The Prosodic (Re)organization of Determiners

Similar documents
The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Phonological encoding in speech production

LEXICAL CATEGORY ACQUISITION VIA NONADJACENT DEPENDENCIES IN CONTEXT: EVIDENCE OF DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGE AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES.

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Truncation to Subminimal Words

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

2,1 .,,, , %, ,,,,,,. . %., Butterworth,)?.(1989; Levelt, 1989; Levelt et al., 1991; Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer, 1999

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

Specification and Evaluation of Machine Translation Toy Systems - Criteria for laboratory assignments

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

The influence of metrical constraints on direct imitation across French varieties

A Cross-language Corpus for Studying the Phonetics and Phonology of Prominence

Journal of Phonetics

2014 Colleen Elizabeth Fitzgerald

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

THE ACQUISITION OF INFLECTIONAL MORPHEMES: THE PRIORITY OF PLURAL S

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

The role of markedness in the acquisition of complex prosodic structures by German-Spanish bilinguals*

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Acquiring verb agreement in HKSL: Optional or obligatory?

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Demonstration of problems of lexical stress on the pronunciation Turkish English teachers and teacher trainees by computer

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 12 December 2011 ISSN

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Liaison acquisition, word segmentation and construction in French: A usage based account

Atypical Prosodic Structure as an Indicator of Reading Level and Text Difficulty

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Interfacing Phonology with LFG

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

The Language of Football England vs. Germany (working title) by Elmar Thalhammer. Abstract

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Som and Optimality Theory

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Underlying Representations

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

On the Notion Determiner

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic

Jacqueline C. Kowtko, Patti J. Price Speech Research Program, SRI International, Menlo Park, CA 94025

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

5/29/2017. Doran, M.K. (Monifa) RADBOUD UNIVERSITEIT NIJMEGEN

Optimality Theory and the Minimalist Program

Transcription:

The Prosodic (Re)organization of Determiners Katherine Demuth, Elizabeth McCullough, and Matthew Adamo Brown University 1. Introduction* * Researchers have long known that children variably produce grammatical morphemes in spontaneous speech (e.g., Brown, 1973). Thus, a given child on a given day may supply a determiner (such as the article a or the) or a tense morpheme (such as the past tense marker t/d) in some utterances but not in others. This has typically led researchers to propose that children s syntactic representations are immature or syntactically underspecified (e.g., Wexler, 1994; Rice, Wexler, & Cleave, 1995). However, in a series of elicited production experiments, Gerken (1996) showed that English-speaking 2-year-olds were more likely to produce determiners (in this case definite articles) when these could be prosodified as part of a Strong-weak metrical foot. Thus, children were more likely to produce a determiner in sentences like (1a) (where the can be prosodified as part of a Strong-weak metrical foot with the preceding monosyllabic verb) than in sentences like (1b) (where the remains unfooted following a disyllabic verb, which is already a foot). (1) a. Tom [pushed the] Ft [zebra] Ft S w b. Tom [pushes] Ft the [zebra] Ft S w w These findings are interesting for several reasons. First, these results suggest that children s variable production of some grammatical morphemes may be more systematic than typically assumed. Second, these results indicate that some of the variability in early morpheme production may be due to prosodic constraints, rather than due to either a lack of syntactic representations or to non-linguistic, processing factors (Valian, 1991). *We thank Delani Gunawardena, Eun Suk Ko, Claartje Levelt, Philip Lieberman, Jim Morgan, Jae Yung Song, Annie Tremblay, and audiences at the University of Massachusetts at Amherst, the MIT Speech Group, and the Institute for Learning and Brian Science (I-LABS) at the University of Washington for comments and discussion. This research has been supported by NIMH Grant #R01MH60922.

Third, similar findings have been noted in other languages with very different prosodic structures, suggesting that such results may be very robust. For example, in a recent quantitative analysis, Demuth & Ellis (in press) found that noun class prefixes in the Bantu language Sesotho were much more likely to be produced if they preceded a monosyllabic nominal stem (e.g., le-ru cloud ) than if they preceded a disyllabic nominal stem, which already constitutes a foot of structure (e.g., le-tsasti sun ). Importantly, these children also demonstrate knowledge of which gender/class the noun belonged to when the noun class prefix was omitted. This was evident when the noun was followed by the appropriately inflected nominal modifier (e.g., (le)-tsasti le-na day that (i.e., that day )) (Demuth, 1988). Thus, noun class prefixes were not being omitted due to the child s uncertainly about which of the 11 noun classes the noun belonged to. These prosodic effects last in Sesotho until around the age of 2;3, when noun class prefixes start to be more reliably produced with nominal stems of 2 syllables or more. The acquisition of French determiners shows a similar pattern, with determiners being more consistently used with monosyllabic nouns several months before they consistently appear with disyllabic and trisyllabic nouns (Tremblay, 2006; Tremblay & Demuth, in press; Demuth & Tremblay, in press). These findings suggest that it should be possible to make predictions about the course of determiner acquisition given the prosodic structure of a given language. However, the Sesotho and French findings come from analyses of longitudinal spontaneous speech corpora, whereas the previous English findings came from cross-sectional, elicited production studies (Gerken, 1996). The goal of the present study was therefore to determine if English-speaking children show the same tendency to prosodify determiners as part of a trochaic foot in spontaneous speech productions. If so, this would provide further support for the notion that some grammatical morphemes are variably produced depending on the prosodic context in which they appear. 2. Study 1: Children s production of Footed vs. Unfooted determiners 2.1 Method To investigate this issue we examined the spontaneous productions of five 1-2-year-old children (3 girls, 2 boys) from the Providence Corpus, focusing on the period when determiners were being acquired (MLU 1.3-3). All interactions were between parent (usually mother) and child during daily play and other activities in the home. Both parent and child wore radio microphones, and all sessions were audio- and video-recorded for later analysis. The files were then downloaded onto a computer and transcribed using CHAT conventions (MacWhinney, 2000). Children s utterances were then phonetically transcribed, with ten percent of each session being retranscribed by another trained transcriber. Inter-coder reliability for segments was 86%.

Using methods employed by Vihman & McCune (1994), we identified all target nouns used in contexts where a determiner should be present. Target determiners were then coded as potentially occurring in either Footed (1a) or Unfooted (1b) contexts. We then calculated the percent of determiners produced in each context. We predicted that the children would show earlier use of determiners in Footed as opposed to Unfooted contexts, and that the use of Unfooted determiners would gradually increase over time. 2.2 Results Four of the children (Naima, Ethan, Violet, William) showed the expected pattern, producing significantly more determiners in Footed as compared to Unfooted contexts. These results are shown in Table 1. Note that the Unfooted determiners constitute the majority of contexts in which target determiners appear. Note also that the higher rate of production of Footed determiners is not due to frequency effects. Table 1. Number (percent) production of Footed vs. Unfooted Determiners NAIMA Footed Unfooted Significance Total 1;4.18 9/15 (60) 5/54 (9) p < 0.001 14/69 (20) 1;5.11 9/12 (75) 2/73 (3) p < 0.001 11/85 (13) 1;6.10 18/21 (86) 8/62 (13) p < 0.001 26/83 (31) 1;7.10 94/99 (95) 24/42 (57) p < 0.001 118/141 (84) ETHAN Footed Unfooted Significance Total 1;5.17 3/14 (21) 4/141 (3) p < 0.01 7/155 (5) 1;6.21 18/32 (56) 9/154 (6) p < 0.001 27/186 (15) 1;7.14 11/14 (79) 3/63 (5) p < 0.001 14/77 (18) 1;8.22 4/5 (80) 6/25 (24) p < 0.05 10/30 (33) 1;9.27 16/19 (84) 19/35 (54) p < 0.05 35/54 (65) VIOLET Footed Unfooted Significance Total 1;8 2/5 (40) 3/35 (9) p < 0.05 5/40 (13) 1;9 6/8 (75) 6/95 (6) p < 0.001 12/103 (12) 1;10 29/33 (88) 18/49 (37) p < 0.001 47/82 (57) 1;11 32/37 (86) 16/46 (35) p < 0.001 48/83 (58) 2;0 21/22 (95) 26/41 (63) p < 0.01 47/63 (75)

WILLIAM Footed Unfooted Significance Total 2;1 20/27 (74) 17/54 (31) p < 0.001 37/81 (46) 2;2 13/14 (93) 4/25 (16) p < 0.001 17/39 (44) 2;3 41/49 (84) 12/37 (32) p < 0.001 53/86 (62) 2;4 47/53 (89) 24/37 (65) p < 0.01 71/90 (79) LILY Footed Unfooted Significance Total 1;9.25 3/5 (60) 23/51 (45) n.s. 26/56 (46) 1;10.08 19/33 (58) 54/74 (73) n.s. 73/107 (68) 1;11.07 46/53 (87) 48/62 (77) n.s. 94/115 (82) 2;0.11 60/65 (92) 55/75 (73) p < 0.01 115/140 (82) Thus, the majority of the children in this study supported our prediction that determiners would be produced earlier and with more accuracy in Footed as opposed to Unfooted contexts. However, one child (Lily) showed no effect of prosodic context, producing determiners in both contexts with the same accuracy. Interestingly, she produced Unfooted determiners at a higher rate earlier than the other children, and Footed determiners at a lower rate. Only at 2;0.11 (MLU 2.1) did her production of Footed determiners increase and a significant difference between the two contexts emerge. Coders had also noted that Lily s determiners tended to be stressed. This suggests that she was initially producing determiners as separate Prosodic Words rather than as Footed or Unfooted prosodic clitics. To explore this possibility further we conducted an acoustic analysis of Lily s determiner productions in Study 2. 3. Study 2: Acoustic analysis of Lily s determiner productions 3.1 Method To further explore why Lily was initially producing all her determiners the same way, and why this changed around 2 years, we first identified all the tokens of determiners that were produced in target Footed and Unfooted contexts at 1;10.8, and 2;0.11 (henceforth Time 1 and Time 2). We then extracted all sound files for each of these tokens where the word following the determiner began with a stop, fricative, or affricate (i.e., a non-sonorant consonant). This would allow for clearly measuring the onset of the following noun. We then listened to each sound file and discarded any with poor acoustics or overlapping speech. This resulting in a total of 52 utterances analyzed at Time 1 (15 Footed and 37 Unfooted), and 47 utterances analyzed at Time 2 (21 Footed and 26 Unfooted). Recall that our hypothesis was that Lily was initially treating all determiners as separate prosodic units. If so, this would mean that she was not prosodifying her early Footed determiners as part of a metrical unit with the preceding word. To investigate this possibility we conducted three different acoustic

(durational) measurements and compared these at Time 1 and Time 2 to see if there was any change in prosodic organization. Consider the contexts in (2). (2) Footed Context Unfooted Context [it s a] [bag] a [dog] a. b. c. First, we predicted that the duration between Footed determiners and the preceding word, indicated in (2a), would shorten between Time 1 and Time 2. This would indicate that these determiners were initially produced as separate prosodic units, and had subsequently undergone prosodic incorporation into a foot. We also measured the durational difference between the Footed determiner and the following noun (2b). Although we did not expect any change in this duration, we anticipated that it might increase if the duration between the determiner and the preceding word decreased by a large amount. Finally, for determiners appearing in target Unfooted contexts, we predicted no change in duration between the determiner and following noun (2c), since there was no change in behavior during this time. It therefore served as a control, ensuring that any changes in duration would not be due merely to an increase in speaking rate. 3.2 Results The results indicated that there was a significant shortening in mean duration from Time 1 to Time 2 between the determiner and preceding word in Footed contexts (2a) (t(31) = 5.929, p < 0.001). There was also a non-significant increase from Time 1 to Time 2 in the mean duration between the determiner and following noun in Footed contexts (2b) (t(32) = -2.020, p = 0.052). Critically, there was no difference in mean duration between the determiner and the following word in Unfooted contexts (2c) (t(59) = 0.504, p = 0.616), ensuring that the changes found in the other contexts were not merely artifacts of increased speaking rate. These findings therefore strongly support our hypothesis that by the age of 2, Lily s grammar had undergone prosodic reorganization, though only for Footed determiners. That is, she had begun to treat Footed determiners as prosodically incorporated metrical units. Her Unfooted determiners remain as prosodically independent entities. 4. Discussion The present study confirms that for many English-speaking children, the early use of determiners is prosodically licensed. That is, determiners are much more likely to be used in children s early speech when these appear in prosodically available contexts. For many children this will entail the earlier

production of both lexical and functional material that can be metrically prosodified as part of a disyllabic foot. That this occurs in prosodically very different languages (e.g., Sesotho (with penultimate duration/trochaic feet), French (with final duration/iambic feet), English (with stress/trochaic feet)), suggests that this is a robust phenomenon (cf. Allen & Hawkins (1978, 1980) for further support of such a position). However, some of our research on Spanish indicates that this may be an artifact of prosodic word structure in each of these languages. Both English and French have very few trisyllabic lexical items, and Sesotho has predominantly disyllabic verbal and nominal stems. Thus, children learning these languages are exposed to a high frequency of monosyllabic and/or disyllabic word forms. We suggest that this has an effect on the size of the prosodic window these learners can handle at early stages of development. In contrast, learners of a language like Spanish are exposed to a much higher frequency of trisyllabic and even quadrasyllabic words (e.g., manzana apple, escalera stairs ) (Roark & Demuth, 2000). Lleó & Demuth (1999) suggest that this is why they exhibit the use of proto-determiners from the earliest stages of acquisition (see also Lleó, 1997, 1998, 2001; Demuth, 2001). Some studies indicate that English-speaking children begin to acquire unfooted syllables as part of lexical items around the age of 2;6 (e.g. Pater, 1997). However, it is as yet unclear exactly what the relationship is between children s acquisition of initial unstressed (unfooted) syllables in a word like banana, and the unfooted determiner in an utterance like the dolly. It is therefore interesting to note that, at 2 years, Lily produced her first full form of the name Manuela. Prior to that time she had always truncated the research assistant s name to Wela, omitting the initial weak unfooted syllable. Tremblay & Demuth (in press) suggest that both of the French-speaking children they studied showed prosodic reorganization of their determiners as they began to be able to produce larger and more prosodically complex lexical items. Such a view suggests that developments at the level of the lexicon may force the development of higherlevel prosodic structures. Consider the Prosodic Hierarchy in Figure 1 (Selkirk, 1984; Nespor & Vogel, 1986).

Utt (Phonological Utterance) I saw the inspector give the boy a banana IP (Intonational Phrase) I saw the inspector PP (Phonological Phrase) the inspector PW (Prosodic Word) inspector Ft (Foot) pector σ (Syllable) pec µ (Mora) pe Figure 1. The Prosodic Structure of Grammatical Function Items Using the Prosodic Hierarchy, Selkirk (1996) outlines the possible prosodic structures available for grammatical function items, as shown in (4). We suggest that children s early determiners may initially be prosodified with in the Foot (4c), but that they later become prosodified as proposed for adult grammars, at higher levels of the Phonological Phrase (4b). This is obviously an issue for further investigation. a. Prosodic Word b. Free Clitic c. Internal clitic d. Affixal clitic PP PP PP PP PW PW fnc PW PW PW fnc lex lex Ft fnc PW fnc lex lex Figure 2. The Prosodic Structure of Grammatical Function Items We have argued here for a prosodic explanation for the variability in children s early production of determiners. That is, determiners will be produced earlier in those contexts where they are prosodically licensed. Further support for this position comes from some of the individual variation found, where one child underwent prosodic reorganization during the course of the study. Could there, however, be an alternative, non-linguistic explanation of the data presented here? For example, perhaps the contexts in which determiners were more likely to be produced (the Footed contexts) were shorter than the contexts where Unfooted determiners were attempted.

To evaluate this possibility we compared the number of words in target Footed and Unfooted contexts. We found that the length of the target Footed contexts were longer than the Unfooted contexts for all the children. We then considered the length of the utterances in which determiners were actually produced. Again, the length of the utterances with Footed determiners was longer for all the children, except for one where there was no difference. Thus, there appears to be no support for a non-linguistic explanation of the findings. 5. Conclusion The goal of this paper was to investigate the possibility that some of the variability found in children s spontaneous production of determiners would be due to prosodic licensing effects. Thus, we predicted that, as found in Gerken (1996), children would be more likely to produce determiners in contexts where these could be metrically prosodified as part of a disyllabic foot. To carry out this study we examined longitudinal spontaneous production data from 5 children aged 1;4 2;4. We found that 4 of the children showed exactly the pattern predicted, exhibiting a high early use of determiners in prosodically licensed contexts. The fifth child showed a different pattern, indicating that she was not prosodically licensing Footed determiners until the age of 2. This was confirmed with acoustic (durational) measurements, showing prosodic reorganization of the child s grammar at this point in time. Further analyses ruled out possible non-linguistic (length effect) explanations of the data. The findings from the present study therefore provide further crosslinguistic support for the hypothesis that some of the variability found in children s early production of grammatical morphemes is systematic and predictable, and that this is due to linguistic, not non-linguistic factors. Specifically, it shows that children s early use of determiners is subject to phonological, or prosodic constraints. This suggests that children may have earlier syntactic knowledge of certain grammatical morphemes than typically thought. This raises important questions for theories of syntactic development, as well as methodological issues for designing syntactic experiments. References Allen, George D., & Hawkins, Sarah (1978). The development of phonological rhythm. In A. Bell & J. B. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and Segments (pp. 173-185). North Holland: Amsterdam. Allen, George D., & Hawkins, Sarah (1980). Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child Phonology Vol. 1: Production (pp. 227-256). New York: Academic Press. Brown, Roger (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Demuth, Katherine (1988). Noun classes and agreement in Sesotho acquisition. In M. Barlow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Agreement in Natural Languages: Approaches, Theories and Descriptions (pp. 305-321). CSLI: University of Chicago Press.

Demuth, Katherine (2001). Prosodic constraints on morphological development. In J. Weissenborn & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to Bootstrapping: Phonological, Syntactic and Neurophysiological Aspects of Early Language Acquisition. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders Series, vol. 24 (pp. 3-21). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Demuth, Katherine, & Ellis, David (in press). Revisiting the acquisition of Sesotho noun class prefixes. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, S. Ervin-Tripp, N. Budwig, S. Özçaliskan, & K. Nakamura (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Festschrift for Dan Slobin. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Demuth, Katherine, & Tremblay, Annie (in press). Prosodic licensing of determiners in French acquisition. Journal of Child Language. Gerken, LouAnn (1996). Prosodic structure in young children s language production. Language, 72, 683-712. Lleó, Conxita (1997). Filler syllables, proto-articles and early prosodic constraints in Spanish and German. In A. Sorace, C. Heycock, & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Language Acquisition: Knowledge, Representation and Processing. Proceedings of GALA '97 (pp. 251-256). Lleó, Conxita (1998). Proto-articles in the acquisition of Spanish: Interface between phonology and morphology. In R. Fabri, A. Ortmann, & T. Parodi (Eds.), Modelle der Flexion: 18. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft. Tübingen: Niemeyer Verlag. Lleó, Conxita (2001). The interface of phonology and syntax: The emergence of the article in the early acquisition of Spanish and German. In J. Weissenborn & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to Bootstrapping: Phonological, Syntactic and Neurophysiological Aspects of Early Language Acquisition. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders Series, vol. 24 (pp. 23-44). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Lleó, Conxita, & Demuth, Katherine (1999). Prosodic constraints on the emergence of grammatical morphemes: Crosslinguistic evidence from Germanic and Romance languages. In A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield, & C. Tano (Eds.), Proceedings of the 23 rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. MacWhinney, Brian (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. 3 rd Edition. Vol 2: The Database. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Nespor, Marina, & Vogel, Irene (1986). Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris Publications. Pater, Joe (1997). Minimal violation and phonological development. Language Acquisition, 6, 201-253. Rice, Mabel L., Wexler, Kenneth, & Cleave, Patricia (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850-863. Roark, Brian, & Demuth, Katherine (2000). Prosodic constraints and the learner s environment: A corpus study. In S. C. Howell, S. A. Fish, & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 597-608). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Selkirk, Elisabeth (1984). Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Selkirk, Elisabeth (1996). The prosodic structure of function words. In J. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition (pp. 187-213). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tremblay, Annie (2006). Prosodic constraints on the production of grammatical

morphemes in French: The case of determiners. In K. Deen, J. Nomura, B. Schulz, & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), Proceedings of the Inaugural Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (pp. 377-388). Cambridge, MA: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics. Tremblay, Annie, & Demuth, Katherine (in press). Prosodic licensing of determiners in children s early French. In A. Belikova, L. Meroni, & M. Umeda (Eds.), Galana 2 Proceedings of the Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Valian, Virginia (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 40, 21-81. Vihman, Marilyn M., & McCune, Lorraine (1994). When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21, 517-542. Wexler, Kenneth (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations in child grammar. In D. Lightfoot & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Verb movement (pp. 305-350). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.