Underlying Representations

Similar documents
Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Portuguese Vowel Harmony: A Comparative Analysis and the Superiority of Autosegmental Representations

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Basic concepts: words and morphemes. LING 481 Winter 2011

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Learning Disability Functional Capacity Evaluation. Dear Doctor,

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

Interfacing Phonology with LFG

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

INTRODUCTION TO MORPHOLOGY Mark C. Baker and Jonathan David Bobaljik. Rutgers and McGill. Draft 6 INFLECTION

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

Teacher: Mlle PERCHE Maeva High School: Lycée Charles Poncet, Cluses (74) Level: Seconde i.e year old students

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

MARK 12 Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Teaching Vocabulary Summary. Erin Cathey. Middle Tennessee State University

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Lower and Upper Secondary

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Primary English Curriculum Framework

18 The syntax phonology interface

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Henry Sweet Lecture Annual Meeting of the Linguistics Association of Great Britain (LAGB), September 16, 2015 University College, London

Problems of the Arabic OCR: New Attitudes

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

Monticello Community School District K 12th Grade. Spanish Standards and Benchmarks

1. Introduction. 2. The OMBI database editor

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Did they acquire? Or were they taught?

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

5/26/12. Adult L3 learners who are re- learning their L1: heritage speakers A growing trend in American colleges

MARK¹² Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

5. Margi (Chadic, Nigeria): H, L, R (Williams 1973, Hoffmann 1963)

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic

Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Transcription:

Underlying Representations The content of underlying representations. A basic issue regarding underlying forms is: what are they made of? We have so far treated them as segments represented as letters. Chapters 6 and 10 revisit representations, showing that segments are not atomic units but are defined in terms of a small set of phonetically defined universal features. Correspondingly, underlying forms are composed of sets of features, and not unanalyzable segments per se. Chapter 9 addresses the famous abstractness issue, concerning the extent to which underlying forms can differ from surface forms. We have partially addressed that issue here, showing that the underlying form is not necessarily a substring of any one word that contains the morpheme. Hence in Palauan, the two vowels of /daïob/ are never both present as full vowels in any single instantiation of the root; in Tonkawa, the root /picena/ is never directly seen in that form, since one of the vowels always deletes. The main question addressed in Chapter 9 is whether an underlying form may even contain segments which are never pronounced as such in the language. A further issue in phonological theory has been the question whether underlying representations must be in principle pronounceable. For instance, an underlying form of the type /sa5t%o / would be typically precluded as an underlying form, since the symbols [5 % ] cannot be pronounced, so it would be natural to assume that such elements cannot be part of a phonological representation. But it is often though not universally assumed that underlying representations can have so-called archiphonemes such as /B/, which conventionally represent a segment that is not definitely voiced as in /b/, and is also not definitely voiceless as in /p/. As such, an archiphoneme is also unpronounceable, though in a different way from supposed / /, in that it is almost pronounceable (whereas nobody has any clue what / / could sound like), and in practice /B/ becomes always becomes pronounceable by making another definite decision, namely applying some rule that tells you whether the segment is [p] or [b]. The difference between / / and /B/ is that the former has no pronounceable attributes, and the latter lacks only one that makes it pronounceable. Morphology and morphophonemics. We have seen that in the taxonomic tradition, phonological rules of the type discussed in this chapter were referred to as morphophonemic rules (a name still used occasionally to describe phonological processes). The implication is that there is a connection between morphology and such phonological processes. Allophonic rules do not neutralize distinctions between underlying segments, so the underlying form can be automatically inferred from the surface form per the condition of biuniqueness. But with the output of a neutralizing rule (such as those presented in this chapter) it is impossible to know which of two (or more) segments a surface segment X derives from, if you only look at the word itself, even if you know the system of rules: you can t know in Russian whether [porok] comes from /porog/ or /porok/. You must look at a different instantiation of that very segment, but appearing in some other context, in order to determine what the underlying segment is. Most often, the other context which provides the crucial evidence must be a word with a morphological relation to the word in question, such as an infinitive of a verb, compared to a 1sg present tense form (as was needed to distinguish /h/ and /p/ in Kikerewe, which neutralize to [p] after a nasal). For that

2 Underlying Representations reason, neutralizing phonological alternations are most often associated with different morphological forms of words: hence the term morphophonemics, which is the selection of a particular (taxonomic) phoneme in some context. In the taxonomic approach, morphophonemic rules are said to select allomorphs of a morpheme. Analogous to the phoneme being seen as the set of all phones that realise the phoneme, the morpheme is seen as being made up of all of the surface (phonemic) variants of the morpheme, thus the plural morpheme in English would be composed of {/s/, /z/, / z/} (there could be other things included in the morpheme, such as /n/ as in oxen ), and the plural morpheme S would have the allomorphs /s/, /z/, and / z/. There could also be auxiliary symbols used to represent processes. As an example of the latter kind of symbol, the morphophoneme ~ is used in Miller s (1965) grammar of Acoma to indicate that the preceding vowel is deleted; M indicates a labial nasal which is retained before a particular list of suffixes and is deleted elsewhere; B symbolizes a consonant that is realised as [b] before certain vowel-initial suffixes, and deletes otherwise. In the case of Tonkawa, the morpheme which might be symbolized as notoxo hoe is realised by the set of allomorphs {/ntox/, /ntoxo/, /notx/, /notxo/}, and the morpheme picena represents the set {/pcen/, /pcena/, /picn/, /picna/}. Morphophonemics would refer to some set of rules that select particular variants in some context, just as there are rules that select the variants of the English verb morpheme be, namely {/Ÿz/, /w z/, /wr/, / m/, /ar/}. The problem with the taxonomic view of morphophonemics and allomorphs is that it fails to distinguish between variations in form that are due to a phonological rule such as vowelharmonic suffix variants in Finnish or phonetically-driven changes in the plural suffix in English, versus suppletive alterations as in the verb be. Not all neutralizing phonological rules involve changes induced by varying the morphological context: the defining context for a rule might be surrounding words in a sentence. We saw this in Korean, with a rule nasalizing stops before a nasal consonant. This rule applies even at the sentence level, so the pronunciation of the word /pap/ rice varies between [pap] and [pam], depending on which word follows: the morphological form of the word /pap/ uninflected noun remains constant in [pam m k ra] eat rice and [pap twemnita] served rice, even though the pronunciation of the word changes. There is a rich literature addressing such neutralizing phonological rules at the sentence level: see for example Kisseberth & Abasheikh (1974), Clements (1978), Odden (1987). Most examples of sentence-level phonology involve just the consideration of a word an an immediately preceding or following word; however, Odden & Robert-Kohno (1999) and Odden (2000) discuss phrasal processes in Kikamba and Zinza which apply across unbounded sequences of words. See Inkelas & Zec (1995) and references therein for various theoretical perspectives on phrasal phonological rules. The relation of phonology to morphology. A different view of the relationship between phonology and morphology is presented in the theory of Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky 1982, Kaisse & Shaw 1985, Mohanan 1986). In the theory presented in this book which represents the standard theory up to the development of LP (and is still maintained by some), it is assumed that the underlying form contains all of the morphemes present in a word, that these morphemes are present at once, and they have whatever shape they are assigned in their dictionary entries. In the Lexical Phonology model, phonology and morphology are separate but bidirectionally interact-

Underlying Representations 3 ing components in a grammar. Rules of phonology and morphology are assigned to particular strata or levels in both phonology and morphology, which allows words to be built up in stages so that some phonological rules may apply early in a derivation, and they output of some phonological rules may then be followed by morphological operations of affixation. Certain morphological operations are defined for the first stratum of the morphological component Level 1 so in English, affixation of the derivational affixes -ion and -ity takes place at Level 1, and other operations such as affixation of -ness and -ing are defined at Level 2. Similarly, certain phonological rules are defined for Level 1 but not Level 2, for example stress assignment and Trisyllabic Shortening (which accounts for the vowel alternation in profane ~ profanity) which apply only when Level 1 affixes are added. Under this model, the output of Level 1 morphology undergoes the rules of Level 1 phonology. The result derived from Level 1 phonology is then resubmitted to the morphological component, and at Level 2 other affixes are added, with the resulting form again being run through the phonology. Thus the derivation of a word, in the view of Lexical Phonology, involves a shuttling back and forth between the morphological component and the phonolological component. Accordingly, each input to the phonology (at Level 1 vs. Level 2) would be the underlying form, at that level. Further consideration of Lexical Phonology will be given in Chapter 5, in connection with rule ordering and the cycle. Morpheme structure conditions and rules. One of the more resilient problems regarding underlying forms is that they seem to exhibit regular properties. For instance, there are no morphemes in English which begin with sequences such as *bn, *km, *gn; in Korean, no noun stems end with the consonants p, t, k, or, for that matter, t; Russian has no underlying consonants plain /c /, palatalized /z y /, or /å/. If all regularities about languages are to be stated in a grammar (a major assumption worth critical thought), how are these facts to be expressed? One answer, exemplified by Halle 1959, is that there are morpheme structure rules (MSR) in a grammar which perform specific repairs on any string not conforming to such rules. Thus there could be a rule of Russian that would converts any non-palatalized (phonetically nonexistent) /c / into palatalised [c y ], whioch would account for the fact that there are no nonpalatalized alveopalatal affricates in Russian. Paired with the assumption that there are such rules is that certain properties would be left blank underlyingly (resulting in archiphonemic representations such as /C / with a segment which is neither explicitly palatal nor explicitly non-palatal), so in Russian the palatalization feature would not be underlyingly specified for the alveopalatal affricate, and would be filled in by a rule. An alternative to such a rule is morpheme structure conditions (MSC) as proposed by Stanley 1967 which do not actively change underlying forms, but rather passively impose requirements on what is a well-formed morpheme. In the MSC approach, underlying representations will contain fully specified segments, thus the voiceless alveopalatal affricate of Russian is definitively palatalized, and is not left in a blank state for palatalization. MSCs do not state what would happen to any segment contradicting the requirements of the condition, but typically violations are considered to impose a cost on the grammar (see the commentary on Chapter 6, 8 for discussion of cost ). The major criticism of the MSC approach is to so-called duplication problem identified in Kenstowicz & Kisseberth 1977, that MSC s are often the same as active segment-changing

4 Underlying Representations rules motivated by the existence of paradigmatic alternations. For example in Kiruundi, all vowels within roots are underlyingly long when followed by a sequence of a nasal plus a consonant an example is [riind-a] watch, which can be explained by a MSC requiring vowels to be long before NC. There is also an active alternation in vowel length triggered by combination of underlying short vowels plus NC sequences, which explains the variation in the length of the infinitive prefix /ku-/ seen between [ku-ror-a] to look at vs. [kuu-n-dor-a] to look at me. The MSC on underlying length duplicates the phonological rule which must exist anyway. It does not solve the problem to derive morpheme-internal vowel length in [riind-a] by simply applying this rule to an underlying short vowel, assuming that all vowels are underlyingly short before tautomorphemic NC. Under the metatheoretical assumption that all regularities must be stated in the grammar, 1 this just replaces the duplication problem with a mirror-image duplication problem, that if there is a rule deriving X in a context, there must be a MSC ruling out X in that context. An alternative would be to say that the assignment of vowel length within morphemes before NC is random, so that the underlying form of [riind-a] might be /rind-a/ but the underlying forms or [baamb-a] might be /baamb-a/. No facts of the language give evidence for supposing /rind-a/ vs. /baamb-a/, so the question of how speakers could learn the specific underlying forms of roots would remain unanswered. One contemporary trend in phonology, Optimality Theory, theoretically precludes the possibility of a grammar with statements about underlying forms (since n that theory all grammatical statements apply to the output). This has resulted in the principle Richness of the Base (Prince & Smolensky 1993), variously understood as saying any imagineable input must map to some output or any string must map to a well-formed output, ideas which have in common the denial that a grammar states regularities about underlying forms. One significant shift of emphasis in that approach, discussed under the rubric Stampean occultation and lexicon optimization (see also Inkelas 1994), is that some observable regularities may not be part of a formal grammar. The lack of any underlying /u / in the lexicon of English words might thus not be accounted for in the formal theory of grammar, but emerges as a byproduct of language acquisition in a nutshell, there is no reason to assign /u / to the underlying form of any words of English. This seems to be a major break with previous tradition, since it rejects the idea that all generalizations about a language need to be explicitly encoded in a grammar. Another way to view ROTB is that it entails that a grammar must contain explicit devices (constraint rankings) which prohibit impossible outputs such as *[stiyïk] in English, no matter what the underlying form is. This question remains unresolved at present. Readings Clements, G. N., 1978, Tone and syntax in Ewe. in D. J. Napoli (ed.) Elements of tone, stress, and intonation, 21-99. Georgetown University Press: Washington) Halle, M. 1959. The sound pattern of Russian. Harris, Z. 1942. Morpheme alternants in linguistic analysis. Language 18:169-180. 1 A number of theoretical problems are rooted in this assumption.

Underlying Representations 5 Hockett, C. 1947. Problems of morphemic analysis. Language 23: 321-343. Inkelas, S. & D. Zec. 1995. Syntax-phonology interface. J. Goldsmith (ed.) Handbook of Phonological Theory, 535-549. Oxford: Blackwells. Kaisse, E. & P. Shaw. 1985. On the theory of lexical phonology. Phonology 2: 1-30. Kenstowicz, M. & C. Kisseberth. 1977. Topics in phonological theory. Academic Press: New York. Kiparsky, P. 1982. Lexical morphology and phonology. I.-S. Yang (ed) Linguistics in the morning calm, 3-91. Seoul: Hanshin. Kisseberth, C. & M. Abasheikh. 1974. Vowel length in Chi-Mwi:ni a case study of the role of grammar in phonology. A. Bruck, R. Fox & M. LaGaly (eds.), Papers from the parasession on natural phonology, 193-209. CLS. Mohanan, K. P. 1986. The theory of Lexical Phonology. Reidel: Dordrecht. Odden, D. 1987. Kimatuumbi phrasal phonology. Phonology 4, 13-36 Odden, D. & R. Roberts-Kohno. 1999. Constraints on Superlow tone in Kikamba. R. Kager & W. Zonneveld (eds), Prosodic and phrasal phonology, 145-170. Nijmegen University Press, Nijmegen (w. R. Roberts-Kohno). Odden, D. 2000. The phrasal tonology of Zinza. Journal of African languages and linguistics 21: 45-75. Prince, A. & P. Smolensky. 1993. Optimality theory: constrarint interaction in generative grammar. Report no. RuCCS-TR-2. Rutgers University: New Brunswick. Stanley, R. 1967. Redundancy rules in phonology. Language 43: 393-436.