Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Similar documents
Computerized training of the correspondences between phonological and orthographic units

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

GraphoGame A learning environment for literacy acquisition: On the route to helping compromised readers across the globe

Does training in syllable recognition improve reading speed? A computer-based trial with poor

Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children. With and Without Language Impairments. Franklin R. Manis. University of Southern California.

The influence of orthographic transparency on word recognition. by dyslexic and normal readers

Ekapeli (in Finnish), GraphoGame (internationally)

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

Title of the book: Improving the Quality of Childhood in Europe 2014, Volume 5

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Cognitive bases of reading and writing in a second/foreign language. DIALUKI (

Exploring Dyslexics Phonological Deficit I: Lexical vs Sub-lexical and Input vs Output Processes

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities.

دافعية القراءة وعالقتها بفاعلية الذات القرائية والقلق القرائي لدى تالميذ صعوبات التعلم

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Running Head: PASS theory of intelligence in Greek 1. PASS theory of intelligence in Greek: A review

ABSTRACT. Some children with speech sound disorders (SSD) have difficulty with literacyrelated

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Short-term memory in Down syndrome: Applying the working memory model

No Parent Left Behind

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Investigating speech perception in children with dyslexia: is there evidence of a. consistent deficit in individuals? Abstract

A STUDY ON THE IMPACT OF ORTON-GILLINGHAM APPROACH ON SOLVING THE WRITING DISORDER OF PRIMARY SCHOOL DYSLEXIC CHILDREN AT COIMBATORE DISTRICT.

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Is rapid automatized naming automatic?

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Protocol: The Effect of Linguistic Comprehension Training on Language and Reading Comprehension: A Systematic Review

South Carolina English Language Arts

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

THE IMPACT OF DYSLEXIA ON LEARNING SIGN LANGUAGE

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

Lower and Upper Secondary

Biomedical Sciences (BC98)

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Rowan Digital Works. Rowan University. Angela Williams Rowan University, Theses and Dissertations

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Orthographic analysis of words during fluency training promotes reading of new similar words

Zealand Published online: 16 Jun To link to this article:

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

GEORGE KYRIACOU GEORGIOU CURRICULUM VITAE. July 2017 to present Full Professor Department of Educational Psychology, University of Alberta, Canada

2. CONTINUUM OF SUPPORTS AND SERVICES

Phonological encoding in speech production

Appendix. Journal Title Times Peer Review Qualitative Referenced Authority* Quantitative Studies

Unequal Opportunity in Environmental Education: Environmental Education Programs and Funding at Contra Costa Secondary Schools.

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Implementing the English Language Arts Common Core State Standards

The Impact of Morphological Awareness on Iranian University Students Listening Comprehension Ability

Scholastic Leveled Bookroom

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Language Center. Course Catalog

The Incentives to Enhance Teachers Teaching Profession: An Empirical Study in Hong Kong Primary Schools

Routledge Library Editions: The English Language: Pronouns And Word Order In Old English: With Particular Reference To The Indefinite Pronoun Man

THE ORAL PROFICIENCY OF ESL TEACHER TRAINEES IN DIFFERENT DISCOURSE DOMAINS

Reynolds School District Literacy Framework

Study Abroad Housing and Cultural Intelligence: Does Housing Influence the Gaining of Cultural Intelligence?

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

5/29/2017. Doran, M.K. (Monifa) RADBOUD UNIVERSITEIT NIJMEGEN

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

Special Education Program Continuum

Identifying the training needs of EFL teachers in teaching children with dyslexia

Enhancing Phonological Awareness, Print Awareness, and Oral Language Skills in Preschool Children

Special Education Services Program/Service Descriptions

RED 3313 Language and Literacy Development course syllabus Dr. Nancy Marshall Associate Professor Reading and Elementary Education

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

LITERACY, AND COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Conseil scolaire francophone de la Colombie Britannique. Literacy Plan. Submitted on July 15, Alain Laberge, Director of Educational Services

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

Reading Comprehension Tests Vary in the Skills They Assess: Differential Dependence on Decoding and Oral Comprehension

IMPROVING THE STUDENTS ENGLISH VOCABULARY MASTERY THROUGH PUZZLE GAME AT THE SIXTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SDN 1 SODONG GUNUNGHALU

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

Inclusion in Music Education

Identifying Students with Specific Learning Disabilities Part 3: Referral & Evaluation Process; Documentation Requirements

Development of the Grammar and Phonology Screening (GAPS) test to assess key markers of specific language and literacy difficulties in young children

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Tier 2 Literacy: Matching Instruction & Intervention to Student Needs

ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS

WHO ARE SCHOOL PSYCHOLOGISTS? HOW CAN THEY HELP THOSE OUTSIDE THE CLASSROOM? Christine Mitchell-Endsley, Ph.D. School Psychology

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Placement breakdown in foster care: Reducing risks by a foster parent training program? Maaskant, A.M.

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Match or Mismatch Between Learning Styles of Prep-Class EFL Students and EFL Teachers

prehending general textbooks, but are unable to compensate these problems on the micro level in comprehending mathematical texts.

BSc (Hons) in International Business

Providing student writers with pre-text feedback

THE IMPACT OF DYSLEXIA ON LEARNING SIGN LANGUAGE. A dissertation submitted to. The University of Bolton. Master of Education

Transcription:

The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 69 Aryan van der Leij, Elsje van Bergen and Peter de Jong Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why some children develop dyslexia and others don t. Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why some children develop dyslexia and others don t. Aryan van der Leij 1, Elsje van Bergen 2 and Peter de Jong 3 Prospective studies which involve children who have a dyslexic parent and, hence, have a family risk of becoming dyslexic, provide the opportunity to examine the differential development of at-risk children who do and do not become dyslexic and controls (i.e., not at-risk non-dyslexics). The question is in what respect at-risk children who do not develop dyslexia differ from those who do. First, it is interesting to know whether atrisk children without dyslexia perform at the level of controls in reading, spelling and related skills, or, alternatively, also show impairments but to a lesser degree than at-risk children with dyslexia. In addition, the question where these group differences stem from can be addressed. As learning to read is often believed to build on (oral) language abilities, one possibility is that at-risk non-dyslexic children show differences in preceding verbal development in comparison to at-risk children with dyslexia. Another possibility is that this group is at lower genetic risk, i.e. has a less dyslexic parent. Finally, it could be that this group experiences more advantageous environmental factors. Literacy and its cognitive underpinnings Elbro, Borstrøm, and Petersen (1998) followed the progress of Danish children with and without family risk from kindergarten through the beginning of second grade, when dyslexia was determined. In kindergarten, the at-risk dyslexics showed deficits as compared to the control group in phoneme awareness, verbal short-term memory, and distinctness of phonological 1 Research Institute Child Development and Education, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands 2 Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK 3 See 1

70 The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 representations, whereas the at-risk non-dyslexics did not. However, the at-risk non-dyslexics did perform as poorly as the at-risk dyslexics on morpheme awareness and articulation, whereas they performed better than the at-risk dyslexics but less well than the controls on letter knowledge. Pennington and Lefly (2001) also conducted an at-risk study. Dyslexia was assessed in second grade. At the start of first grade, the at-risk non-dyslexics performed poorer than controls but better than atrisk dyslexics on phoneme awareness and speech perception. Relating letter knowledge, verbal short-term memory, phonological awareness and rapid naming in kindergarten to reading in Grade 2, Boets et al. (2010; 2007) found that the performance of the at-risk non-dyslexic group was at an intermediate position between that of the other two groups, but did not differ significantly from either of them. In one of the samples of the Dutch Dyslexia Programme (DDP), at-risk children were followed from kindergarten through Grade 5 (van Bergen, de Jong, Regtvoort, Oort, van Otterloo, & van der Leij (2011). The at-risk children who went on to have dyslexia had poor letter knowledge in kindergarten. In addition, they were impaired on rapid naming. The at-risk children without dyslexia performed better on letter knowledge and rapid naming, but still below the level of controls. The groups did not differ on phonological awareness. At-risk dyslexics read less fluently from first grade onwards than the other groups. At-risk non-dyslexics reading fluency was at an intermediate position between the other groups at the start of reading. By fifth grade they had reached a similar level as the controls on word reading, but still lagged significantly behind on pseudoword reading. Replicating this study in a larger sample of the DDP, it was confirmed that, at the end of Grade 2, the at-risk children with dyslexia performed poorer than the other two groups on all measures, whereas the at-risk no-dyslexia children performed better than the at-risk dyslexia children, but still below the level of the controls on all tasks (van Bergen, de Jong, Plakas, Maassen & van der Leij, 2012). Interestingly, on rapid naming the at-risk no-dyslexia children were as fast as the controls.

The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 71 In sum, although not all differences reach significance, the evidence suggests that at-risk children who do not develop dyslexia show signs of milder, i.e. not clinical, impairments. The possible exception may be rapid naming which is comparable to controls when reading development is well under way (Grade 2). Environmental factors With regard to the possible origins of the differences between at-risk groups, the findings of several studies do not support an explanation in terms of environmental differences. As part of the Jyväskylä Longitudinal Study of Dyslexia (JLD), Torppa, Poikkeus, Laakso, Tolvanen, Leskinen, Leppänen, and Lyytinen (2007) assessed the relation between home literacy, reading interest, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and emergent literacy variables. They found a direct effect of home-literacy environment (shared reading) on vocabulary growth, but not on emergent literacy. Similarly, Elbro, Borstrøm and Petersen (1998) did not find differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic parents, nor between parents of dyslexic and non-dyslexic children in the amount of shared reading in kindergarten. Snowling, Miter and Carroll (2007) did not find differences between the two at-risk groups in parent reading behavior, family literacy behavior, and socioeconomic background. Finally, van Bergen et al. (2011) found that, although the parents of the groups of at-risk children differed in educational level and reading skills, there were no differences in literacy environment. In sum, there is hardly any evidence that children who develop dyslexia grow up in a relatively disadvantageous literacy environment. Verbal development In her seminal study, Scarborough (1990) compared at-risk children who later became dyslexic with control children with no family background of reading problems. She found that the at-risk dyslexic children were deficient in several language skills from age 2½ onwards. Slow language development was also shown by a prospective study of Snowling, Gallagher, and Frith (2003). When the children were 3 years and 9 months,

72 The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 the at-risk children who became dyslexic had deficiencies in vocabulary, narrative skills, and verbal short-term memory. At 6 years of age verbal ability of the dyslexic children was significantly lower than that of their peers without familial risk. Torppa, Lyytinen, Erskine, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2010) examined very early language markers of dyslexia. From age 2 onwards, the at-risk children who later became dyslexic showed impairments relative to the controls. At-risk children who became normal readers also tended to show weak expressive syntax and vocabulary at age 5, but this difference was not significant. In a related study (van Bergen, de Jong, Maassen, Krikhaar, Plakas, & van der Leij, in revision) it was detected that the at-risk group which developed dyslexia was characterized by relatively poor sentence comprehension, poor expressive syntax, weak vocabulary, and poor verbal short-term memory at 4 years of age. The study also revealed that, relative to the controls, the at-risk group without dyslexia had subtle, but significant, impairments in the verbal domain, while the at-risk group with dyslexia lagged more behind on verbal IQ. Because the described studies were executed in USA, UK, Finland and The Netherlands, it may be concluded that poor language development across a wide range of verbal abilities is an early marker of dyslexia, independent of orthography and language. Genetic risk Behavioral studies (e.g., Snowling et al., 2003) have indicated that the family risk of dyslexia is continuous. As a consequence, it may be hypothesized that at-risk dyslexic children have a higher genetic liability than at-risk non-dyslexics: their parents have more severe reading and spelling problems. However, Snowling et al. (2007) reported that the parents of at-risk dyslexics did not differ from the parents of at-risk non-dyslexics on spelling and word-reading accuracy. In contrast, van Bergen et al. (2011) found relatively better reading skills of parents of at-risk non-dyslexics, which suggests that these children might have a lower genetic liability. The findings of the study of Torppa, Eklund, van Bergen and Lyytinen (2011) supported the differences in liability of at-risk children with and without

The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 73 dyslexia: the parents of the at-risk children with dyslexia had more severe difficulties in pseudoword reading, spelling accuracy, rapid word recognition and in text reading fluency, than the parents of the at-risk children without dyslexia. Van Bergen et al. (2012) also investigated rapid naming, phonology, spelling, and word and pseudoword reading of parents and their children. With regard to parental reading, the conclusion of the earlier study (van Bergen et al., 2011) that the at-risk children who develop dyslexia are likely to have a higher liability was supported, although less outspoken and restricted to word reading (and not pseudoword reading). Interestingly, the non-dyslexic parents of the at-risk children who developed dyslexia, had poorer reading performance than its counterpart of the at-risk non-dyslexics, indicating that genetic liability may also be a result of genetic influences from both parents. In addition, an important finding was that the parents of the at-risk children without dyslexia, relative to those of dyslexic children, were better in rapid naming (as were their children). Both the group comparisons and the parent-child relations highlight the importance of good rapid naming skills for reading acquisition. Conclusion The described prospective studies, in particular the comparison of at-risk children who do and do not become dyslexic and controls, have added much to our understanding of developmental dyslexia. First, genetic risk affects reading and spelling performance on a scale of more to less. Although the performance in reading and spelling of at-risk children without dyslexia is within the normal range, mild impairments have been detected with regard to (nearly) all the relevant determinants of reading and spelling (phonological awareness, letter knowledge, rapid naming). Possibly, these impairments give them a slow start which they overcome in due time. Apart from the fact that their impairments differ quantitatively from the more severe deficits shown by the at-risk children who develop dyslexia, the question whether there are compensating mechanisms has still to be answered. The mechanism that enables rapid serial

74 The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 naming seems to be an interesting candidate that deserves further study. Second, the prospective studies indicate that poor language development across a wide range of verbal abilities, including verbal IQ, is an early marker of dyslexia, independent of orthography and language. As was the case with the determinants of reading and spelling, the at-risk group which does not develop dyslexia seems to have an intermediate position between the at-risk, dyslexic, group and the non atrisk controls. It should be noted, however, that the differences in early language markers are small and that even the poorer performances of the children who develop dyslexia do not qualify for the clinical range. Third, supporting the hypothesis of a transgenerational continuum of genetic risk, the Finnish JLD and Dutch DDP studies have provided evidence that the chance of becoming dyslexic partly depends on the severity of the reading problems of the dyslexic parent. On addition, two intriguing findings warrant further investigation. As was the case with the children, the performance in rapid naming of the dyslexic parent seems to have an impact on the chance of becoming dyslexic as a child. In addition, the reading performance of the other, i.e. non dyslexic, parent might be of importance. That genetic liability may depend on these genetic factors is also indicated by the fact that it is unlikely that children who develop dyslexia grow up in a relatively disadvantageous literacy environment. At-risk families seem to cope with the problem of stimulating emergent literacy at the same level as control families. References Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Genes, Cognition, and Communication. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156 (The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009), 1-18. Boets, B., De Smedt, B., Cleuren, L., Vandewalle, E., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in

The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 75 Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5-31. Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high-risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1608-1620. Elbro, C., Borstrøm, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 36-60. Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72(3), 816-833. Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children Child Development, 61(6), 1728-1743. Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A. M., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74(2), 358-373. Snowling, M. J., Muter, V., & Carroll, J. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: a follow-up in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(6), 609-618. Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Tolvanen, A., Leskinen, E., Leppanen, P. H. T., et al. (2007). Modeling the early paths of phonological awareness and factors supporting its development in children with and without familial risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(2), 73-103. Torppa, M., Eklund, K., van Bergen, E., & Lyytinen, H. (2011). Parental literacy predicts children s literacy: a longitudinal family-risk study. Dyslexia, 17, 339-355. Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills,and predictive

76 The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308-321. van Bergen, E., de Jong, P.F., Regtvoort, A., Oort, F., van Otterloo, S., & van der Leij, A. (2011). Dutch children at family risk of dyslexia: Precursors, reading development, and parental effects. Dyslexia, 17 (1), 2-18. van Bergen, E., de Jong, P.F., Plakas, A., Maassen, B., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53 (1), 28-36. van Bergen, E., de Jong, P.F., Maassen, B., Krikhaar, E., Plakas, A., & van der Leij, A. (submitted). IQ of four-year-olds who go on to develop dyslexia.