Dissertation Summaries. Headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics: Evidence from Italiot and Cypriot (University of Patras, 2014)*

Similar documents
A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

On the Notion Determiner

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

Beyond constructions:

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

STANDARDS. Essential Question: How can ideas, themes, and stories connect people from different times and places? BIN/TABLE 1

CX 105/205/305 Greek Language 2017/18

Phonological encoding in speech production

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Grammatical constructions, frame structure, and metonymy: Their contributions to metaphor computation

Som and Optimality Theory

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Akan verb-noun compounds

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Corpus Linguistics (L615)

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Ontologies vs. classification systems

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Advanced Grammar in Use

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Language contact in East Nusantara

Problems of the Arabic OCR: New Attitudes

1 Nonapriorism vs. apriorism

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

Mercer County Schools

South Carolina English Language Arts

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

- «Crede Experto:,,,». 2 (09) ( '36

What Can Neural Networks Teach us about Language? Graham Neubig a2-dlearn 11/18/2017

Compositional Semantics

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Linking Task: Identifying authors and book titles in verbose queries

Negation through reduplication and tone: implications for the LFG/PFM interface 1

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Words come in categories

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

Ontological spine, localization and multilingual access

Khairul Hisyam Kamarudin, PhD 22 Feb 2017 / UTM Kuala Lumpur

Transcription:

brill.com/jgl Dissertation Summaries Headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics: Evidence from Italiot and Cypriot (University of Patras, 2014)* Marios Andreou University of Patras, Greece andreoum@upatras.gr Abstract In this contribution, I offer a summary of my 2014 Ph.D. dissertation from the University of Patras on headedness in word formation and lexical semantics. Keywords morphology lexical semantics morphology-syntax interface head exocentricity bracketing paradoxes * I would like to take this opportunity to thank my Ph.D. committee members, Prof. Angela Ralli, Prof. Rochelle Lieber, and Prof. Brian D. Joseph, for their encouragement and guidance. This research has been funded by the Constantin Carathéodory program (d.159) of the University of Patras. koninklijke brill nv, leiden, 2014 doi: 10.1163/15699846-01402004

250 andreou 1 Introduction A basic notion in morphological theory and especially in frameworks which attribute internal hierarchical structure to words is the notion head. Since the seminal works of Lieber (1980), Williams (1981), Selkirk (1982), and Di Sciullo & Williams (1987), the syntactic notion of head has been extended from syntax to morphology. A corollary of this is that words, like syntactic phrases, are considered to have heads and thus the identification of head in all morphological configurations, derived words, compounds, and inflected forms, has been central to linguistic morphology. This extension, however, has not been without problems and several authors have expressed their scepticism regarding the overall use of this notion (see for instance the work of Bauer 1990). In recent years there has been an upsurge of interest in the study of head and related issues, such as the presence and absence of head, and several approaches have been developed (Bauer 2008, Scalise & Fábregas 2010, Kageyama 2010, Ralli 2013, Ralli & Andreou 2012). The application of the notion of head to morphological analysis, however, is highly problematic since head has been used in very different and often confusing ways. As far as semantics is concerned, the head is usually identified by the hyponymy test which renders the hyperonym head of the word. Head in lexical semantics, however, has never been studied in any detail despite the fact that the hyponymy test is not particularly useful for the identification of head in a number of morphological configurations. In addition, our understanding of the head-dependent linearization and the factors system-internal or systemexternal which govern this linearization, is rather limited. 2 The Study 2.1 Aims of the Thesis The thesis aims to sort out some of the confusions associated with the notion of head, focusing on headedness in Word Formation and Lexical Semantics. In particular, the purpose of my thesis is to inquire into the notion of head with a focus on the following three issues: (a) (b) (c) delimitation, position, and presence and absence of a head in morphological configurations.

headedness in word formation and lexical semantics 251 Given that the thesis aims to provide a comprehensive study of headedness, data comes from a number of languages and from different evolutionary stages. For example, in order to delimit head, one must take into consideration a number of morphological phenomena, such as compounding, English prefixation, and Greek evaluative suffixation. In addition, the question of position of the head necessarily involves the presentation of data from previous evolutionary stages of the Greek language, such as Ancient Greek. The thesis also aims to contribute to the study of Modern Greek dialects. In particular, I focus on data from two peripheral dialects, namely Italiot and Cypriot Greek. The analysis of morphological phenomena in these dialects can greatly inform the general discussion of headedness. In particular, the analysis of Italiot compounds allows one to comment on the head-dependent linearization, since in this dialect one finds left-headed compounds despite the fact that Greek compounds are generally right-headed. In a similar vein, Cypriot is particularly rich in compounding patterns and exhibits some of the rarest attested types of compounds, that is, verbal bahuvrihis. The analysis of compounding in Italiot and Cypriot is largely based on the rigorous descriptive studies of Andreou & Koliopoulou (2012) and Andreou (2013). 2.2 Structure of the Thesis and Main Proposals The thesis comprises four parts with two chapters each and a final chapter which summarizes the discussion. The nine chapters are followed by two appendices. Appendix a contains Greek left-headed [n n] n compounds and Appendix b presents an overview of the history of the distinction between endocentric and exocentric compounds and shows that credit for the introduction of the term exocentricity into linguistic theory is not due to Bloomfield (1933). 2.2.1 Introducing Head Part i serves as an introduction to the aims and structures of the thesis (chapter 1) and to the issues that concern us in the thesis. In chapter 2, I broadly set out the background and motivation for the investigation. An overview of the way head has been used in morphology and syntax reveals that there are widely divergent views with respect to the definition and properties of head. The presentation of the various definitions of head (see among others Zwicky 1985, Plag 2003, Booij 2007), shows that this lack of agreement partly stems from the fact that head is usually defined in such a broad way that the application of this notion to morphological analysis is rendered highly problematic. For most scholars, for example, the notion head captures the intuition that in a configuration comprising two elements, one of the elements is considered more

252 andreou important than the other, but this is of course a broad description rather than a definition of head. In addition, no consensus has been reached with respect to the properties of a head, and head has been used as a notion which unifies a number of other notions, including amongst others the subcategorizand and the morphosyntactic locus. 2.2.2 On the Definition of Head In Part ii, I address the question of delimitation and definition of head. In chapter 3, I comment on the delimitation of head in morphology with a focus on derivation and compounding. In order to do so, I first present the criteria used in the relevant literature with respect to the identification of the head and I then apply these criteria to derivation and compounding, respectively. The considerably vexed picture which emerges from the application of the various headship criteria to word formation reveals that most of the assumed head-like notions, such as the subcategorizand, might very well not be relevant to the head-nonhead asymmetry. The discussion of the results of the various headship criteria argues for a strictly categorial definition of head, in that the head for the purposes of morphology should be identified with the category determinant. The purpose of chapter 4 is to investigate the way the head-nonhead asymmetry should be accounted for in Lexical Semantics. To this aim, I adopt the framework of Lexical Semantics as developed by Lieber (2004). In this chapter, I argue that the head for the purposes of Lexical Semantics should be a matter of the skeletal features of a morpheme and, more specifically, the head should be identified with the ontological class determinant. In addition, the application of the notion head to the creation of semantically complex configurations reveals that the mechanism of subordination of functions should be split into subordination of functions with, (1a), and without indexation of arguments, (1b). In particular, non-argument-taking inflection, prefixation, and evaluative morphology should be accounted for by the mechanism in (1b): (1) Subordination of Functions a. [αf1 ([ ], [βf2 ([ ])])] b. [αf1 ([βf2 ([ ])])] This has implications for the way we classify morphemes in lexical semantics since any classification should take into consideration the following two main criteria: (a) the semantic features which are relevant to each morpheme and (b) the internal organization of the lexical-semantic representation of a morpheme. In this respect, the distinction between prefixes and suffixes which is

headedness in word formation and lexical semantics 253 based on the linear order of morphemes cannot be used as a criterion in lexical semantics. 2.2.3 On the Position of the Head Although head was introduced into morphology in the early 80 s, there is very little in the literature to date that deals with the issue of the head-nonhead order inside words. Part iii aims to present system-external (chapter 5) and system-internal (chapter 6) factors which govern constituent order in morphology with a focus on Greek compounding. More specifically, although Greek compounds are generally right-headed, in chapter 5, I present and analyze a number of left-headed compounds from various evolutionary stages of the Greek language and its dialects (with focus on the dialects spoken in Southern Italy). Consider the following left-headed compounds from Italiot Greek: table 1 Left-headed compounds in Italiot Greek Compound Comp. member 1 Comp. member 2 fiḍḍámbelo vine leaf klonósparto twig of sedge ššulopótamo lit. wood of the river, driftwood sporomáratho fennel seed fiḍḍ(o) leaf klon(o) twig ššul(o) wood spor(o) seed ambel(i) vine spart(o) sedge potam(o) river marath(o) fennel First, I comment on whether the presence of these formations could be attributed to the contact of Greek with Italian, which exhibits left-headed compounds, and then I present a number of factors, such as typological dissimilarity, that militate against the language interference hypothesis. In chapter 6, I delve more deeply into those system-internal factors which could affect the head-dependent linearization. In particular, I show that leftheadedness manifests itself in previous evolutionary stages of the Greek language and other Modern Greek dialects as well. In addition, I show that leftheadedness is not limited to compounds of the sporomáratho type, as it manifests itself in verb-first compounds of the type arche-laos ( lead-people ) leading the people, Homeric prepositional compounds such as en-oikios ( inhouse ) (one who is) in the house, and compounds composed of an adjective

254 andreou with governing properties as in iso-theos ( equal-god ) equal to the gods. The analysis of left-headed patterns reveals that the order of constituents in compounds may not be autonomous from syntax, given that the head-nonhead linearization inside compounds cannot change without previous change in the head-nonhead order in syntactic constructions (Harris & Campbell 1995, Gaeta 2008). This conclusion allows one to comment on the validity of the Lexical Integrity Hypothesis and the idea that the order of elements inside compound words obeys morphological settings only (Bresnan & Mchombo 1995). Finally, the exceptionality of the phenomenon of reordering and variation in compounding is highlighted by the fact that it has no parallel in derivation and inflection. 2.2.4 On the Presence and Absence of Head The presence or absence of head cuts across the endocentricity and exocentricity distinction but in order to address the question: The presence or absence of which head? Part iv aims to offer a detailed investigation of a phenomenon which, in my opinion, partly reflects the problematic way scholars have treated the notion head. Chapter 7 serves as an introduction to the distinction between endocentric and exocentric compounds. In this chapter, I critically evaluate literature on this issue and present data that militates against recent proposals that exocentricity and the notion head can be split into morphological, semantic, and categorial. Based on the distinction between nominal and adjectival bahuvrihis, which is often not taken into consideration by scholars, I argue that the former should be analyzed via metonymy and that the latter can be better understood if we examine the relation between compounding and derivation. In Chapter 8, I focus on the relation between two word-formation processes: compounding and derivation. In particular, I argue that the head in exocentric compounds is a derivational suffix (overt or zero) and present evidence in favour of zero-derivation. To adduce an example, Greek verbal bahuvrihis, such as the Cypriot varikart-izo to have a hard heart, to be sad, which are the rarest type of bahuvrihi compounds, are built on the basis of the pattern [[stem stem]-dsuf]. In addition, I tackle the issue of the inconsistency between the structure suggested by the morphophonological properties and the structure suggested by the meaning of exocentric compounds (bracketing paradoxes). Finally, I comment on the distinction between de-compounds and exocentric compounds and argue that exocentricity is compounding for the purposes of derivation. Chapter 9 summarizes the main proposals and looks ahead to possible advances.

headedness in word formation and lexical semantics 255 3 Conclusions In a nutshell, the main proposals with respect to headedness in morphology and lexical semantics are as follows: (a) (b) (c) Most of the assumed head-like notions, such as the subcategorizand, the morphosyntactic locus, and the obligatory constituent, might very well not be relevant to the head-nonhead asymmetry. The head for the purposes of morphology should be identified with the category determinant and with the ontological class determinant as far as lexical semantics is concerned. The enquiry into the postulation of functions and arguments reveals that non-argument taking inflection and derivation should be accounted for by the mechanism of subordination of functions without indexation of arguments. This has implications for the way we classify morphemes in lexical semantics. (d) With respect to the position of head, the phenomenon of left-headed [n n] n compounds in Italiot Greek should not be studied independently of the same phenomenon evident in previous evolutionary stages and in other dialects. (e) (f) (g) In addition, morphology does not combine and manipulate asymmetric relations only (see Di Sciullo 2005), given that morphemic order is amenable to change and variation may arise inside the morphological system of a language. From a diachronic point of view, the order of constituents in compounds may not be autonomous from syntax, since the head-nonhead linearization inside compounds cannot change without previous change in the head-nonhead order in syntactic constructions. With respect to the absence of a head, the analysis of the compound patterns which are considered to be exocentric reveals that exocentric compounds are bracketing paradoxes which involve compounding and derivation in this particular order. In this respect, I part company with previous analyses which identify exocentricity based on the hyponymy test and which collapse exocentricity and semantic non-compositionality. To conclude, my work on the notion head in word formation and lexical semantics raises a number of secondary questions, some of which I address in my thesis. I hope that future work will delve more deeply into the study of these questions.

256 andreou References Andreou, Marios. 2013. Compounding in the Greek dialects of Southern Italy. In Mark Janse, Brian Joseph, Angela Ralli & Metin Bagriacik (eds.). Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Modern Greek Dialects & Linguistic Theory, 1 18. Patras. Andreou, Marios & Maria Koliopoulou. 2012. I sinthesi stin Kipriaki ke Kalimniki: Mia proti sigkritiki proseggisi. [Compounding in Cypriot and Kalymniot: a first comparative analysis]. Neoelliniki Dialektologia 6: 7 29 (Athens: Academy of Athens). Bauer, Laurie. 1990. Be-heading the word. Journal of Linguistics 26: 1 31. Bauer, Laurie. 2008. Exocentric compounds. Morphology 18: 51 74. Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Holt, Rinchart and Winston. Booij, Geert. 2007. The grammar of words: An introduction to linguistic morphology (2nd ed.). Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press. Bresnan, Joan & Sam Mchombo. 1995. The lexical integrity principle: Evidence from Bantu. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 13: 181 254. Di Sciullo, Anna Maria. 2005. Asymmetry in morphology. Cambridge, ma: mit press. Di Sciullo, Anna Maria & Edwin Williams. 1987. On the definition of word. Cambridge, ma: mit Press. Gaeta, Livio. 2008. Constituent order in compounds and syntax: typology and diachrony. Morphology 18(2): 117 141. Harris, Alice C. & Lyle Campbell. 1995. Historical syntax in cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kageyama, Taro. 2010. Variation between endocentric and exocentric word structures. Lingua 120(10): 2405 2423. Lieber, Rochelle. 1980. On the organization of the lexicon. Ph.D. Diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, ma. [Published by Indiana University Linguistics Club, 1981 and Garland Press, 1990.]. Lieber, Rochelle. 2004. Morphology and lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Plag, Ingo. 2003. Word-formation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ralli, Angela. 2013. Compounding in Modern Greek. Dordrecht: Springer. Ralli, Angela & Marios Andreou. 2012. Revisiting exocentricity in compounding: evidence from Greek and Cypriot. In Ferenc Kiefer, Mária Ladányi & Péter Siptár (eds.). Current issues in morphological theory: (ir)regularity, analogy and frequency, 65 81. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Scalise, Sergio & Antonio Fábregas. 2010. The head in compounding. In Sergio Scalise & Irene Vogel (eds.). Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding, 109 126. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1982. The syntax of words. Cambridge, ma: mit Press.

headedness in word formation and lexical semantics 257 Williams, Edwin. 1981. On the notions of lexically related and head of the word. Linguistic Inquiry 12: 245 274. Zwicky, Arnold M. 1985. Heads. Journal of Linguistics 21(1): 1 29.