Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Similar documents
A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

Writing a composition

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

ACADEMIC AFFAIRS GUIDELINES

Control and Boundedness

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Number of students enrolled in the program in Fall, 2011: 20. Faculty member completing template: Molly Dugan (Date: 1/26/2012)

PH.D. IN COMPUTER SCIENCE PROGRAM (POST M.S.)

Sources of difficulties in cross-cultural communication and ELT: The case of the long-distance but in Chinese discourse

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Myths, Legends, Fairytales and Novels (Writing a Letter)

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

LQVSumm: A Corpus of Linguistic Quality Violations in Multi-Document Summarization

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

Compositional Semantics

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Intermediate Academic Writing

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Nancy Hennessy M.Ed. 1

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Twenty-One Suggestions for Writing Good Scientific Papers. Michal Delong and Ken Lertzman. 1. Know your audience and write for that specific audience.

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Syntactic and Lexical Simplification: The Impact on EFL Listening Comprehension at Low and High Language Proficiency Levels

Mercer County Schools

Heritage Korean Stage 6 Syllabus Preliminary and HSC Courses

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Developing Grammar in Context

Creating Travel Advice

Vocabulary Usage and Intelligibility in Learner Language

DISTRICT ASSESSMENT, EVALUATION & REPORTING GUIDELINES AND PROCEDURES

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Effects of Open-Set and Closed-Set Task Demands on Spoken Word Recognition

This publication is also available for download at

SINGLE DOCUMENT AUTOMATIC TEXT SUMMARIZATION USING TERM FREQUENCY-INVERSE DOCUMENT FREQUENCY (TF-IDF)

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

University-Based Induction in Low-Performing Schools: Outcomes for North Carolina New Teacher Support Program Participants in

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Unit 7 Data analysis and design

On the Notion Determiner

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

First Grade Standards

MASN: 1 How would you define pragmatics today? How is it different from traditional Greek rhetorics? What are its basic tenets?

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Words come in categories

Evaluation of Hybrid Online Instruction in Sport Management

WiggleWorks Software Manual PDF0049 (PDF) Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company

Merbouh Zouaoui. Melouk Mohamed. Journal of Educational and Social Research MCSER Publishing, Rome-Italy. 1. Introduction

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Transcription:

Tyler Perrachione LING 451-0 Proseminar in Sound Structure Prof. A. Bradlow 17 March 2006 Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Abstract Although the acoustic and articulatory correlates of clear speech have been extensively studied, the syntactic, pragmatic, and lexical factors underlying native speaker to non-native speaker communication are less well understood. A case study is described where one individual performed the same communicative task with either a native or non-native listener. Lexicon size and distribution, as well as extent of adjectival modification are examined between the two conditions to determine the degree of audience design at these levels of speech production. Introduction The study of clear speech examines the sort of acoustic and articulatory changes that speakers make when they are in a situation where they believe they may be poorly understood. These sorts of changes are compared against how speakers behave normally, when comprehension can be taken for granted (see Uchanski 2005 for a review). The phenomenon of clear speech can be framed within the domain of audience design. Audience design is the notion that individuals adjust the ways they speak in order to target a particular listener or group of listeners. The study of audience design generally contrasts such situations as intimate versus formal conversation, jargon versus simplicity, lecture versus conversation, etc. The relationship between clear speech and audience design is straightforward enough to assume they are part of the same, larger, communicative phenomenon. Both clear speech and audience design can be viewed within the framework set forth by Grice (1975) for analyzing discourse. When speakers make their contributions as meaningful and relevant as possible, communication is most facilitated.

Perrachione 2 Although the domain of clear speech tends to encompass interactions between speakers of high and low proficiency, especially between native speakers and non-native listeners, the vast majority of clear speech study has investigated only the acoustic and articulatory correlates of adapting to less-proficient listeners. In these situations, the entire realm discourse features usually studied under the domain of audience design, such as word choice, lexicon complexity, syntactic complexity, etc, is less well understood. This present investigation is a first step towards a study of intra-talker variability in audience design when addressing native and nonnative speakers of English. This investigation focuses primarily on the size and makeup of the discourse lexicon, as well as the degree and extent of adjective modification. Methods This study makes use of the newly christened Wildcat Corpus of Accented English from Northwestern University (Bradlow et al. 2006). Two conversations were taken from the database for analysis. Each conversation takes place over the course of a map task, in which one of a pair of participants attempts to arrange items on a map (the Receiver) based on instructions given by the other (the Giver). In one conversation, a native English-speaking undergraduate student (21 years old from Wisconsin) gives instructions to another native English-speaking undergraduate student (19 years old from New York). In the second conversation, the same native English-speaker as before gives instructions to an English-L2, native Korean graduate student listener. The two conversations analyzed in this present study were specifically selected because the same native English-speaking individual was an interlocutor in both. Having the same speaker involved allows for a stronger standard of comparison between the two conditions: talking with a native listener and talking with a non-native listener.

Perrachione 3 The task was performed in a sound-attenuated chamber, and each speaker was digitally recorded on a unique microphone, with the two streams combined into one stereo file, sampled at 22.05 khz. After recording, the conversations were orthographically transcribed for assessment (see Appendix A Conversations). The lexicon of the Giver within the discourse was analyzed by counting the total number of words uttered across the course of the task. Hesitations, false-starts, and non-words were not included in the analysis. Words that were cliticized, contracted, or otherwise inflected were considered only one word. This value was the number of unique tokens in the lexicon. Once the total tokens were established, any duplicate tokens were removed to determine the total number of types of words used by the Giver. Words that were inflected (e.g. do / does, runs / running) or contracted (it / its, we / we re) were considered repeat instances of the same token. Special consideration was not made for semantic referent, and words such as block that may have referred to two unique referents (a cube or a length of street) were collapsed into one token. After the overall lexicon was established, it was subdivided into the distribution of content words. Content words were nouns, adjectives, verbs, or adverbs that contributed to identifying specific referents in the discourse. Pronouns, complementizers, determiners, prepositions, and discourse markers were excluded from the domain of content words. Once the total number of tokens of content words was established, content words were assessed for type in the same fashion as the entire lexicon. Finally, the frequency and familiarity of the words in each subset of the lexicon were measured using the Hoosier Mental Lexicon (Nusbaum, Pisoni & Davis 1984). The conversations were also analyzed for distribution of adjectival modification. A count was made of the number and degree of adjunct adjectival modification of nouns or pronouns.

Perrachione 4 This included adjectives that directly modified a noun or pronoun (e.g. the other house with the green roof or no the red one but the purple one. ), but not those that modified a noun as a predicate (e.g. the roof that is green, or it s wooden. ) Results Lexicon Size and Distribution Overall, the Giver (N3) had a larger lexicon when talking to the non-native speaker (K3) than when talking to the native speaker (N2). However, the relative number of types and tokens did not differ significantly across the two lexicons [χ 2 (1,1479) = 2.38, n.s.], nor did the relative number of types and tokens of content words differ [χ 2 (1,688) = 0.08, n.s.] Likewise, the distribution of content word tokens relative to total word tokens did not differ across the two conditions [χ 2 (1,322) = 0.08, n.s.]. The difference in distributions of content words to overall words uttered approached significance [χ 2 (1,1157) = 3.25, p < 0.10] across two conditions, with a slightly larger ratio of content words to function words in that native condition. The size and distribution of words in the Giver s lexicon are summarized in Table 1, below. Table 1 Giver s Lexicon All Lexicon Content Words Language Task Tokens Types Tokens Types N3-N2 Map 2 Giver 448 140 199 91 N3-K3 Map 1 Giver 709 182 277 121 The frequency and familiarity of the words in the two lexicons were also systematically assessed via the Hoosier Mental Lexicon (Nusbaum, Pisoni & Clark 1984). The log frequency scores for all words uttered (types) did not differ between the two conditions (ttest, 2-tailed, p = 0.34) (see Figure 1), nor did the familiarity scores differ across all types (t-test, 2-tailed, p = 0.60). Likewise, the log frequency scores or familiarity scores for content words

Perrachione 5 alone did not differ across the two conditions (t-test, 2-tailed, p = 0.24 and p = 0.71, respectively) (see Figure 2). Log Lexical Frequency (All) 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 1 2 3 4 5 Figure 1 Histogram of Log Lexical Frequency of All Types Shaded bars indicate native to native condition, empty bars are native to non-native condition. Log Lexical Frequency (Content) 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 1 2 3 4 Figure 2 Histogram of Log Lexical Frequency of Content Word Types Shaded bars indicate native to native condition, empty bars are native to non-native condition.

Perrachione 6 Adjective Modification The degree and types of adjective modification were also systematically assessed. The results of the adjunct adjective measurements are given in Table 2, below. Table 2 Degree of Adjunct Adjective Modification Adjective Modifiers Language Task 0 1 2 3 N2-N3 Map 2 Giver 38 20 8 2 N3-K3 Map 1 Giver 65 33 5 2 Although the Giver used many more words (modified or unmodified) when talking to the non-native, there was not a significant difference in the distribution of adjectival modifiers across the two conditions [χ 2 (1,173) = 3.19, n.s.]. Discussion With regards to size and quality of the lexicon and degree of adjective modification, it appears that the difference between communicative styles used by the present talker when talking to native speakers or non-native speakers did not differ substantially. The most prominent difference between the two conditions was simply the amount of speech produced by the Giver, with much more being said to the non-native speaker than the native one. A qualitative reading of the transcripts reveals that this difference might be motivated by greater repetition and efforts and grounding reference by the Giver in the non-native condition than in the native condition. That is, the Giver appears to make additional references to locations on the map and in space in the non-native condition, perhaps in order to ensure that the location conveyed by his message was getting across. This includes greater references to both locations pre-drawn on the map (e.g. Hude Street) and objects that had been placed by the Receiver earlier in the conversation. Additionally, part of the greater quantity of speech produced in the non-

Perrachione 7 native condition could be driven by meta-discourse between the Giver and Receiver when confusion occurred. Likewise, in the non-native condition, the Giver summarized the overall map at the end to make sure they had set everything correctly, an undertaking he did not attempt in the native condition. Also driving the larger amount of speech produced by the Giver in the non-native task was the fact that he had undertaken this condition first. This experience could have facilitated the efficiency with which he completed the task the second time, when a native listener was present. Regardless of the quantity of speech produced, it is interesting that the quality of speech from a lexical standpoint did not differ between the two conditions. That is, the Giver produced lexicons with similar frequencies and familiarities, as well as degrees of adjectival modification, in both conditions. The Giver, then, did not select words that he thought would be easier for the Receiver to understand when the receiver was a non-native-speaker of English, nor did he engage in more complex discourse when speaking with a native speaker. Although examinations of the histograms in Figures 1 & 2 suggest a trend toward using higher-frequency words in the non-native listener condition, this trend was not statistically significant. This could be motivated by a number of factors, including the speaker s familiarity with talking to non-native listeners, the high degree of fluency of the non-native listener, or the priming experience from having completed the non-native task first when talking to the native speaker. Additionally, the apparent lack of difference in lexical frequencies could be a factor of second-language education, in which high-frequency, high-familiarity words for native-speakers are not necessarily taught first. (For example, many non-native speakers say clever, which is relatively low frequency in English, and, anecdotally, other high-familiarity words that are learned early in one s native language, e.g. barn and other farmyard terms, are learned late, if at all, in a non-native language).

Perrachione 8 Although the present case study failed to show tailoring for audience design in the speech of a native speaker when addressing a native or non-native speaker, it does not preclude this being a possibility. Other forms of complexity remain unanalyzed, including other sentential modifiers such as adverbs and relative clauses. Qualitatively, a reading of the transcript suggests that substantially more prepositional phrases, either of locative or compositional, were used to identify objects in the non-native condition of the map task. Future studies, including studies with larger samples of the corpus, are necessary to determine there is no natural difference in audience design when native speakers are addressing non-native speakers. Task order, including especially intertask priming effects, and participant fluency need to be systematically controlled for. It may be such that, in natural speech, individuals do not tailor their discourse to the lexical or syntactic demands of their audience, although they do make other articulatory and phonetic alterations. In such a case, two additional conditions would need to be considered: one in which individuals were directed to speak in a clear and straightforward manner to facilitate the comprehension of their non-native interlocutor, and another in which they were allowed to speak naturally without investigator-suggested bias. References Bradlow et al. (2006) Wildcat Corpus of Accented English. Database in preparation, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois. Grice, H. P., (1975), "Logic and Conversation", in P. Cole and J. Morgan, eds., Syntax and Semantics, vol. 3, Academic Press, pp. 41-58 Nusbaum, H.C., Pisoni, D.B., & Davis, C.K. (1984). Sizing up the Hoosier Mental Lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on Speech Perception Progress Report No. 10, 357-376. Uchanski, R.M. (2005) Clear Speech in D.B. Pisoni & R.E. Remez, The Handbook of Speech Perception. Blackwell: Malden, MA.