On the Notion Determiner

Similar documents
Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Words come in categories

The building blocks of HPSG grammars. Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) HPSG grammars from a linguistic perspective

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Control and Boundedness

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Writing a composition

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Structure-Preserving Extraction without Traces

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Indeterminacy by Underspecification Mary Dalrymple (Oxford), Tracy Holloway King (PARC) and Louisa Sadler (Essex) (9) was: ( case) = nom ( case) = acc

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

2014 Colleen Elizabeth Fitzgerald

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

Feature-Based Grammar

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Basic Parsing with Context-Free Grammars. Some slides adapted from Julia Hirschberg and Dan Jurafsky 1

Syntactic types of Russian expressive suffixes

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Som and Optimality Theory

Update on Soar-based language processing

LFG Semantics via Constraints

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

lgarfield Public Schools Italian One 5 Credits Course Description

15 The syntax of overmarking and kes in child Korean

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Switched Control and other 'uncontrolled' cases of obligatory control

Implementing the Syntax of Japanese Numeral Classifiers

Word Formation is Syntactic: Raising in Nominalizations

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

ON THE SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

INTRODUCTION TO MORPHOLOGY Mark C. Baker and Jonathan David Bobaljik. Rutgers and McGill. Draft 6 INFLECTION

A Usage-Based Approach to Recursion in Sentence Processing

Interactive Corpus Annotation of Anaphor Using NLP Algorithms

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

11/29/2010. Statistical Parsing. Statistical Parsing. Simple PCFG for ATIS English. Syntactic Disambiguation

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Collocations of Nouns: How to Present Verb-noun Collocations in a Monolingual Dictionary

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Advanced Grammar in Use

1 Nonapriorism vs. apriorism

Construction Grammar. Laura A. Michaelis.

The Role of the Head in the Interpretation of English Deverbal Compounds

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

The Interface between Phrasal and Functional Constraints

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Specifying a shallow grammatical for parsing purposes

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Transcription:

On the Notion Determiner Frank Van Eynde University of Leuven Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar Michigan State University Stefan Müller (Editor) 2003 CSLI Publications pages 391 396 http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/hpsg/2003 Van Eynde, Frank. (2003). On the Notion Determiner. In Stefan Müller (Ed.): Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, Michigan State University (pp. 391 396). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

Abstract Following a common practice in generative grammar, HPSG treats the determiners as members of a separate functional part of speech (DET), just like the complementizers, the coordinating conjunctions, and (in some frameworks) the auxiliaries. The status of such functional parts of speech is a matter of debate and controversy. The auxiliaries, for instance, are commonly treated as members of a separate category (AUX or INFL) in many variants of generative grammar, including GB, MP and LFG, but in GPSG and HPSG, it is a matter of equally common practice to treat them as members of V and to reject the postulation of a separate functional category, see (Pullum and Wilson 1977) and (Gazdar, Pullum and Sag 1982). This text makes a similar case for the determiners; more specifically, I will argue that they are categorially heterogeneous, in the sense that some determiners are members of A, whereas others are members of N. The argumentation is mainly based on inflectional morphology and on morpho-syntactic agreement data. The consequences of the categorial heterogeneity are hard to reconcile with the specifier treatment of the determiners of (Pollard and Sag 1994), and even more with the Detas-head treatment of (Netter 1994), but it can smoothly be integrated in the functor treatment of the prenominals of (Allegranza 1998) and (Van Eynde 2003b). 1 The categorial heterogeneity of the determiners Adopting the classical X-bar distinction between specifiers and adjuncts, as proposed in (Chomsky 1970), (Pollard and Sag 1994) treats the determiners as specifiers and the other prenominal dependents as adjuncts. In his many beautiful pictures, for instance, the possessive is a specifier, whereas many and beautiful are adjuncts. Adjuncts are optional and can be stacked; specifiers, by contrast, are sometimes obligatory, as in the case of singular count nouns in English, and cannot be stacked, as in *the his pictures. Moreover, adjuncts are projections of substantive categories (N,V,A,P), whereas specifiers are projections of functional categories, such as DET. This systematic correlation between syntactic function (specifier of NP) and part of speech (determiner) is unfortunate and had better be removed, both for methodological and empirical reasons. Methodologically, it goes against the grain of cross-categorial generalization which is typical of X-bar syntax and of the HPSG framework. A complement or a head, for instance, can belong to any kind of category; so why should a specifier be a priori restricted to belong to one particular part of speech (Det)? Empirically, there is ample evidence from various languages that the set of words which are standardly treated as determiners is a rather heterogeneous collection which comprises both signs with adjectival properties and signs with nominal properties. Some of this evidence will be presented in this section. 392

[SG,MASC] [SG,FEM] [PL,MASC] [PL,FEM] Adj alto alta alti alte high facile facile facili facili easy Dem questo questa questi queste this Wh quale quale quali quali which Table 1: The forms of the Italian prenominals [ AGR, DCL] [ AGR,+DCL] [+AGR] Adj goed goede goeden goeder goeds good koel koele koelen cool Poss ons onze onzen onzer onzes our Dem deze dezer this Wh welk welke which Table 2: The forms of the Dutch prenominals 1.1 Adjectival determiners In languages in which the prenominal adjectives show inflectional variation, one commonly finds the same variation in the case of the determiners. In Italian, for instance, the demonstratives show the same variation with respect to gender and number as the prenominal adjectives which end in -o; similarly, the wh-determiner quale which shows the same variation as the adjectives which end in -e, see table 1. The same holds for the Dutch determiners. Their variation in terms of agreement (AGR) and declension (DCL) mirrors the one of the prenominal adjectives, see table 2. 1 This similarity in inflectional variation is significant, since it is one of the main criteria for motivating part of speech membership: a word like operation, for instance, is treated as a noun, since it inflects like a noun, and the fact that its meaning is closely related to the one of a verb, does not matter in this respect. In keeping with this practice, I will assume that the determiners in tables 1 and 2 are members of A. Further evidence for this assumption is provided by the fact that the determiners are subject to the same agreement constraints as the prenominal adjectives. The Italian prenominals with the -a suffix, for instance, only combine with singular feminine nouns, both when they are adjectives and when they are determiners. In Dutch, the agreement facts are more complex than in Italian, but they confirm the observation that the determiners are subject to the same constraints as the prenominal adjectives, see (Van Eynde 2003a). 1 The forms with an AGR affix are either genitive or dative. They are not commonly used and therefore absent in many paradigms, but notice that such gaps occur both among the determiners and the prenominal adjectives. 393

1.2 (Pro)nominal determiners The specifiers of NPs can also be genitives of proper nouns and pronouns. In Dutch, they are in complementary distribution with the possessive adjectives. Compare, for instance, onze kat our cat with Peters/wiens kat Pete s/whose cat. In terms of morphology and agreement, though, the genitives do not behave as adjectives. 2 They do not take any of the typically adjectival affixes, such as the declension affix, 3 and they do not show any agreement with the head noun. Compare, for instance, the agreement in case, number and gender between the possessive and the head noun in mijns/*mijn inziens my-gen insight-gen with the lack of agreement between the genitive NPs and the head noun in Peters/wiens boeken Pete s/whose books, in which the prenominal is a singular masculine genitive, whereas the head noun is a plural neuter noun in standard case. This lack of agreement can also be observed in the combination of a noun with a prenominal adjunct of the category common noun, as in aluminium tubes, in which the singular mass noun aluminium does not show agreement with the plural count noun tubes. Another class of NP specifiers with nominal characteristics are the non-genitive pronouns. As an example, let us take the Italian interrogative che what ; this pronoun is not only used as an argument of the verb, as in che dici? what say-you, but also as a prenominal, as in che/quali intenzioni hai? what/which intentions have-you. In this use, it has the same meaning and function as quale which, but in contrast to the latter it does not show any adjectival morphology or agreement. A similar example is the Dutch quantifying wat some(thing) ; it can be used as the argument of a verb, as in er is nog wat over there is still some left, but also as a prenominal, as in er zijn nog wat erwten there are still some peas. In that use, the singular wat does not show any agreement with the head noun erwten, which demonstrates that it behaves like a (pro)noun rather than like an adjectival determiner. Summing up, the specifiers of NP do not belong to a separate part of speech, but are either adjectives or nouns. In the former case they show the same inflectional variation and the same agreement as the prenominal adjectives, in the latter, they do not show any agreement. 2 Accommodating the categorial heterogeneity The conclusion of the previous section is a problem for the treatment of the determiners as specifiers in (Pollard and Sag 1994), for if determiners belong to either A or N, then there is no categorial basis anymore for the distinction between specifiers and adjuncts. Further complications arise when one adopts the assumption, also made in (Pollard and Sag 1994), that the nouns lexically select their specifier, for in that case the value of the selecting feature (SPR) will be A N, so 2 In contrast to the English possessive s, which can be argued to be a word which takes an NP as its specifier, as in (Pollard and Sag 1994), the Dutch -s is a genitive affix. 3 The affix in the pronoun ikke I-EMP is not a marker of declension, but of emphasis. 394

that the addition of an adjectival or nominal adjunct will inadvertently trigger the cancellation of the noun s SPR requirement. The conclusion is even more problematic for the DetP style analysis in (Netter 1994). Netter treats the determiners as heads which take a nominal projection as their complement and in order to get a uniform result for nominals with and without determiner claims that the determiner inherits the category of its complement, i.e. N. As a result, it cannot accommodate the fact that most of the determiners are adjectival. Moreover, since the determiners also inherit the HEAD AGR value of their nominal complement, which includes case, number and gender, it erroneously predicts that genitive NPs have to show agreement with the head noun. A treatment which is compatible with the findings of the previous section is the one of (Allegranza 1998) and (Van Eynde 2003b). They treat the determiners as functors which select a nominal projection as their head and which contribute their MARKING value to the combination. SYNSEM LOC CAT MARKING marked HEAD-DTR SYNSEM synsem hd-func-phr NONHEAD-DTRS SYNSEM LOC CAT HEAD SELECT MARKING This phrase type models all combinations in which the non-head daughter selects the head daughter, and hence subsumes the head-adjunct, head-specifier and head-marker phrase types of (Pollard and Sag 1994). The differences between determiners and prenominal adjectives are captured in terms of the MARKING value. They both select an unmarked nominal, but while the MARKING value of the determiners is marked, the one of the adjectives is unmarked. This accounts for the fact that adjectives can be stacked, whereas the determiners cannot, as well as for the fact that the determiners must precede the adjectives. 4 N[marked] A[marked] N[unmarked] his A[unmarked] large N[unmarked] head This treatment has no problem with the categorial heterogeneity of the determiners, for since the determiner status is captured in the MARKING value and since the HEAD value of the determiner is not shared with the NP, one gets a uniform NP 4 The distinction captured by the MARKING value is not a semantic one. The possessives, for instance, are marked in Dutch and English, but not in Italian, cf. il suo cane the his dog. Similarly, while the English quantifying each is marked, its near-synonym every is not, cf. his every move and where a film s every truckling nuance is debated (TIME, January 13, 2003, 50). 395

analysis, both when the determiner is an adjective and when it is a (pro)noun. At the same time, the part of speech distinction provides the means to differentiate the agreeing adjectival determiners from the non-agreeing nominal ones. 3 Conclusion Determiners do not belong to a separate functional category, but are categorially heterogeneous: some are adjectives, others (pro)nouns. This is a problem for the specifier treatment of (Pollard and Sag 1994) and for the head treatment of (Netter 1994), but not for the functor treatment of (Allegranza 1998) and (Van Eynde 2003b). The latter s emphasis of the different roles of HEAD and MARKING values allows for a cleaner distinction between form and function. References Allegranza, V. (1998). Determiners as functors: NP structure in Italian, in S. Balari and L. Dini (eds), Romance in HPSG, CSLI Publications, Stanford, pp. 55 107. Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on nominalization, in R. Jacobs and P. Rosenbaum (eds), Readings in English transformational grammar, Ginn and Company, Waltham, Mass., pp. 184 221. Gazdar, G., Pullum, G. and Sag, I. (1982). Auxiliaries and related phenomena in a restrictive theory of grammar, Language 58: 591 638. Netter, K. (1994). Towards a theory of functional heads: German nominal phrases, in J. Nerbonne, K. Netter and C. Pollard (eds), German in HPSG, CSLI Publications, Stanford, pp. 297 340. Pollard, C. and Sag, I. (1994). Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar, CSLI Publications and University of Chicago Press, Stanford/Chicago. Pullum, G. and Wilson, D. (1977). Autonomous syntax and the analysis of auxiliaries, Language 53: 741 788. Van Eynde, F. (2003a). Morpho-syntactic agreement and index agreement in Dutch NPs, in T. Gaustad (ed.), Computational Linguistics in the Netherlands 2002. Selected Papers from the Thirteenth CLIN Meeting, Rodopi, Amsterdam - New York, pp. 111 127. Van Eynde, F. (2003b). Prenominals in Dutch, in J.-B. Kim and S. Wechsler (eds), Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on HPSG, CSLI Publications, Stanford University, pp. 333 356. 396