ENCODING VARIABILITY AND DIFFERENTIAL NEGATIVE TRANSFER AND RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE IN CHILDREN THESIS. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

Similar documents
Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Retrieval in cued recall

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Encoding. Retrieval. Forgetting. Physiology of Memory. Systems and Types of Memory

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

AP PSYCHOLOGY VACATION WORK PACKET UNIT 7A: MEMORY

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

Science Fair Project Handbook

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

PROFESSIONAL TREATMENT OF TEACHERS AND STUDENT ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT. James B. Chapman. Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

Learning and Teaching

Thesis-Proposal Outline/Template

NCEO Technical Report 27

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

South Carolina English Language Arts

Reading Horizons. Organizing Reading Material into Thought Units to Enhance Comprehension. Kathleen C. Stevens APRIL 1983

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Transfer of Training

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

KENTUCKY FRAMEWORK FOR TEACHING

D Road Maps 6. A Guide to Learning System Dynamics. System Dynamics in Education Project

Assessment and Evaluation

Fearless Change -- Patterns for Introducing New Ideas

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

San José State University Department of Psychology PSYC , Human Learning, Spring 2017

Proposal of Pattern Recognition as a necessary and sufficient principle to Cognitive Science

Alpha provides an overall measure of the internal reliability of the test. The Coefficient Alphas for the STEP are:

ANGLAIS LANGUE SECONDE

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Urban Legends Three Week Unit 9th/10th Speech

A Process-Model Account of Task Interruption and Resumption: When Does Encoding of the Problem State Occur?

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Contents. Foreword... 5

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall

On-the-Fly Customization of Automated Essay Scoring

A cognitive perspective on pair programming

School Size and the Quality of Teaching and Learning

Proficiency Illusion

Films for ESOL training. Section 2 - Language Experience

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

The Round Earth Project. Collaborative VR for Elementary School Kids

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

CONTINUUM OF SPECIAL EDUCATION SERVICES FOR SCHOOL AGE STUDENTS

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

NORTH CAROLINA VIRTUAL PUBLIC SCHOOL IN WCPSS UPDATE FOR FALL 2007, SPRING 2008, AND SUMMER 2008

Unit 3. Design Activity. Overview. Purpose. Profile

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Critical Thinking in the Workplace. for City of Tallahassee Gabrielle K. Gabrielli, Ph.D.

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Evidence-Centered Design: The TOEIC Speaking and Writing Tests

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Unit 13 Assessment in Language Teaching. Welcome

Course Syllabus Advanced-Intermediate Grammar ESOL 0352

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Lecturing Module

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Lecture 10: Reinforcement Learning

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Case study Norway case 1

San Marino Unified School District Homework Policy

Success Factors for Creativity Workshops in RE

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Transcription:

ai IV,5O ENCODING VARIABILITY AND DIFFERENTIAL NEGATIVE TRANSFER AND RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE IN CHILDREN THESIS Presented to the Graduate Council of the North Texas State University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE By Frederick G. Fleming, B. A. Denton, Texas August, 1975

ABSTRACT Fleming, Frederick G., Encoding Variability and Differential Negative Transfer and Retroactive Interference in Children. Master of Science (Psychology), August, 1975, 40 pp., 1 illustration, bibliography, 37 titles. Second-graders were tested for negative transfer and retroactive interference using an A-B, A-D paradigm. Four-pair, word-number lists were aurally presented to the children. Subjects were classified as being whole-only encoders or multiple encoders by the use of a recall test presented after list one. Significant negative transfer and retroactive interference were found. The multiple encoders experiences less difficulty in learning the second list that did the whole-only encoders, but these two groups did no differ with regard to transfer or retroactive interference effects. The results were considered in the context of Martin's encoding variability hypothesis.

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure Page 1. Proposed relationship between developmental learning strategy and negative transfer...-.-...5 iii

ENCODING VARIABILITY AND DIFFERENTIAL NEGATIVE TRANSFER AND RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE IN CHILDREN Paired-Associate Learning in Children Keppel (1964) addressed the question of whether "the laws of verbal learning obtained on the college student will hold reasonably well for younger populations of Ss" (p. 63). This is a worthwhile question because through the years it has been assumed that the verbal learning results obtained with college underclassmen could be generalized to the rest of the population. This has been a convenient assumption since college students have been readily available in the academic community to serve as subjects. However, this assumption would have to be modified if it were found that significant groups of the population, e.g., children, have different laws of verbal learning. In the area of paired-associate learning in children, Keppel's review of the extant literature does not undermine the assumption that college students and children are probably using similar techniques to learn. However, Keppel notes that the research on children is sparse, and the question is not completely settled. A more recent experimental study (Loomis and Hall, 1968) has shown that 5 year old children may be learning differently than 7 year old children. Using an A-B, A-C negative transfer paradigm, the researchers found negative 1

2 transfer for their 7 year old subjects but not for their 5 year old subjects. "It would appear that in verbal learning... there is an age difference in performance between 5 and 7 year olds" (p. 186). Loomis and Hall had children memorize short lists of word pairs such as CUP-FISH. Then they taught the children a "conflicting" list with items such as CUP-BOOK. By "conflicting," I mean that the second list was designed to interfere with memory for the first list. Ease of learning verbal material is thought to be related to the meaningfulness (M) of that material (Butler and Merikle, 1970; Martin, 1968). Yet Keppel (1964) predicted that variations in M should have little effect on learning in children. He considered M to be a function of the strength of the language habits of the subject. He thought variations in M would affect children very little as compared with college students. However, Loomis and Hall (1968) found a contrary interaction between meaningfulness and age in their sample. The 5 year olds were more affected by change in M as defined by Di Vesta's (1965) scale of entropy than were the 7 year olds. Other studies have found learning difference between children of different ages. Shapiro (1966) noted differences between fifth and eighth graders for type of stimulus modality. The younger children performed better on aurally

3 presented than visually presented stimuli, while the older children showed no difference for type of modality. Koppenaal, Krull and Katz (1964) found that 4 and 5 year olds show less retroactive and proactive interference than do 8 year olds. However, Palermo (1961) and McCullers (1961) failed to find differences between fourth and sixth graders in a paired-associate task. I believe that Martin's variable stimulus encoding theory (1972) can be applied to the Loomis and Hall (1968) findings of developmental difference in negative transfer. Perhaps the younger children are more able to encode differently the identical stimuli encountered in the second list in a manner unavailable to the older children. Although both groups of children are receiving the same A-B, A-C paradigm, perhaps the 5 year olds, by changing their encodings on the second list, are able to change the task into "a non-specific transfer paradigm (A-B, C-D)" (Ellis, 1973, p. 173). If a child encodes words as unit concepts, then he should be learning a relationship between two words, e.g., CUP and FISH, in one of only a few possible ways. If, on the other hand, he selectively encodes aspects or parts of the stimulus word in memory, there are numerous ways in which he might be learning the CUP-FISH pair. For example, the child may be encoding the first letter of the stimulus. Or he may encode in terms of the last letter of the stimulus. In the former instance, CUP-FISH

4 would be encoded as C-FISH in memory. In the latter it would be encoded as P-FISH. However, one must look at this example with some reservation. Martin (1971) argues that subjects encode verbal material in other ways in addition to letters. This ability to selectively encode certain aspects of a stimulus can be looked at in two ways. One, the subject may be using a rudimentary form of learning where only the parts of a stimulus are encoded. A second possibility is a more advanced form where the subject encodes both the unit stimulus and its constituent parts. In both cases the subject has many ways to encode the nominal stimulus while the unit learner has only a few. As Martin (1971) notes, associative interference theory argues that in order to learn a new relationship, CUP-BOOK, that conflicts with an old relationship, CUP- FISH, one must, to a certain extent, "forget" the old relationship. "A learner cannot retain both A-B and C-D where A and C are identically encoded" (Martin, 1972, p. 71). In this discussion, this principle should hold more for unit learners than for part learners. Part learners should be able to circumvent conflicting pairedassociates. By "part" learners, I mean both part-only and multiple encoders. It is possible, then, that the results of Loomis and Hall (1968), and Koppenaal, et. al (1964) can be explained by saying that older children are more likely to respond

5 to words as units than are younger children. When presented with a list-two item, CUP-BOOK, that conflicts with a list-one item, CUP-FISH, a child who responds to words only as units should experience greater negative transfer from his list one associations, since he has only a few possible encoding strategies. A child who responds to words only in parts, or who can make multiple encodings of those words, might select new encodings during list-..two learning, and thus avoid negative transfer. I am proposing here a curvilinear type of relationship between age and negative transfer in the 5 to 8 year old age range. In Figure 1 below, the idea is expressed that the younger age children would be encoding stimuli in terms of their parts only. As the child develops language skills, he may at first encode in terms of units only. Then as he grows older, the child may once again increase his number of possible encodings. Amount of Negative Transfer part unit multiple only only encoding Encoding Strategy Figure 1. A proposed relationship between developmental learning strategy and negative transfer.

6 What could cause this switch from perceiving stimuli in terms of parts to perceiving stimuli in terms of units? I believe it is caused by the development of the language skills of the children to a level at which the sound of the word will evoke a unit mental picture of that word. "The person can apparently substitute a mental image for a word having the same referent. One might say that the image codes the word" (Bower, 1972, p. 88). Younger children may have less of this ability and be more likely to respond to words in terms of their composite sounds only. Meaningfulness is related to this ability to form mental images of words. As was said above, high-m stimuli should be easier to learn. Loomis and Hall (1968) and Koppenaal et al. (1964) found that older children learn faster than younger children. This leads to the possible conclusion that the stimuli were more meaningful to the older children. It might be a possible conclusion from this line of reasoning that the older children were forming more unit mental images. I used the Loomis and Hall (1968) study as a general model for my experiment. However, I altered it in certain aspects to suit the needs of testing the theories stated above. (1) I changed the lists from word-word to wordnumber. This was designed to limit the use of mnemonic devices which could be used to encode word pairs. I was

careful in the construction of my lists to avoid such obvious 7 mnemonic pairs as LUCK-7 and CAT-9. I was careful to avoid having the word and the number begin with the same phoneme. (2) Each list, which consisted of four word-number pairs, had within it 12 stimulus phonemes. The phonemes in each stimulus corresponded to a CVC trigram, e.g., (sed). This made an encoding strategy using single phonemes easy and practicable. (3) All stimuli were substantives drawn from the CA 7 list of Di Vesta (1965). They were selected for high-h (entropy), high meaningfulness. It was hoped that this method would aid the children in learning the list. The two lists were equated for average H value. (4) Unfortunately, during a pilot study it was found that the kindergarten subjects had extreme difficulty in learning the lists. Even when the lists were reconstructed, this difficulty persisted. It was feared that making the lists too easy would mask individual differences among the second graders. Therefore, only second graders were tested. A recall test was administered after list one learning to determine the encoding strategy being used by the children. Despite the narrow age range used, it was hoped that children with all three different learning strategies could be found. (5) The criterion for classifying children on the recall tests was as follows: Children who were able to

8 give a correct response to the first phoneme of the stimulus better than 75% of the time, but who also failed to give a correct response to the whole stimulus better than 50% of the time, were classified as "part-only" learners. Children who failed to give a correct response to the first phoneme of the stimulus better than 50% of the time were classified as "whole-only" learners. Children who gave a correct response to both the first phoneme and the whole stimulus better than 75% of the time were classified as "multiple encoders." The Encoding Problem in Verbal Learning Perhaps the best place to begin an analysis of the encoding problem in verbal learning is to look at Martin's (1971) Psychological Review article. In it, he examines the theoretical history in this area from 1894 to the present. Two theories in the field have long-standing reputations. These are associative interference theory and list differentiation theory. Associative interference theory has clear similarities with the Pavlovian-Thorndikian tradition. Martin (1971) describes the tenets of this theory: A-B learning produces an A-B association; A-C learning produces, concomitantly, an A-C association and a weakening of the original A-B association; as time elapses, the A-B association recovers some of its strength; upon presentation of stimulus A, the responses B and C compete for emission in accordance with their relative associative strengths. (p. 316)

9 The principles of classical conditioning can clearly be seen in an associative interference theory. Acquisition, extinction and spontaneous recovery are all included in this theory, which has made it attractive to researchers. This theory predicts a dependency relationship between recall of responses B and C. The strength of the A-B association depends upon the relative strength of the A-C association and vice versa. However, the dependency relationhip between B and C does not exist. Martin (1971) notes that F. J. DaPolito discovered that in an A-B, A-C paradigm, the probability of recalling a C response to an A stimulus was independent of the probability of recalling a B response to the same A stimulus. The discovery of this "independent retrieval phenomenon" supplied evidence counter to associative interference theory. A second theory of long-standing repute is list differentiation theory. This view holds that subjects tend to organize responses in memory in terms of lists, using a notion of permissible response set (Martin, 1971). That is, when presented with an A-C list, subjects have the B responses available, but fail to give them because they sense them as being "inappropriate." However, when subjects are tested on free recall (a test procedure where the A stimuli are not presented) of responses B and C, they fail to strongly organize

10 responses on the basis of list set (Martin and Mackay, 1970). Martin and Mackay found that subjects were more likely to group their responses in terms of common stimuli. A common stimulus was a stronger response organizer than a common list even at 30-min delay. Martin (1971), in summarizing the failures of the associative interference and list differentiation theories, noted that (1) stimulus organization of responses dominates list organization, (2) retroaction and proaction are still viable concepts and (3) there is no interference between A-B and A-C associations. These are the somewhat surprising facts that a theory of verbal paired-associate learning must explain. To do so, Martin developed the variable stimulus encoding theory, which is best described in Martin (1972). Before going further, an explanation of terminology is in order. The concept of stimulus encoding in verbal learning is not by any means new. It was used almost twenty years ago by Miller (1956) in his paper on the capacity for information processing. Bower (1967) describes encoding in this manner: It seems reasonable to tie the memory trace of an event to the variables operating in the perception of that event. Within this context, the major assumption seems innocuous; it is supposed that the person does not store the literal input stimulus, but rather some encoded representation of it. (p. 232)

11 From the above quote we can arrive at operational definitions of nominal and functional stimuli. By nominal stimulus is meant "the literal input stimulus" and by functional stimulus is meant an "encoded representation" of the nominal stimulus. The functional stimulus is the work-horse of memory. Martin adds to Bower's idea the hypothesis that the functional stimulus attended to by the subject changes from trial to trial, i.e., "encoding variability." Variable stimulus encoding theory (Martin, 1972) holds that human subjects often encode nominal stimuli in memory in a piecemeal manner. It was developed from a number of different traditions in learning theory. Three of the most important are those of Estes (1959), Lawrence (1963) and Underwood (1963). W. K. Estes proposed that learning is a probabilistic event. A given stimulus is seen as consisting of a number of elements. A certain probability exists that the organism will connect these stimulus elements to response classes in an all-or-none fashion on any given trial. D. H. Lawrence proposed a coding response that acts on sensory input (the stimulus) to produce a stimulus-ascoded, or s-a-c. This s-a-c differs from the sensory input. "It is the s-a-c that is directly associated with, and elicits, the overt behavior" (p. 189). B. J. Underwood postulated that subjects make use of only fractions of the stimulus compound as learning cues.

12 The subject economizes and only uses "the minimally necessary differentiating component for the functional stimulus" (p. 47). By combining these three approaches, it can be seen that an organism selectively samples part of the stimulus and codes this stimulus part by connecting it with a response. At a later time presentation of the s-a-c or functional stimulus alone should be sufficient to elicit the response. Through a process of selective attention (Trabasso and Bower, 1968), subjects will choose particular aspects of a nominal stimulus in a learning task. The subject uses this selective encoding process "to facilitate the link between a nominal stimulus and an overt response" (Ellis, 1973, p. 124). James and Greeno (1967), using word-nonsense syllable paired-associates with college students, found that their data were "generally consistent with the hypothesis that Ss actually select among stimulus aspects until the list is mastered, and then relax the selection of attention during overtraining" (p. 75). This theory contrasts with the older associative interference theory which says that in forming an A-C association the previous A-B association must be weakened or "unlearned" (Martin, 1971; Wichawut and Martin, 1971). On the contrary, variable stimulus encoding theory would

say that A-B and A-C are different tasks due to the selective 13 variable encoding of A. Associative interference theory fails to analyze the stimulus. It assumes that the stimulus is always a perceptual unit. Variable stimulus encoding theory differs from list differentiation theory in that the former states that responses are organized in terms of functional stimuli and not in terms of response list sets. Response B is paired with functional stimulus Al, and response C is paired with functional stimulus A2. List differentiation theory explains retroaction and proaction in terms of perceived appropriateness, but variable stimulus encoding theory explains these effects by the subject's persistence in maintaining their list one or list two encodings. It is generally accepted that the important construct underlying encoding variability is that of stimulus meaningfulness (M) (Butler and Meriklte 1970; Feuge and Ellis, 1969). Ellis (1973) and Underwood (1963) hold that the probability of stimulus component selection is inversely related to meaningfulness. Subjects have a small number of possible alternative encodings to high-m nominal stimuli and a large number of possible alternative encodings to low-m nominal stimuli. Encoding variability can be scaled in other ways beside meaningfulness. Butler and Merikle (1970) obtained four other measures of encoding variability. The product-

14 moment correlations were computed for each of these measures with meaningfulness (M), as defined by Archer (1960), from the data given by Butler and Merikle. These measures are: Associative Reliability (AR), r =.35; Number of Different Associations (NDA), r = -.44; Probability of Prime Response (PPR), r =.33; and Associative Uncertainity (UN), r = -.06. AR and PPR are directly related to M, while NDA and UN are inversely related to it. Granted that a subject selectively encodes a pairedassociate list and that the number of possible encodings is determined by the meaningfulness of the stimulus items, what happens when this theory is applied to an area of theoretical interest such as retroactive interference or (RI). Although RI is an old concept, most of the work in stimulus encoding studies looks at negative transfer or what happens in second-list learning. For example, Loomis and Hall (1968) found differences across age groups, but they did not examine the RI question. A test for RI has been added in this study. Martin (1973) accounts for RI by saying that "at the end of A-C learning the original A,B code has a very low probability of being sampled," (p. 496). Amount of RI is a function of the similarity of the A-B and A-C encodings.

15 Schneider and Houston's (1968) study leads to the prediction that part learners face less RI than unit learners: Assume different responses are learned in two successive trigram-adjective lists.... If S selects a component of the second-list stimulus which is identical to the {functional} stimulus utilized in the learning of the first list... then the situation corresponds to the A-B, A-C paradigm. Considerable unlearning and competition are expected to occur in this situation. On the other hand, if S utilizes a component of the second list stimulus which is different from that used in the first-list learning... then the situation corresponds to the A-B, C-D paradigm and much less RI is expected. Partial attention to both components might result in intermediate amounts of RI. (p. 166) Certain studies have questioned the validity of variable stimulus encoding theory. In the Goggin and Martin (1970), and Williams and Underwood (1970) experiments, subjects who shifted their functional encodings showed more negative transfer than did those subjects who maintained their original encodings. Richardson and Stanton (1972) report a failure of subjects to change their functional encodings when faced with a negative transfer situation. These results are contrary to the predictions of variable stimulus encoding theory. Martin (1971) defends his theory by saying that researchers may be confusing "obvious, external, experimenterdefined stimulus attributes with the learner's subtle, internal, subjectively defined functional encodings" (p. 330). Williams and Underwood assumed that subjects would only encode in terms of letter position. Variable stimulus

16 encoding theory does not make such an assumption. Even in a simple situation, a subject may make numerous different functional encodings. Martin goes on to say that although Williams and Underwood (1970) did not find shifts in stimulus encoding, Schneider and Houston (1968) did. Merryman and Merryman (1971) also defend Martin's theory. They argue that the similarity of the Goggin and Martin (1970) study to a concept identification task explains why subjects failed to shift encodings on the second task, "it is not surprising that their Ss failed to shift to the previously irrelevant dimension.... the connection between Goggin and Martin's experiment and the VSE {variable stimulus encoding} theory of PA {paired-associate} learning seems very tenuous" (p. 684). Similarly, Merryman and Merryman (1971) criticize the "use of backward recall tests" to measure "stimulus selection" in the Williams and Underwood (1970) study. The Merrymans found support for variable stimulus encoding theory by using "a forward association test for stimulus selection" (p. 684). Polzella and Martin (1973) describe the current position of variable stimulus encoding theory with regard to this controversy. Variable stimulus encoding theory would say that in an A-B, A-C paradigm subjects are reluctant to give up their successful first list encodings on list two. This is the explanation of proaction. After

17 finally switching to successful second list encodings, subjects are reluctant return to their old list one encodings. This is the explanation of retroaction. However, unless subjects are flexible enough to switch encodings, they will experience negative transfer and retroactive interference. If they do switch encodings, then their performance becomes comparable to subjects in an A-B, C-D control paradigm. Rudy (1974) also has raised objections to variable stimulus encoding theory, but from a different approach than those critics just mentioned. He disagrees with the selective attention approach of Martin and others, (e.g., Lashley, 1942; Lawrence, 1963). Rudy sees that the stimulus selection data are caused by variations in the associative process rather than by a selective attention mechanism. The crucial assumption underlying Rudy's theory "is that the associative process is variable and engaged only to the extent that the learner experiences an unpredictable event" (p. 282). In an A-B, A-C negative transfer paradigm, the associative process mechanism is brought into play whenever the subject experiences interference. This associative process mechanism links the stimulus component cues to the correct responses on each learning trial when interference is encountered (Rudy, 1974). Thus, the learning process only occurs after the subject makes an error. om - 3tmajlll -1-1 1 Il 11 I'll

18 Rudy gets around having to resort to a selective attention process by postulating "saliency parameters" (p. 288) for component cue classes be they color, shape, letter, or phoneme position, etc. Thus, he removes the cause of which cue gets associated to which response from the organism, and makes it an inherent part of the stimulus. Granting that this is a possibility, Rudy fails to give any orders of magnitude for these saliency parameters. An estimation of the saliency parameters of component cue classes commonly used in paired-associate learning studies would be helpful in testing his theory. Until these parameters are specified, it will be difficult to test his notion of saliency parameters. Rudy's theory and Martin's theory have certain similarities. Both agree that the functional stimulus is paired with the response. Both theories see learning as a two-stage process, an encoding stage and an associative stage. They disagree over how these two stages operate: In Martin's view the major theoretical process for association is the encoding process; since the associative process is viewed as invariant, attending or encoding the stimulus is equivalent to associating it with the response. In contrast, although the VAP {variations in the associative process} view implicitly acknowledges that attending to a stimulus is a necessary condition for rendering it available for association with some other event, it denies that attending to a stimulus is a sufficient condition for it to become associated with another event. The associative process must also be activated, and it should be clear that the associative process is assumed to be subject to variation and is not automatic. (Rudy, 1974, p. 294)

19 Rudy admits that the literature does not allow us to choose between his theory and Martin's. It is hoped that the associative strength of certain encodings can be predicted and tested. Rudy (1974) also predicts that a subject will not change stimulus cues unless a change is made in the nominal stimulus. A review of the theoretical issues with regard to the encoding problem was made. The two old theories, associative interference theory and list differentiation theory were described and their weaknesses examined. An explanation of Martin's variable stimulus encoding theory was presented. The relationship between stimulus encoding and meaningfulness was furnished. Variable stimulus encoding theory was extended to cover an area of theoretical interest, retroactive interference. Prediction and experimental results were examined. The recent theoretical evolution of variable stimulus encoding theory was examined as it weathered a storm of conflicting research data. Rudy's (1974) variations in the associative process theory was also examined and comparisons were made with Martin's theory. In conclusion, a scientific revolution as described by Kuhn (1970) is occurring in verbal learning. The old theories are dying out, and a number of new theories are competing for acceptance.

Method Subjects The subjects were 54 boys and girls, (23 boys and 31 girls), enrolled in the second grade at Frank Borman elementary school in Denton Texas during the spring of 1975. Each subject was assigned to one of four treatment groups on the basis of a predetermined random order of conditions. Fourteen subjects were unable to learn the first list to a criterion of two consecutive perfect repetitions by 24 trials and were discarded from the experiment. Thus, data were gathered on 40 subjects. This group of 40 subjects included 18 boys and 22 girls. There were 10 subjects in each treatment group. At the time of individual testing, the subjects ranged from 7 years, 4 months, 0 days, to 9 years, 3 months, 25 days (Mean = 8 years, 0 months, 18 days). Design The design (see Appendix A) included a "recall test" (see Appendices B and C) which was inserted between list one and list two to determine the type of encoding strategy whole-only, part-only, or multiple encoding, the subjects were using on list one. The use of such a test was noted in Postman and Greenbloom (1967).and Wichawut and Martin (1970). 20

21 Materials The stimulus words were selected from the Di Vesta (1965) list of 100 substantives on the basis of H or entropy values. Eight words were selected from the CA 7 list, which each consisted of three phonemes and were high in H-value ("A" words, mean H-rank = 13.5; "D" words, mean H-rank = 14.25). It was thought that these words would be easiest for the children to master. The lists were randomly constructed with the restriction that no two words in a list would have the same initial phoneme. Also the restriction was imposed that no word-number pair was allowed to have the same initial phoneme. The responses (see Appendix D) were the numbers one through eight, randomly paired with the stimulus words except for the restrictions noted above. The lists were recorded on a standard tape recorder (3M Wollensak) in six randomly determined orders to avoid serial learning effects. I recorded all of the lists at a standard recording level using a memory drum from which to read the lists. Procedure Each subject was run by me in either the counselor's office, the nurse's room, or the band practice room at Frank Borman school. There was little distracting noise from outside the experimental rooms. Standard instructions, adapted from McCullers (1963) were read to all subjects. Prior to the experimental task,

22 all subjects were presented with a sample two-pair list (see Appendix D) to assure understanding of the instructions. During the experimental task, the stimulus was presented for a 3-sec interval and then the stimulus and response were read without pausing during the second 3-sec interval. The lists were four pairs long and the intertrial interval was 6-sec. All of the subjects repeated the first list until two consecutive correct trials were performed with the restriction that the perfect recitation had to occur on or before trial 24 or the subject was discarded from the experiment. The subjects were then given the recall test. Then the subjects received a second list. After reaching the criterion of two consecutive perfect repetitions on list two, the subjects relearned the first list. learned to a criterion of one perfect repetition. This was Testing one subject normally took 20 to 30-min. Results A 2 (experimental, A-B, A-C paradigm vs. control, D-B, A-C paradigm) x 2 (whole-only vs. multiple encoding learning strategy) analysis of variance was computed for six dependent variables. These were: trials to criterion on list one, total errors on list one, trials to criterion on list two, total errors on list two, trials to criterion on the reacquisition of list one, and total errors on the reacquisition of list one.

23 Recall Test From the recall test scores, I was able to classify all 40 subjects as to type of learning strategy employed. I classified 24 subjects as multiple encoders, i.e., encoding the stimulus as a unit and also in terms of initial phonemes. I similarly classified 16 of the children as whole-only encoders, i.e., encoding the stimuli only as units. I was not able to classify any of the children as being initial phoneme only or part-only encoders. First List Learning On the acquisition of list one, neither of the main effects nor the interactions were significant for either of the two dependent variables. Second List Learning Using trials to criterion on list two as the dependent variable, a significant effect due to the transfer paradigm was found, F(1,34) = 8.82, p <.01. The A-B, A-C subjects required more trials to criterion than did the D-B, A-C subjects. The mean number of trials to criterion was 11.17 and 7.20 for the A-B, A-C and D-B, A-C groups, respectively. A significant strategy effect was also found, F(1,34) = 11.59, p <.01. The whole-only encoders required more trials to second list criterion than did the multiple encoders. The mean number of trials to criterion was 11.75 and 7.14 for the whole-only encoding and multiple encoding groups, respectively. No significant interactions were found.

24, Using total errors on list two as the dependent variable, a significant transfer effect was again found, F(1,34) = 5.32, p <.05. The mean number of total errors to criterion was 19.56 and 12.90 for the A-B, A-C and D-B, A-C groups, respectively. A significant strategy effect was also again found, F(1,34) = 9.00, p <.01. The mean number of total errors to criterion was 21.13 and 12.36 for the whole-only and multiple encoding groups, respectively. The interaction between these two groups was not significant. First List Reacquisition For trials to criterion, the retroactive interference effect was significant, F(1,30) = 16.47, p <.01. The mean number of trials to criterion was 3.73 and 1.89 for the A-B, A-C and D-B, A-C groups, respectively. The A-B, A-C group experienced more retroactive interference than did the D-B, A-C group. Neither the strategy effect nor the interaction was significant. For total errors to criterion, the retroactive interference effect was again significant, F(1,30) = 13.44, p <.01. The mean number of total errors to criterion was 4.07 and 1.11 for the A-B, A-C and D-B, A-C groups, respectively. Neither the strategy effect nor the interaction was significant. A listing of the means and standard deviations of these results is in Appendix E.

Discussion Due to the unforeseen difficulties encountered with kindergarten children in running this study, an adequate test of Martin's variable stimulus encoding theory (Martin, 1972) with regard to learning in children was not possible. However, this study did show some important results with second graders. A significant negative transfer effect was found with second graders which was a replication of the Loomis and Hall (1968) finding. This study also showed that second graders do experience retroactive interference with verbal material. This finding is a replication of the Koppenaal et al. study. There are few studies in the literature dealing with paired-associate transfer and retroaction in children of this age, (see Koppenaal et al., 1964; Loomis and Hall, 1968; Shapiro, 1966). Findings of negative transfer and retroactive interference in young children should lend support to Keppel's (1964) idea that the laws of learning should be generalizable across age groups. The finding in this study that those children who are able to encode a stimulus in multiple ways to better on second list learning than children who are only able to encode a stimulus as a unit is important. As I see it, there are two possible ways to interpret this finding. One way is that those children who were able to multiple encode the 25 - -, IWVAMWAVAWAWAkMlWWl*- * - 4-, --

26 stimulus were the superior learners. As such, the greater ease which they had in learning list two was due to their inherent superiority. A second interpretation of these results would be to say that the multiple encoders had numerous alternative strategies and thus could switch functional stimuli when they experienced difficulty learning. More data need to be collected before a definite interpretation can be advanced. Originally, when this study was designed, it was hoped that some of the children could be classified as using a part-only strategy. That is, it was hoped that some of the children would be learning the stimuli in terms of their individual sounds only, and not in terms of units. None of the second graders could be so classified. It may be that kindergarten age children would be using such a strategy. Perhaps with more difficult materials, partonly encoding could be observed in second graders. Since no significant interactions were found between the transfer paradigm and the strategy the subjects utilized, a test of the curvilinear relationship I proposed above was not possible. To support my hypothesis, children need to be classified into all three types of learning strategies, and an interaction between strategy and the transfer paradigm should be demonstrated. Although the children classified as multiple encoders had less difficulty in learning list two than did the

27 children classified as whole-only encoders, these two groups did not differ significantly with regard to RI. As was noted above, I predicted that the multiple encoders would also experience less RI than the whole-only encoders on the basis of the Schneider and Houston (1968) study. Still, the dependent variables are in the right direction. If more subjects could have been tested, or if the nature of the RI task had been different, then perhaps a significant result could have been found. The restricted ranges on the dependent variables for the reacquisition of list one probably masked some difference. on trials to criterion was one to seven. The range For total errors the range was zero to ten. The greatest difficulty I had in running this study was to find materials that would be easy enough for 5 year olds to master, but would be difficult enough that 8 year olds would find them challenging. I was surprised, when using similar materials to those of Loomis and Hall (1968) to find that my 5 year olds had difficulty where theirs did not. Perhaps my subjects had difficulty because I required a more stringent criterion than Loomis and Hall did. Also, my response materials were more abstract than theirs, numbers rather than concrete nouns. It might be helpful if the lists were shortened from four wordnumber pairs to three. Perhaps greater success could be had from a visual presentation.

28 It was necessary to keep the children interested in the task for the entire 20 to 30-min session. Some data had to be discarded because the child's interest in the task appeared to wane. Yet, a number of children were fascinated by the task and were disappointed when it was finished. I found that the tape recorder itself helped to stimulate the child's interest.

APPENDIX A Design Group I lst List 2nd List 3rd List Experimental A-B Recall Test Number One A-C A-B Control D-B Recall Test Number Two A-C D-B Gtoup II lst List 2nd List 3rd List Experimental D-B Recall Test Number Two D-C D-B Control A-B Recall Test Number One D-C A-B Note. The design has been counterbalanced across Group I and Group II with the "A" and "D" stimulus sets to control for differences in stimulus materials. 29

APPENDIX B Recall Test Number One (Sample) Stimulus Read C 0 G BOOK CAT 7 A 1 ROPE 0 5 T L LOG 4 P K 00 B R to Subject Subject's Response 30

APPENDIX C Recall Test Number Two (Sample) Stimulus Read MOON S D 5 00 CUP P E A 1 U LAKE SEED K N 7 C M L 4 to Subject Subject's Response I 31

APPENDIX D Paired Associate Lists Sample List Dog - 9 Cow - 10 List A-B List D-B Cat - 1 Book - 4 Log - 5 Rope - 7 Lake - 1 Seed - 4 Cup - 5 Moon - 7 List A-C List D-C Book - 2 Cat - 3 Log - 6 Rope - 8 Seed - 2 Lake - 3 Cup - 6 Moon - 8 32

APPENDIX E Means and Standard Deviations by Cells I. Trials to Criterion, List One Whole-only Multiple A-B, A-C 11.56 (6.41) 9.55 (4.63) D-B, A-C 12.71 (5.88) 11.31 (5.45) II. Total Errors, List One A-B, A-C Whole-only 22.22 (13.53) Multiple 15.91 (8.96) D-B, A-C 23.86 (11.80) 21.31 (10.93) III. Trials to Criterion, List Two A-B, A-C Whole-only 13.89 (6.74) D-B, A-C 9.00 (4.12) IV. Total Errors, List Two Whole-only Multiple A-B, A-C 25.78 (14.21) 13.33 (7.60) D-B, A-C 15.14 (6.59) 11.70 (5.28) 33

APPENDIX E--Continued Means and Standard Deviations by Cells V. Trials to Criterion, RI Test Whole-only Multiple A-B, A-C 3.88 (2.04) 3.63 (1.06) D-B, A-C 2.33 (1.51) 1.69 (0.75) VI. Total Errors, RI Test Whole-only Multiple A-B, A-C 4.57 (3.91) 3.63 (2.13) D-B, A-C 1.83 (2.40) 0.77 (0.93) Note. The standard deviations are in parentheses below the corresponding means. 34

REFERENCES Archer, E. J. A re-evaluation of the meaningfulness of all possible CVC trigrams. Psychological Monographs, 1960, 74 (10, Whole No. 497). Bower, G. H. A multicomponent theory of the memory trace. In K. W. Spence and J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press, 1967. Bower, G. H. Stimulus sampling theory of encoding variability. In A. W. Melton and E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. New York: Winston and Wiley, 1972. Butler, B. E., and Merikle, P. M. Uncertainty and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 634-641. Di Vesta, F. J. Developmental patterns in the use of modifiers as modes of conceptualization. Child Development, 1965, 36, 185-213. Ellis, H. C. Stimulus encoding processes in human learning and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 7). New York: Academic Press, 1973. Estes, W. K. The statistical approach to learning theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959. Feuge, R. L., and Ellis, H. C. Generalization gradients in recognition memory of visual form: The role of stimulus meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79, 288-294. Goggin, J., and Martin, E. Forced stimulus encoding and retroactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 84, 131-136. James, C. T., and Greeno, J. G. Stimulus selection at different stages of paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 74, 75-83. 35

Keppel, G. Verbal learning in children. Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 61, 63-80. 36 Koppenaal, R. J., Krull, A., and Katz, H. Age interference, and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1964, 1, 360-375. Kuhn, T. S. The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970. Lashley, K. S. An examination of the "continuity theory" as applied to discriminative learning. Journal of General Psychology, 1942, 26, 241-265. Lawrence, D. H. The nature of a stimulus: Some relations between learning and perception. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 5). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963. Loomis, D. J., and Hall, V. C. The effects of age and overlearning in an A-B, A-C design using high- and low-h value. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968, 7, 182-188. McCuller, J. C. Effects of associative strength, grade level, and interpair interval in verbal paired-associate learning. Child Development, 1961, 32, 773-778. Martin, E. Stimulus meaningfulness and paired-associate transfer: An encoding variability hypothesis. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 421-441. Martin, E. Verbal learning and independent retrieval phenomena. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 314-332. Martin, E. Stimulus encoding in learning and transfer. In A. W. Melton and E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. New York: Winston and Wiley, 1972. Martin, E. Memory codes and negative transfer. Memory and Cognition, 1973, 1, 495-498. Martin, E., and Mackay, S. A. A test of the list-differentiation hypothesis. American Journal of Psychology, 1970, 83, 311-321. Merryman, C. T., and Merryman, S. S. Stimulus encoding in the A-Br', AX-B and A-Br', AX-B paradigms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10, 681-685. -- 4., -, -.- I -. I 11, -'- I - - -4& 'Mmol"A

37 Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 81-97. Palermo, D. S. Backward associations in the paired associate learning of fourth and sixth grade children. Psychological Reports, 1961, 9, 227-233. Polzella, D. J., and Martin, E. Stimulus encoding in A-B, A-D transfer. American Journal of Psychology, 1973, 86, 589-600. Postman, L., and Greenbloom, R. Conditions of cue selection in the acquisition of paired-associate lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1967, 73, 91-100. Richardson, J., and Stanton, S. K. Some effects of learning to a set of components on stimulus selection. American Journal of Psychology, 1972, 85, 519-534. Rudy, J. W. Stimulus selection in animal conditioning and paired-associate learning: Variations in the associative process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1974, 13, 282-296. Schneider, N. G., and Houston, J. P. Stimulus selection and retroactive inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 77, 166-167. Shapiro, S. S. Aural paired associate learning in grade school children. Child Development, 1966, 37, 417-424. Trabasso, T., and Bower, G. H. New York: Wiley, 1968. Attention in learning. Underwood, B. J. Stimulus selection in verbal learning. In C. N. Cofer and B. J. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning: Problems and processes. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963. Wichawut, C., and Martin, E. Selective stimulus encoding and over-learning in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 85, 383-388. Wichawut, C., and Martin, E. Independence of A-B and A-C associations in retroaction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10, 316-321. Williams, R. G., and Underwood, B. J. tests of the Martin hypothesis. Psychology, 1970, 86, 317-324. Encoding variability Journal of Experimental