, the description lexicons within the four s have different coincidence (that is the rate of shared lexicons).

{Dimension-Lexicon} max Geometrical Physical-chemical Frequency of being used smooth rough plain/flat bumpy fine coarse granular ungranular regular irregular repetitive unrepetitive line-scattered dot-scattered linear nonlinear ridgy - unridgy isotropic anisotropic speckled unspeckled indented bitty continuous Frequency of being used warm cold moist/clammy dry hard soft shining unshining sticky unsticky bright dark glossy matt slippery abrasive reflective unreflective dense loose light heavy fragile pliable Emotional Associative Frequency of being used comfortable uncomfortable lively/cheerful dull elegant ugly modern traditional tasteful tasteless clean dirty exciting boring relaxed nervous flexible rigid/solid safe dangerous clear vague attractive tedious happy unhappy depressing strong weak soothing irritating expensive cheap Frequency of being used rubbery (R123) plastic-like (AL1, ABS7891, ABS123, AL5, AL891) metallic (AL891, AL234, AL1, ABS7891) matt-like (AL234) leather-like (ABS7891, R123) silky (AL5, AL891, C12) wood-like (C12, AL234, ABS7891) sandpaper-like (AL234) mirror-like (AL5) animalskin-like (ABS7891) bubbly (AL891, ABS7891) oily (AL5) glass-like (AL5) honeycomb-like (AL891) ceramic-like (C12) stone-like (C12) orangy (AL891) (some examples are given here) Fig.5 {Dimension-Lexicon} max the secondary lexicons collection of material tactual texture The frequency of the lexicons in the geometrical is generally er for visual touch than for blindfold touch. This means the responsive sensitiveness to geometrical configuration for visual touch is greater than that for blindfold touch. In other words, the combination of vision and touch can strengthen the subjective perception to geometrical configuration of a material surface. But interestingly, the response to smooth rough, seems to be approximately the same level for both conditions, or even a bit more sensitive for blindfold touch. The reason for this is to be further explored. (Fig.6 a)) Within the physical-chemical, the subjective response to the three est-frequency lexicons warm cold, moist dry, hard soft, and slippery abrasive, is stronger for blindfold touch than that for visual touch. On the other hand, the response to other characters such as shining unshining, bright dark, glossy matt, etc. is more sensitive for visual touch than that for blindfold touch. This is not difficult to understand because these

Texture lexicons in geometrical a) 1 8 6 4 2 visual touch blindfold touch 91.1 95.4 36.75 42.5 21.2 23.7 23.1 16.3 16.6 12.5 1 13.95 7.4 8.1 4.9 6 6.6 1.6 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 texture lexicons 1.smooth rough 2.fine coarse 3.plain bumpy 4.regular irregular 5.granular ungranular 6.simple complex 7.repetitive unrepetitive 8.linescattered dotscattered 9.isotropic anisotropic 1.ridgy unridgy Texture lexicons in Physical-chemical b) 1 8 6 4 2 visual touch blindfold touch 73.4 63.55 61.4 52.5 52.8 38.15 4.5 3.1 21.15 17.9 2.7 15.9 6.2 1.95 3.25 7.25 8.4 12 4.2 5.8 2.2 6.3 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 11 texture lexicons 1.warm cold 2.moist dry 3.hard soft 4.shining unshining 5.bright dark 6.glossy matt 7.sticky unsticky 8.reflective unreflective 9.dense loose 1.light heavy 11.slippery - abrasive c) 1 8 6 4 2 5.3 51.55 48.5 29.1 Texture lexicons in emotional visual touch blindfold touch 19.95 23.55 19.65 6.9 5.7 9.15 1.8 9.15 4.2 5.85 4.75 6.2 6.37.9 8.5 1.45 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 11 12 texture lexicons 1.Lively/cheerful dull 2.comfortable uncomfortable 3.tasteful tasteless 4.elegant ugly 5.modern traditional 6.relaxed nervous 7.exciting boring 8.attractive tedious 9.depressing 1.clear vague 11.clean dirty 12.flexible rigid/solid d) 1 8 6 4 2 Texture lexicons in associative (Design participants) visual touch blindfold touch 13.2 1.7 3.6 8.8 5.7 4.9 4.8 6 7.6 5.8 3.5 3.5 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.3 3.9 3.6 3.5 2.5 2.1 2.1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 texture lexicons 1.plastic-like 2.metallic 3.rubbery 4.bubbly 5.wood-like 6.leather-like 7.silky 8.matt-like 9.ceramic-like 1.stone-like 11.orangy 12.animalskin-like 13.honeycom-like 14.sandpaper-like 15.oily 16.ripples-like 17.mirror-like 18.clay-like Fig.6 Comparison of the lexicons of subjective texture description between visual touch and blindfold touch: a) geometrical ; b) physical-chemical ; c) emotional ; d) associative.

characteristics are directly related to vision. But it is interesting that these usually vision-related characteristics can still be perceived when subjects are blindfolded. (Fig.6 b)) The number of lexicons in the emotional is larger for visual touch than for blindfold touch. Except for the feeling of comfortable uncomfortable, other emotional feelings for visual touch seem stronger than those for blindfold touch. (Fig.6 c)) The lexicons in the associative tend to be at the same level both for visual touch and blindfold touch, but the situation might not be so simple because these descriptions are much more dependent upon material surface finish, material categories, and subject individuals. This issue will be addressed separately. (Fig.6 d)) The number of lexicons, lexicons coincidence and response sensitiveness are slightly different for texture perception for visual touch and for blindfold touch. It is concluded that when subjects touch the surface of materials, vision can increase the response to geometrical configuration. Emotional feelings can also be enriched and strengthened. Interestingly, blindfold touch can increase the responsive sensitiveness to some physicalchemical characteristics, particularly warm cold, moist dry, and hard soft. The results of texture perception lexicons in terms of materials surface finish, subject groups (disciplines, gender, etc.) will be reported in further papers. 4 Conclusion 1) Human perception to materials should be investigated systematically in a holistic environment, which would be an important aid for materials selection in product design. 2) Texture and texture perception or perceived texture are two different concepts, the former is objective, the latter is subjective. For investigating the correlation between them, the s both for objective texture identification and for subjective texture description need to be explored in parallel. 3) Subjective texture description of materials are to be classified into four s: geometrical, physical-chemical, emotional, and associative. 4) Under different conditions, texture lexicons within four s are slightly different in the number of lexicons, and responsive sensitiveness. 5) In the material texture perception by touch, vision can increase the response to geometrical configuration, and enrich, strengthen the emotional feelings. Blindfold can increase the responsive sensitiveness to some physical-chemical characteristics, particularly warm cold, moist dry, and hard soft. Reference 1. C Thomas Mitchell, (1996), New Thinking in Design: Conversations on Theory and Practice, Van Nostrand Reinhold. 2. R W Cahn, P Haasen, eds, (1996), Physical Metallurgy, 4 th edt. Elsevier Science BV. 3. Nalini B, A Ravishankar Rao, (1997), The Texture Lexicon: Understanding the Categorization of Visual Texture Terms and Their Relationship to Texture Images, Cognitive Science, vol.21, no.2, p.219-246 4. Ohno R, (198), Visual Perception of Texture: Development of A Scale of the Perceived Surface Roughness of Building Materials, Environmental Design Research Association, vol.11, p.193~2. 5. Ozawa K, Terauchi F, Kubo M, Aoki H, Suzuki T, (1996), Effect of Light Source on Visual Image of Wood Surface, Bulletin of JSSD. 6. Tanaka M, (1998), Experimental Manufacture and Image Estimation on Handmade Straw Paper, Bulletin of JSSD, vol.44, no.5, p.27~36. 7. Terauchi F, Fukami R, Matsuoka Y, Kubo M, Aoki H, (1997), Evaluative Structure of the Gathered Leather Seat and Personal Preference Difference, Bulletin of JSSD, vol.44, no.4, p.49-56. 8. Aoki H, Suzuki T, Kamei A, (1985), Study on Correspondence between Sensory and Physical Properties of materials ( I ), Bulletin of JSSD, no.53, p.37~42. 9. Aoki H, Suzuki T, Matsuoka Y, (1985), Study on Correspondence between Sensory and Physical Properties of materials ( II ) Comparison of natural and substitute leather for hand, Bulletin of JSSD, no.53, p.43~48