Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13,

Similar documents
Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Discussion Data reported here confirm and extend the findings of Antonucci (2009) which provided preliminary evidence that SFA treatment can result

Index. Language Test (ANELT), 29, 235 auditory comprehension, 4,58, 100 Blissymbolics, 305

Presentation Summary. Methods. Qualitative Approach

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

MD, USA Published online: 03 Jan 2014.

Adults with traumatic brain injury (TBI) often have word retrieval problems (Barrow, et al., 2003; 2006; King, et al., 2006a; 2006b; Levin et al.

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

A Corpus of Dutch Aphasic Speech: Sketching the Design and Performing a Pilot Study. E. N. Westerhout November 10, 2005

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Pediatr Rehabil Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 August 25.

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

Clinical Review Criteria Related to Speech Therapy 1

Accelerated Learning Course Outline

A STUDY ON THE IMPACT OF ORTON-GILLINGHAM APPROACH ON SOLVING THE WRITING DISORDER OF PRIMARY SCHOOL DYSLEXIC CHILDREN AT COIMBATORE DISTRICT.

Accelerated Learning Online. Course Outline

Unit 9. Teacher Guide. k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z. Kindergarten Core Knowledge Language Arts New York Edition Skills Strand

Development of an Impairment-Based Individualized Treatment Workflow Using an ipad-based Software Platform

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

The Bruins I.C.E. School

Large Kindergarten Centers Icons

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SAN MARCOS SCHOOL OF EDUCATION

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

The Complete Brain Exercise Book: Train Your Brain - Improve Memory, Language, Motor Skills And More By Fraser Smith

V Congress of Russian Psychological Society. Alexander I. Statnikov*, Tatiana V. Akhutina

Multi-sensory Language Teaching. Seamless Intervention with Quality First Teaching for Phonics, Reading and Spelling

Get Your Hands On These Multisensory Reading Strategies

Running Head: PASS theory of intelligence in Greek 1. PASS theory of intelligence in Greek: A review

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

THE USE OF TINTED LENSES AND COLORED OVERLAYS FOR THE TREATMENT OF DYSLEXIA AND OTHER RELATED READING AND LEARNING DISORDERS

Course Law Enforcement II. Unit I Careers in Law Enforcement

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

PART 1. A. Safer Keyboarding Introduction. B. Fifteen Principles of Safer Keyboarding Instruction

Speech/Language Pathology Plan of Treatment

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

The Cambridge Cookie-Theft Corpus: A Corpus of Directed and Spontaneous Speech of Brain-Damaged Patients and Healthy Individuals

ASSISTIVE COMMUNICATION

Phonological encoding in speech production

Considerations for Aligning Early Grades Curriculum with the Common Core

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTHERN MISSISSIPPI Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences SHS 726 Auditory Processing Disorders Spring 2016

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Norms For Boston Naming Test And Token Test Neuro Cog

Films for ESOL training. Section 2 - Language Experience

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Special Education Services Program/Service Descriptions

Tip-of-the-tongue states as metacognition

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Exploring Dyslexics Phonological Deficit I: Lexical vs Sub-lexical and Input vs Output Processes

Supported Reading Comprehension for People with Aphasia: Visual and Linguistic Supports

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

All Kinds of Minds. Web-site: To Contact NY Student Success Center. or

COMMUNICATION DISORDERS. Speech Production Process

Reading Horizons. A Look At Linguistic Readers. Nicholas P. Criscuolo APRIL Volume 10, Issue Article 5

Special Educational Needs and Disabilities Policy Taverham and Drayton Cluster

Occupational Therapy Guidelines

BIOH : Principles of Medical Physiology

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Cortical Visual Impairment in Children: Presentation Intervention, and Prognosis in Educational Settings

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

Dr. Shaheen Pasha Division of Education University of Education, Lahore

Writing quality predicts Chinese learning

Effective Practice Briefings: Robert Sylwester 03 Page 1 of 12

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

Dyslexia/LD Attention Deficit Disorders

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

Electronic Edition. *Good for one electronic/printed copy. Do not distribute.

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Developmental coordination disorder DCD. Overview. Gross & fine motor skill. Elisabeth Hill The importance of motor development

Broca s Area (BA) and Language Articulation: Evidence from Arab Broca s Aphasic

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

MFL SPECIFICATION FOR JUNIOR CYCLE SHORT COURSE

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Intervening to alleviate word-finding difficulties in children: case series data and a computational modelling foundation

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

Transcription:

Pure alexia is a well-documented syndrome characterized by impaired reading in the context of relatively intact spelling, resulting from lesions of the left temporo-occipital region (Coltheart, 1998). It is considered a disconnection syndrome in that visual information about written words does not reach the cortical areas critical for word recognition (i.e., the orthographic lexicon). The relative preservation of orthographic knowledge is confirmed in individuals with pure alexia by the fact that they can spell and they can also recognize orally spelled words with little difficulty. To compensate for their deficit, pure alexics often use a letter-by-letter (LBL) strategy, where reading is achieved by sequentially identifying the component letters of a word. In some cases, however, letter identification is impaired and a LBL reading approach is obviated. The inability to name letters may reflect a visual perceptual deficit, or a disconnection of visual-verbal information, where the letters are correctly perceived but fail to access letter names. The treatment of pure alexia characterized by poor letter-naming is particularly challenging. Several investigators have used a motor/kinesthetic approach to improve letter identification in such individuals (see Table 1 for details; Greenwald & Gonzalez- Rothi, 1998; Lott & Friedman, 1999; Lott et al., 1994; Maher et al., 1998). The rationale for this method is that the orthographic lexicon is accessed through the tactile/kinesthetic modality, circumventing the impaired access through the visual modality. Once letter identification has reached an acceptable level, training is geared toward improving speed and accuracy of reading. In the present study, we report on an individual with severe alexia who appeared to be similar to the cases described in Table 1, and thus, an appropriate candidate for the motor/kinesthetic approach to improve letter identification and single word reading. Method Patient Description TS was a 74-year old, right-handed female with 12 years of education, who was 15 months post onset of a temporo-parieto-occipital stroke at the time of this study. CT scan showed a lesion affecting the inferior and lateral aspects of BA 20/21 and 37, including the visual word form area. Superiorly, the lesion extended into the angular gyrus (BA 39) and dorso-medial occipital cortex (BA 18, 19). Extensive damage to interhemispheric callosal fibers traveling in the forceps major was also evident (Figure 1). The stroke resulted in right homonymous hemianopia, anomia, and alexia with agraphia. Her spoken language profile was consistent with anomic aphasia, with an Aphasia Quotient of 77 on the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesz, 1982). Pre-treatment Assessment TS reading and spelling performance was assessed using a list of 40 regular and 40 irregular words (balanced for frequency, length and imageability), and 20 pronounceable non-words. She demonstrated significantly impaired reading (12% correct overall) relative to recognition of oral spelling (65% correct overall). Her reading was characterized by attempts to read letter-by-letter, but with frequent errors in letter identification. Writing words to dictation was better preserved than reading, but was also impaired (76% correct overall). Her spelling of irregular words was more impaired than

spelling of regular words and nonwords, a profile consistent with lexical agraphia. It was noteworthy, however, that TS was able to write words to dictation with much greater accuracy than she was able to copy printed words (see Figure 2). Additional tests of visual/orthographic knowledge and peripheral writing abilities showed marked deficits in copying single words, case conversion (e.g., writing uppercase letter in response to visual presentation of lowercase), and naming visually-presented letters (see Table 2). In contrast, TS was able to correctly perform visual processing tasks including the identification of mirror-reversed letters, and matching of upper-to-lowercase letters, and vice versa. She was also able to correctly write all letters of the alphabet to dictation, write the letters associated with spoken phonemes (18/20), and could write CVC nonwords like meb with few errors (57/60 sound-letter correspondences correct). Written word-to-picture matching was surprisingly good (38/40) considering her profound impairment in oral reading of single words. Thus, performance was near normal on purely visual tasks, but impaired on tasks that required links between visual input and spoken or written output modalities. Treatment Treatment was aimed at improving reading of single words using a motor/kinesthetic approach to facilitate the recognition of component letters. TS was trained to copy individual words, attempting to name each letter aloud as she wrote it. Correct sequential writing and naming of the letters was expected to result in correct word recognition. Daily homework was provided using high frequency, imageable nouns ranging from 3-5 letters that were trained in sets of six. For each word, a picture and the printed word were provided. TS was instructed to look at the picture, then copy the word attempting to name each letter aloud, and finally say the word. This adaptation of the Copy and Recall Treatment (CART) that has been used to retrain spelling for targeted words (Beeson, 1999), was intended to facilitate reading by providing semantic information from the picture along with lexical information derived using the motor/kinesthetic strategy. Results TS received fifteen 1-hour treatment sessions over the course of 8 weeks. As shown in Figure 3, letter naming improved from 12/26 correct at pre-treatment to an average of 25/26 correct over the last three sessions, by employing the motor/kinesthetic strategy to self-cue her responses. Re-administration of the single-word reading list showed a slight improvement in oral reading (12% to 17% correct overall), but this difference was not statistically significant ( 2 = 2.46, p =.12). TS attempted to read letter-by-letter supported by the motor/kinesthetic strategy; however, it was extremely laborious. She often required several copies of a letter before it was correctly named, adding considerable demands on working memory as she tried to decode the written words. In addition, some of TS s reading errors reflected intrusions of trained words beginning with the same letter or visually similar to the target (e.g. tone bone). Discussion

In the case of alexia with agraphia presented here, a motor/kinesthetic strategy of copying individual letters served to improve letter identification, however, improved naming of letters in isolation did not result in a corresponding increase in single-word reading accuracy. This limited response to treatment differs from several other cases reported in the literature. The underlying premise of the motor-kinesthetic approach is that copying of individual letters provides sensorimotor information about letter identities that supports the degraded visual information. It appeared, however, that this cross-modality information was not easily accessible to TS, and that, in addition to a visual-verbal disconnection, TS also had a persistent disruption of visual-motor connections necessary to copy letters. This disconnection was evident on the pre-treatment assessment, but we did not anticipate that it would be resistant to remediation. The difference between TS and the other treatment cases may relate to the location and extent of the lesion. Although localizing information was somewhat limited in the other cases, it appeared that damage was predominantly in the left inferior temporo-occipital region, as is typical of individuals with pure alexia (Black & Behrmann, 1994). By contrast, TS s lesion had greater dorsal extension and produced more extensive damage to inter-hemispheric white matter fibers. Damage to dorsal callosal pathways is the most likely neural substrate of the visual-verbal and visual-motor disconnections that appeared to underlie TS persistent reading deficits.

References Black, S.E. & Behrmann, M. (1994). Localization in alexia: In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization and neuroimaging in neuropsychology (pp 331-376). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment. Aphasiology, 13, 367-386. Coltheart, M. (Ed.). (1998). Pure alexia (Letter-by-letter reading) [Special issue]. Cognitive Neuropsychology 15(1/2). Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston Diagnostic Examination for Aphasia. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E. & Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test, 2 nd Edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins. Greenwald, M. L., & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (1998). Lexical access via letter naming in a profoundly alexic and anomic patient: A treatment study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 595-607. Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kertesz, A. (1982). The Western Aphasia Battery. New York: The Psychological Corporation. Lott, S. N., Friedman, R. B., & Linebaugh, C. W. (1994). Rationale and efficacy for a tactile-kinaesthetic treatment for alexia. Aphasiology, 8, 181-195. Lott, S. N., & Friedman, R. B. (1999). Can treatment for pure alexia improve letter-byletter reading speed without sacrificing accuracy? Brain and Language, 67, 188-201. Maher, L. M., Clayton, M. C., Barrett, A. M., Schober-Peterson, D., & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (1998). Rehabilitation of a case of pure alexia: Exploiting residual abilities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 636-647. Rothi, L. J. G., Coslett, H. B., & Heilman, K. M. (1986). Battery of Adult Reading Function. Unpublished test.

Table 1. Summary of lesion and selected behavioral characteristics of alexic readers who exhibited positive response to tactile-kinesthetic treatment to improve letter identification Lott, Friedman & Linebaugh 1994 Lott & Friedman 1999 Greenwald & Gonzalez Rothi 1998 Maher et al. 1998 Patient TL DL MR VT Age/Gender 67/M 67/M 72/F 43/F Time post onset Lesion Aphasia profile Naming Visual deficit Oral reading single words Recognition of oral spelling Written spelling Visual lexical decision tasks Letter naming Stimuli/ Treatment approach Treatment outcome 14 months 5 months 13 months 14 months residual damage to the left posterior temporal and lateral occipital lobes Transcortical sensory aphasia 56/114 (49% correct) on BDAE confrontation naming Lower right homonymous quadrantanopia 24/30 (80% correct) on BDAE 8/8 (100% correct) on BDAE 6/10 (60% correct) on BDAE spelling to dictation None reported 38-58% correct during baseline Words/letters; copied into palm using capped pen Improved accuracy of letter naming, trained words and untrained words left posterior temporal-occipital lobe infarct Moderate anomic aphasia 50/114 (44% correct) on BDAE confrontation naming None reported 2/30 (7% correct) on BDAE 8/8 (100% correct) on BDAE 8/10 (80% correct) on BDAE spelling to dictation None reported hemorrhagic lesion involving occipital lobe, inferior temporal region to TPO junction Anomic aphasia 3/60 (5% correct) on BNT Right homonymous hemianopsia 0% correct on 96% correct on 67% correct on ; 77% oral spelling 22/80 (28%) on high image words/nonwords BA 28, 31, 18, left cerebellar hemisphere No evidence of aphasia WAB Naming: 9.7/10 59/60 (98% correct) on BNT Right homonymous hemianopsia 0% correct on 100% correct on 100% correct on 99% using motor strategy 16/26 (62% correct) 3/26 (12% correct) severely impaired Single letters; copied into palm using capped pen Improved accuracy of naming letters in isolation, in strings andinwords Single letters; traced with finger Significant improvement in oral letter naming; improved reading of written words using LBL strategy BDAE = Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983) BNT = Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1983) WAB = Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1982) = Battery of Adult Reading Function (Rothi et al., 1986) Words; traced with finger Improvement in reading speed using motor strategy; generalized to untreated sentence probes

Table 2. TS performance on pre-treatment assessment tasks % Score Correct Visual Processing 98% Mirror reversal letter identification (PALPA 18) 36/36 100% Upper-lower case match (PALPA 19) 26/26 100% Lower-upper case match (PALPA 20) 26/26 100% Lexical decision (words/illegal nonwords; PALPA 24) 56/60 93% Written word-picture match (PALPA 48) 38/40 95% Composite Score Visual Input Verbal or Motor Output 22% Direct copy of words (number of letters correct) 34/55 62% Case conversion: upper-to-lower 5/26 19% Case conversion: lower-to-upper 5/26 19% Letter naming (PALPA 22) 12/26 46% Letter to sound (visually presented letter) 3/20 15% Oral reading of regular words 4/40 10% Oral reading of irregular words 8/40 20% Oral reading of nonwords (length matched to words) 0/20 0% Oral reading CVC nonwords 4/60 7% Auditory Input Verbal or Motor Output 79% Writing letters to dictation 26/26 100% Writing regular words to dictation 33/40 83% Writing irregular words to dictation 23/40 58% Writing nonwords to dictation 19/20 95% Recognition of orally spelled regular words 29/40 73% Recognition of orally spelled irregular words 23/40 58% Recognition of orally spelled nonwords 13/20 65% Letter to sound (e.g., What sound does a B make? ) 14/20 70% Sound to letter name (e.g., What letter goes with /b/? ) 18/20 90% Spelling CVC nonwords 57/60 95% PALPA = Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia (Kay, Lesser & Coltheart, 1992)

Figure 1. CT showing TS left temporo-parieto-occipital damage R L R L

Figure 2 Sample of TS performance on direct copy of words vs. writing words to dictation gang mile doubt lose grumble drive honest

Figure 3 TS letter identification performance across duration of treatment Letter Identification 25 Baseline 20 # Correct 15 10 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Session