A Trio of Phonetic Details in Homalco

Similar documents
Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Unvoiced Landmark Detection for Segment-based Mandarin Continuous Speech Recognition

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Phonology Revisited: Sor3ng Out the PH Factors in Reading and Spelling Development. Indiana, November, 2015

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

Phonological encoding in speech production

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

DIBELS Next BENCHMARK ASSESSMENTS

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

NCU IISR English-Korean and English-Chinese Named Entity Transliteration Using Different Grapheme Segmentation Approaches

An Acoustic Phonetic Account of the Production of Word-Final /z/s in Central Minnesota English

The Bruins I.C.E. School

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Why Is the Chinese Curriculum Difficult for Immigrants Children from Southeast Asia

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

ADDIS ABABA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES MODELING IMPROVED AMHARIC SYLLBIFICATION ALGORITHM

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

RP ENGLISH AND CASTILIAN SPANISH DIPHTHONGS REVISITED FROM THE BEATS-AND-BINDING PERSPECTIVE

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

The Journey to Vowelerria VOWEL ERRORS: THE LOST WORLD OF SPEECH INTERVENTION. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education. Preparation: Education

Developing Grammar in Context

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

1. READING ENGAGEMENT 2. ORAL READING FLUENCY

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD IN STANDARD MALA Y

Learning to Read and Spell Words:

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

Teachers Guide Chair Study

Conducting an interview

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Automatic English-Chinese name transliteration for development of multilingual resources

Weave the Critical Literacy Strands and Build Student Confidence to Read! Part 2

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

A Fact in Historical Phonology from the Viewpoint of Generative Phonology: The Underlying Schwa in Old English

Contrasting English Phonology and Nigerian English Phonology

Journal of Phonetics

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Underlying Representations

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

Films for ESOL training. Section 2 - Language Experience

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Don t Let Me Fall inspired by James McBride's memoir, The Color of Water

ANT4034: HISTORY OF ANTHROPOLOGICAL THEORY Spring 2014 Syllabus

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Fisk Street Primary School

2 months: Social and Emotional Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

An argument from speech pathology

Transcription:

A Trio of Phonetic Details in Homalco John Hamilton Davis Bellingham, Washington Abstract: This paper presents three phonetic features of Homalco and other dialects of Comox. These three features are offglides between a palatal obstruent and a following nonhomorganic vowel, laryngealized stops and vowels, and the heterosyllabic nature of sequences of consonants, which do not function as consonant clusters in the generally accepted meaning of the term cluster. 1 Introduction In their 2008 editorial The phonetics of North American languages, McDonough and Whalen point out that even the best orthographies do not note phonetic detail and are usually of limited value for phonetic sciences. They quote Sapir (1921, p, 55), There remains the important question of the dynamics of these phonetic elements [which] are important for the proper understanding of the phonetic genius of a language as the sound system itself, often far more so. They also write, In examining the phonetic structure of an under-documented language, the focus of the research is the natural phenomena, not a particular theory. The phenomena must be described as fully as possible. Homalco is the northernmost dialect of the language known in the literature as Mainland Comox. The first speakers who made an impression on me with their style of enunciation were the Homalco speakers Noel George Harry, born in 1892, Bill Galligos, born in 1903, and Jimi Wilson, born in 1945, along with numerous casual speakers from 1969 up to 1980. I also worked extensively with Mary George, born in 1924, who lived all her life at Sliammon, as well as having casual conversations with other men and women at Sliammon and Church House, the then home of the Homalco Band. In my master s thesis at the University of Victoria in 1970, I did not identify each utterance by speaker, partly because not all of my observations were made during formal language sessions. But I did distinguish between men s and women s speech, which includes the differences between Mary George and my other language consultants. 2 First history The first field notes that I ve been able to obtain are from Franz Boas, written onto file slips around 1887. Boas worked with the Island Comox dialect and not with one of the three Mainland Comox dialects. These slips are in the Smithsonian Anthropological Archives as Document 711 b. Although this document gives much phonetic data, fine phonetic detail is not discernable. For In Papers for the International Conference on Salish and Neighbouring Languages 50, University of British Columbia Working Papers in Linguistics 40, Natalie Weber, Erin Guntly, Zoe Lam, and Sihwei Chen (eds.), 2015.

example, although the letter ç uniquely represents the sound [ ], the letter q represents any one of the three sounds [ç w ], [ ], [ w ]. Edward Sapir, in his 1915 publication Noun Reduplication in Comox, wrote As not infrequently happens in American Indian languages, the long vowels are not always held out with even stress, but end with short rearticulations which give the whole vowel in each case a quasi-diphthongal effect. they cannot be considered the normal forms of the long vowels; sometimes the short rearticulations seem to serve as glides to following consonants, particularly velars. The quasi-diphthongal long vowels are here indicated by long vowels followed by superior short vowels, the vocalic quality of the latter being indicated as in normal short vowels (Sapir 1915: 3 4). Writing before the invention of portable recording machines, Sapir intended to convey fine phonetic detail, giving the reader as complete an idea as possible of the actual and exact pronunciation of the language. He did so without sacrificing a thoroughgoing analysis of reduplication patterns in the data he collected. 3 One source of Sapir s rearticulated vowels In my 2005 ICSNL paper, High consonants, articulatory transitions, and nonhigh vowels in Comox, I described one source of the phenomenon of what Sapir calls rearticulated vowels as being a palatal consonant followed by a phonemic /a/: Phonemic Phonetic Written for learners Gloss /pəq sčaǰən/ [pᴧq sčéᴧǰɪn] peq schîajen weasel /čalas/ [čéᴧlᴧs] chîalas three /č agay/ [č éᴧgᴧy] ch îagay old time wooden spoon When I asked how many syllables these words have, he replied two and a half Bill Galligos had been analyzing his language for years, thinking about it and making comparisons and analogies. Thom Hess had worked with Bill Galligos and recommended him highly. In this paper the digraph ia has approximately the same value as it has in the Pinyin alphabet, used to write Chinese. Illustrative alternations in the spelling herein between the digraphs ia and îa are (1) /čam / [č y ɛmʔ] chiam how, why (2) /x w aʔ čam as/ [ç w ʊ čéᴧmᴧs] whe chîamas nohow, noway, can t, won t 38

(3) / amšaʔ/ [ amš y ɛʔ] ~ [ amšíᴧʔ] thamshia ~ thamshîa twenty These examples illustrate the pedagogical orthography that I am using to transcribe my field notes and the texts which have been told to me. Parallel with the offglide from a palatal stop being heard before an /a/, most instances of a palatal stop followed by the vowel /u/ also have an audible offglide an exception is the word for child /čuy / [čuyʔ]. Some examples of the audible offglide are: (4) /č umən/ [č íumɪn] ch îumen a screw (5) /čuʔuɬqinəm/ [číuʔoɬqɛnəm] chîu olhqinem stealing food These examples of the y offglide are parallel with the pronunciation of the w offglide after a velar: (6) /k w amnač/ [k w ámnᴧč] ~ [kúᴧmnᴧč] kwamnach ~ kŵamnach root The phenomenon of the offglides being pronounced separately from the neighboring phonemic vowel suggests that Homalco and its related dialects are mora timed rather than being syllable timed or stress timed. They are also parallel with the normal, unemphatic pronunciation of the word no where the unvoiced segment is stressed and the neighboring voiced segment is unstressed. (7) /x w aʔ/ [çúᴧʔ] ŵha no Compare the emphatic pronunciation of no! (8) /x w aʔ/ [ç w áʔ a ] wha no! Here the initial segment is pronounced not as its own syllable (or mora) but is phonetically a syllable initial consonant. One instance of this emphatic denial came during a conversation with Tommy Paul, when I asked him what sasquatch may al alh [mayʔᴧlʔᴧɬ] eats. His response was Tam qigath, tam majath [tam qegᴧ, tam mᴧǰᴧ ] any kind of deer, any kind of meat then when I asked pileq? [p ləq] mushrooms his answer was an emphatic NO! 39

4 Stød as another source of Sapir s rearticulated vowels In the spring of 1970, Jimi Wilson visited me in Victoria. Jimi was a member of the Homalco band, raised in Church House. He visited the University of Victoria campus and classrooms and attended a number of social events. One of these was a cookout on the beach near Sooke. At that cookout, Jim Hoard asked Jimi to pronounce several words. When Jimi said the word for mussel Jim immediately identified the pronunciation as being an instance of stød with the same pronunciation as it would have if it were a word in Danish. Wikipedia defines stød [sdøð] as a suprasegmental unit of Danish phonology, which in its most common form is a kind of creaky voice (laryngealization), but may also be realized as a glottal stop, above all in emphatic pronunciation. The article goes on to say that the IPA character for glottal stop is used to transcribe stød. This description from Wikipedia also describes the situation in Homalco. I described this alternation in my M.A. thesis (pp. 24-27). Mary George pronounced a glottal stop where Noel George Harry and Bill Galligos regularly pronounced a rearticulated vowel, a dragged out vowel, with creaky voice and lower pitch. The Comox word for mussel which Jimi told Jim is /sam a/ [saᴧmᴧ] or [sa a mᴧ] (alternative notations for the same phenomenon) with creaky voice. In his 1915 publication, Edward Sapir wrote the word for mussel as sā a ba (p. 30). This pronunciation of [b] for /m/ is not unexpected. For example, Noel George Harry sometimes said /maǰa / as [bᴧǰᴧ ] meat and /ǰənəs/ as [ǰɪdɪs] tooth in allegro speech. One alternation which occurs in all dialects of Comox is /w/ ~ /g/ and /y/ ~ /ǰ/. One example is the formation of the word puppy (9) /č anəw/ [č éᴧno] ch îano dog (10) /č anaguɬ/ [č éᴧnagʊɬ] ch îanagolh puppy However, when the original glide is a laryngealized or glottalized resonant, the glottalization precedes the resulting voiced stop, resulting in the alternation called stød. Remember that the description of stød is an alternation between glottal stop and creaky voice. In Homalco this describes the following alternation: (11) /ɬaw / [ɬawʔ] lhaw escape (12) /ɬag it/ (or /ɬaʔgit/) [ɬa a git] / [ɬaᴧgit] ~ [ɬaʔgit] lha git he got away with /w/ /g/ before a vowel. A second example of this alternation is 40

(13) /təw / [tuʔ] tew freeze (14) /tag it/ [ta a git] / [taᴧgit] ~ [taʔgit] ta git frozen The negation of the word for good /ʔəy / [ʔi:ʔ] illustrates the alternation of /y/ /ǰ/ before a vowel. (15) /x w aʔ ʔaǰ as/ [ç w ʊ ʔa a ǰ y s] ~ [ç w ʊ ʔaʔǰ y s] whe a jas not good To restate, Wikipedia states that stød is represented by the symbol for a glottal stop. When the glottal stop is not realized, Sapir s rearticulated vowel is heard. However, during the 1970s the speakers who pronounced the rearticulated vowel also had creaky voice. Another example of stød before a laryngealized resonant is: (16) /ʔal as/ [ʔa a lᴧs] / [ʔaᴧlᴧs] not recorded as [ʔaʔlᴧs] sea cucumber Together with the words which Bill Galligos called two and a half syllables, this rearticulated, or echo, vowel phenomenon suggests that Homalco and its related dialects are mora-timed. 5 Studies of Other Languages Sonya Bird of the University of Victoria has done extensive analysis of laryngealized resonants in St at imcets, most recently in 2011. In that publication, Bird states, [one] way in which [laryngealized resonants] exhibit substantial variability is in the realization of the laryngeal gesture: from a complete stop to a small dip in fundamental frequency. This statement is reminiscent of the description of stød as given in Wikipedia. There have also been studies of laryngealized vowels in Otomanguean (Mazatec, Mixtec) and Hokan (Oaxaca Chontal) and Panoan (Capanhua) languages. Some of these studies are listed in the reference list of this paper. The laryngealization of vowels in Comox is not an isolated phenomenon. 6 Another source of laryngealized vowels in Homalco Vowels adjacent to glottalized stops and affricates can also be laryngealized. Some examples of the entire syllable being laryngealized are: (17) /t in/ [t n] t in barbecued fish (18) /ʔap uk w / [ʔap ok w ] ap ok w maggot 41

(19) /p u w / [p o w ] p oxw stink, bad smell Here the stops are not ejective as pronounced by Bill Galligos and Noel George Harry, but are laryngealized, and the vowels are creaky voice. Although other speakers, such as Noel George Harry, often pronounced glottalized stops and affricates not as ejectives but as laryngealized, with adjacent vowels enunciated as creaky voice, Bill Galligos never pronounced ejectives. When asked, he described /p / as on the side of b and /t / as on the side of d in other words, more like the voiced than the unvoiced stops in English. Ladefoged (1965) distinguishes between voiceless, ejective, laryngealized, and voiced stops in languages of the world. All four occur phonetically in Mainland Comox. However, when a glottalized stop was laryngealized rather than ejective, Bill Galligos and Noel George Harry, among others, pronounced the adjacent vowels as laryngealized. The Wikipedia article on the Achumawi language states, The laryngealized stops are similar in articulation to the ejective glottalized stops of neighboring languages, but more lenis, that is, not "popped" unless an unusual effort is made at articulating the distinction. Citing Ladefoged s phonation-types tape, Professor Phil Hoole of the University of Munich gives examples of Danish stød with the laryngealization equally likely to be on the vowel or on the adjacent resonant. This same website transcribes Hausa laryngealized stops using the symbols for voiced consonants with a subscript tilde. This is reminiscent of Bill Galligos equating glottalized stops with voiced English stops. 7 Another source of Sapir s rearticulated vowels Sapir (1915) transcribes a large number of forms with vocalic offglides into following consonants. Among the speakers whom I heard, the most noticeable offglide was when the front vowel was followed by the post-velar stop. Examples include: (20) /səp iq w atas/ [sᴧp ə q w atᴧs] sep iqwatas he hit him in the head (21) /t əšiq w / [t ɪš ə q w ] t eshiqw snot 8 Similar timing in the enunciation of consonants In their 2008 editorial The phonetics of North American languages, McDonough and Whalen write that Salishan languages have long sequences of consonants, uninterrupted by vowels that violate theoretical notions of syllable structure and phonetic salience. 42

In his 1978 paper, Syllabification in Northwest Indian languages, James Hoard describes how such sequences of consonants are actually pronounced here in the Northwest. In this paper, Hoard distinguishes between tautosyllabic and heterosyllabic consonant clusters. All the consonants in a tautosyllabic consonant cluster are pronounced together as a unit. By contrast, in heterosyllabic consonant clusters, the consonants are pronounced separately or in pairs. A Comox illustration of a heterosyllabic consonant cluster is: (22) /t ut θ štas/ [t ót θ.š.tᴧs] t oz shtas (S)he shot him/her in the foot/lower leg Each of the three consonants in the sequence -z sht- [t θ št] is enunciated separately and clearly. Here I am adopting James Hoard s convention of using a period on the line to show the separation of one mora from another. The segment [-š-] is a reduced form of the lexical suffix /-šən/ before the transitive suffix /-t/; the full form of this lexical suffix is seen in the following intransitive form, where there is no agent (23) /t ut θ šən čan/ [t ót θ.šin č y ɛn] t oz shen chian I was/am shot in the foot/lower leg Another example of how individuals consonants in a sequence are enunciated separately is (24) /ʔasx w / [ʔa.sç w ] aswh fur seal and the word for falling snow or snowflake is a reversal of the word for no wherein each continuant vowel and fricative is enunciated separately: (25) /ʔax w / [ʔa.ç w ] awh falling snow ; snowflake (26) /x w aʔ/ [ç w.ᴧʔ] ŵha no Similarly, I usually heard the following word pronounced bimoraically: (27) /qaw / [qa.w ] or [qa.u ] qawth potato But Susan Blake reported (p. 22) the diminutive with a vowel after the /w/ and the /w/ becomes /g/ prevocalically, which means that the form /qaw / does not have a vowel in the second mora. The bimoraic pronuncian of /qaw / potato sheds light on the following reduplicated forms: 43

(28) /qin qin/ [q.n.q n] qi nqin mallard (29) /tol tol/ [to.l.tol] to ltol Noel George Harry described to ltol as a spear throwing game or contest, where a stone wheel with a hole in its center was rolled across the ground and the players or contestants tried to throw a spear through the hole as the wheel rolls. These two examples suggest that some instances of glottal stop are epenthetic, inserted as the resonant is enunciated separately from the preceding vowel. In his paper, James Hoard cited examples from Quileute, Nisqually, Columbian, Nez Perce, and Bella Coola. In his conclusion, he writes, Nearly all descriptions of Northwest languages contain phonemic transcriptions which mask some of the important phonetic properties of these languages. This is especially true of syllabication. He advises learning to pronounce the individual consonants themselves [rather] than pronouncing combinations of them in clusters. This is true of Homalco (and Sliammon) and the same advice can be given for pronouncing the vowels as well. 9 A mora-timed language The individual enunciation of serial consonants is one more example suggesting that Homalco and its related dialects are mora-timed. It is mora-timed not only in its vowels, as noted in Sapir's transcription, but also in its syllable initial consonants as well as its syllable final consants. This gives the language a rhythm quite different from spoken English. The moraic nature of Sliammon syllable peaks and syllable final consonants has been noted previously by Susan Blake (1992). However, I have not found that she discusses a word initial consonant receiving its own moraic value. 10 A possible language change One word which is remembered and used today is the word which Noel George Harry translated as cousins and friends jiajia. However, its pronunciation has changed from the old pronunciation of two and a half syllables (30) /ǰaǰa/ [ǰíᴧǰ y ] jîajia to be just two syllables (31) /ǰaǰa/ [ǰ y ǰ y ] jiajia so that now it seems to follow the rhythm pattern of English. 44

11 An areal phenomenon James Hoard has shown that careful enunciation of consonants is an areal phenomenon. Careful enunciation not only of consonants but also of glide transitions between vowels and consonants may also be an areal phenomenon. 12 Afterword In their 2008 editorial The phonetics of North American languages, McDonough and Whalen write, Given the fact that he best (i.e., most practical) orthographies gloss over phonetic detail, these records are usually of limited value for phonetic science. Even the best IPA transcriptions require a segmentation and linearization that is often at odds with the phonetic phenomena at hand. The big question is how do we annotate these languages so that they can be taught in a way that is true to our consultants and teachers who are no longer with us? How do we represent their pronunciation so that it can be emulated? Straight taxonomic phonemic notation has been useful for academic linguistic comparison, but it can be debated whether or not this notation is optimal for language preservation and revitalization among people who are not trained in linguistics. We have strayed far from the detail preserved in Sapir s transcriptions. References: Bird, Sonya. 2011. The nature of laryngealization in St at imcets laryngealized resonants. International Journal of American Linguistics 77:159 84. Blake, Susan. 1992. Two aspects of Sliammon phonology: glide/obstruent alternation and vowel length. M.A. thesis, University of British Columbia. Boas, Franz. [1887?]. [Thalholhtwh - English vocabulary]. Document 711 b. Smithsonian Anthropological Archives. Davis, John H. 1970. Some phonological rules in Mainland Comox. M.A. thesis, University of Victoria. Davis, John H. 2005. High consonants, articulatory transitions, and nonhigh vowels in Comox. Papers for ICSNL 40 (Brown, J., M. Kiyota, & T. Peterson, eds.) UBCWPL 16:89 97. Elías-Ulloa, José. 2009. The distribution of laryngeal segments in Capanahua. International Journal of American Linguistics 75:159 206. Gerfen, Chip, & Kirk Baker. 2004. The production and perception of laryngealized vowels in Coatzospan Mixtec. Journal of Phonetics 33: 311 314. Golston, Chris, & Wolfgang Kehrein. 1998. Mazatec onsets and nuclei. International Journal of American Linguistics 64:311 37. 45

Hoard, James E. 1978. Syllabification in Northwest Indian languages, with remarks on the nature of syllabic stops and affricates. In Syllables and Segments (Bell, Alan, & Joan Bybee Hooper, eds.) Amsterdam: North Holland (pp 59 72). Hoole, Phil. [n.d.] Laryngealization. (accessed May 21, 2015) www.phonetik.uni-muenchen.de/~hoole/kurse/artikul/laryngealization.pdf Ladefoged, Peter N. 1965. The nature of general phonetic theories. Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics. Washington: Georgetown University Press. 16:27 42. Macaulay, Monica, & Joseph Salmons. 1995. The phonology of glottalization in Mixtec. International Journal of American Linguistics 61:38 61. Maddieson, Ian, Heriberto Avelino, & Loretta O Connor. 2009. The phonetic structures of Oaxaca Chontal. International Journal of American Linguistics 75:69 101. McDonough, Joyce, & D.H. Whalen. 2008. The phonetics of native North American languages. Journal of Phonetics 36:423 6. Sapir, Edward. 1915. Noun Reduplication in Comox. Canada Department of Mines, Geological Survey, Memoir 63, No. 5, Anthropological Series. Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace. 46