Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Similar documents
Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants

Language Acquisition Chart

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Course Law Enforcement II. Unit I Careers in Law Enforcement

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Unit: Human Impact Differentiated (Tiered) Task How Does Human Activity Impact Soil Erosion?

Visual Cognition Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Word learning as Bayesian inference

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Word Segmentation of Off-line Handwritten Documents

PREP S SPEAKER LISTENER TECHNIQUE COACHING MANUAL

Ohio s New Learning Standards: K-12 World Languages

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

1 Copyright Texas Education Agency, All rights reserved.

Lecturing Module

Retrieval in cued recall

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

teacher, peer, or school) on each page, and a package of stickers on which

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

TRANSFER APPLICATION: Sophomore Junior Senior

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

Unit 3. Design Activity. Overview. Purpose. Profile

HIGH SCHOOL PREP PROGRAM APPLICATION For students currently in 7th grade

Fostering social agency in multimedia learning: Examining the impact of an animated agentõs voice q

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Going to School: Measuring Schooling Behaviors in GloFish

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

THE PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION OF STRESS AND INTONATION BY CHILDREN WITH COCHLEAR IMPLANTS

Enduring Understandings: Students will understand that

been each get other TASK #1 Fry Words TASK #2 Fry Words Write the following words in ABC order: Write the following words in ABC order:

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Increasing Student Engagement

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall

Why Misquitoes Buzz in People s Ears (Part 1 of 3)

Adaptations and Survival: The Story of the Peppered Moth

An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

Language Arts: ( ) Instructional Syllabus. Teachers: T. Beard address

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Cooper Upper Elementary School

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Table of Contents. Introduction Choral Reading How to Use This Book...5. Cloze Activities Correlation to TESOL Standards...

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

A cautionary note is research still caught up in an implementer approach to the teacher?

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Making the ELPS-TELPAS Connection Grades K 12 Overview

Examinee Information. Assessment Information

MULTIDISCIPLINARY TEAM COMMUNICATION THROUGH VISUAL REPRESENTATIONS

Age-Related Differences in Communication and Audience Design

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

Practices Worthy of Attention Step Up to High School Chicago Public Schools Chicago, Illinois

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Merbouh Zouaoui. Melouk Mohamed. Journal of Educational and Social Research MCSER Publishing, Rome-Italy. 1. Introduction

One Stop Shop For Educators

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Contents. Foreword... 5

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Essentials of Ability Testing. Joni Lakin Assistant Professor Educational Foundations, Leadership, and Technology

Redirected Inbound Call Sampling An Example of Fit for Purpose Non-probability Sample Design

Unit 13 Assessment in Language Teaching. Welcome

LEXICAL CATEGORY ACQUISITION VIA NONADJACENT DEPENDENCIES IN CONTEXT: EVIDENCE OF DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGE AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES.

Transcription:

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds Anne L. Fulkerson 1, Sandra R. Waxman 2, and Jennifer M. Seymour 1 1 University of Toledo, 2 Northwestern University 1. Introduction The infant s environment is filled with objects. If each were treated as unique, this complexity would quickly become overwhelming. Therefore, the task for the young infant is to construct categories that capture similarities among objects and to learn words that express these categories. And the task for developmental researchers is to figure out how infants do just that. Recent work has revealed that categorization and word learning are not isolated abilities that develop independently, but are linked early in life. In particular, providing the same name (e.g., dog ) for a series of distinct objects (e.g., Golden Retriever, Collie, Poodle) helps infants to identify commonalities among those objects and promotes the formation of object categories (Balaban & Waxman, 1997, Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003, Waxman & Markow, 1995). This phenomenon was first documented in a study with 12- to 13-month-old infants (Waxman & Markow, 1995). In that study, infants were familiarized to four different objects from the same category (e.g., animals) with either a labeling phrase (e.g., See the fauna?, See the faunish one? ) or a non-labeling phrase (e.g., See here? ). Following familiarization, infants detection of the category was assessed with a novelty-preference test in which a new object from the familiarized category (e.g., a new animal) was presented with an object from a different category (e.g., vehicle). Infants hearing novel words successfully formed categories, as evidenced by their preference for the out-of-category test object. Infants in the no word control condition did not form these categories. Other studies reveal that object naming also supports the acquisition of completely novel categories (Booth & Waxman, 2002; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, in press) and directs infants attention beyond similarities that can readily be perceived, focusing them on deeper, nonobvious commonalities (Graham, Kilbreath, & Welder, 2004; Welder & Graham, 2001). Findings such as these point to an early and powerful link between object naming and object categorization. But what is the origin of this link and when does it become specific to language? The weight of the currently available evidence indicates that words and object categories may be linked from the onset of lexical acquisition. Several studies have demonstrated that infants start out with a general expectation that words (independent of their grammatical form class) highlight a broad range of commonalities among objects, including category- and propertybased commonalities. This broad expectation has several distinct developmental advantages: It guides infants earliest efforts to map words to their meanings and promotes the acquisition of object categories. Moreover, with exposure to the language under acquisition, this initially broad expectation becomes increasingly fine-tuned, permitting infants to discover the grammatical forms marked in their language (e.g., noun, verb, adjective) and map them to distinct types of meaning (e.g., object categories, object properties, event categories) (Waxman, 1998; Waxman & Lidz, in press). Moreover, current evidence suggests that this link may be specific to language. For example, for infants as young as 9 months of age, words facilitate object categorization, but nonlinguistic sounds (tones) do not (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003). In one study, 9-month-olds were visually familiarized to line-drawn objects from a single category (e.g., rabbits); these were presented either in conjunction with a word (e.g., rabbit ) or a pure tone. Results of a novelty-preference test indicated that infants hearing the word detected the category, while those hearing the tone did not (Balaban & Waxman, 1997). The same pattern of results emerged in a different task, using a face-to-face object-examining procedure and more complex sounds: 9-month-olds successfully formed categories when objects were named, but not when they were presented with a melody or mouth sounds (Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003). In sum, the facilitative effect of novel words on categorization is robust in infants as young as 9 to 12 months of age. And although there are circumstances in which non-linguistic stimuli (e.g., tones, beeps, gestures) do promote object categorization, a careful review of the evidence reveals that this is the case only when interpersonal, social, and contextual cues are sufficiently strong to convey to the infant that the non-linguistic stimulus is being offered as a name (Namy, 2001; Namy & Waxman, 1998; Woodward & Hoyne, 1999). This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grant #HD045296-01 to the first author. We thank the infants and parents who generously participated in this research as well as Kristin Szymanowski and Julie Hupp for their assistance with data collection.

In the present study, we pursued the evolution of the noun-object category link by comparing the influence of novel words vs. novel tones on object categorization in a novelty-preference task. We considered infants at two strategically-selected developmental points. To capture the very commencement of word learning, we included infants at 6 months, when infants first begin to a) comprehend highly familiar words such as mommy and daddy (Tincoff & Jusczyk, 1999) and b) reveal a preference for lexical (open-class) over grammatical (closed-class) words (Shi & Werker, 2001). For comparison, we included 12-month-olds, who have typically begun to produce their first words and who have previously demonstrated their sensitivity to a link between words and object categories (Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, in press; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995). Half of the infants at each age were randomly assigned to either a Word or Tone condition. This design permitted us to ask a) whether words promote categorization in infants at both ages, and b) whether this effect is specific to words. For the 12-month-olds, our predictions were clear: We expected that novel words but not tones would facilitate the formation of object categories (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Waxman & Markow, 1995; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, 2003, in press). For the 6-month-olds, we reasoned as follows. If novel words have conceptual consequences just as soon as infants begin to parse them from the speech stream, then words should facilitate the formation of object categories at 6 months. If this is a general effect, then tones (like words) should promote object categorization. However, if words play a unique conceptual role from the start, then tones should not promote object categorization. 2. Method 2.1. Participants The final sample included a total of 64 six- and twelve-month-old infants. There were 19 male and 13 female 6-month-olds (M = 184.3 days, range = 167 to 199 days), and 12 male and 20 female 12-month-olds (M = 366.7 days, range = 356 to 377 days). All infants were full-term, healthy, learning English as their first language, and free from any known vision or hearing problems. Infants were identified from county birth records in the greater Toledo, Ohio area, and were predominantly from white middle-class families: 84.3% White Non-Hispanic, 8.3% Multi-Racial, 3.2% Black Non-Hispanic, 3.2% Hispanic, and 1% unknown race/ethnicity; 95.8% of mothers and 86.5% of fathers had at least some college education; 58.3% of mothers and 93.8% of fathers were employed. Parents of infants were first sent a letter, and then contacted by phone. A parent or legal guardian of each participating infant gave both verbal and written informed consent. One additional 6-month-old was tested but not included in the final sample due to fussiness. 2.2. Stimuli 2.2.1. Visual Stimuli Visual stimuli included 20 slides of line-drawn dinosaurs and fish, shown in Figure 1. All stimuli were outlined in black, filled with a solid color, and presented on a white background. There were two familiarization sets of eight stimuli each and two test sets of two stimuli each. Stimuli within a familiarization set varied in color; stimuli within a test set were identical in color. Although the exact sizes varied, stimuli averaged 14.5 cm in width (SD = 2.3 cm) and 9.7 cm in height (SD = 2.0 cm). The average angular size was 13.3 by 9.0 degrees. 2.2.2. Auditory Stimuli The auditory stimuli consisted of two sets of labeling phrases ( Oh look, it s a toma! Do you see the toma? and Oh look, it s a modi! Do you see the modi? ) and two sequences of pure tones (400 Hz, 800 Hz). The labeling phrases were spoken by a female using characteristics of infant-directed speech (high pitch, exaggerated pitch contours; Fernald & Thomas, 1984). The tone sequences were created using Adobe Audition 1.0 and were matched to the labeling phrases in total length (3.4 s) as well as the duration of each syllable and pause. All auditory stimuli were recorded onto audiotape and played at a volume of approximately 92 db.

Figure 1. Representation of familiarization and test stimuli. Familiarization Trial 1 Familiarization Trial 1 Familiarization Trial 2 Familiarization Trial 2 Familiarization Trial 3 Familiarization Trial 3 Familiarization Trial 4 Familiarization Trial 4 Familiarization Trial 5 Familiarization Trial 5 Familiarization Trial 6 Familiarization Trial 6 Familiarization Trial 7 Familiarization Trial 7 Familiarization Trial 8 Familiarization Trial 8 Test Trial 2.3. Apparatus Infants were seated on a parent s lap in a three-sided observation chamber, with its interior walls and ceiling covered in a medium-gray material. The rear wall of the observation chamber contained two 18 by 18 cm (16.4 by 16.4 degrees visual angle) projection screens positioned 61 cm from infants eye level. The lateral separation between screens was 30 cm (26.2 degrees). A video camera located behind the rear wall of the observation chamber recorded infants through a 6 cm hole centered 11 cm below the projection screens. Visual stimuli were presented by slide projector; auditory stimuli were played on a tape player and presented through a hidden speaker centered 58 cm below the two screens. A computer equipped with an input/output buffer system timed trial durations, recorded fixation times, operated the slide projectors, and controlled the output of sound to the speakers.

2.4. Procedure Infants were tested using a visual familiarization/novelty-preference procedure. During the familiarization phase, infants were shown eight different exemplars from a given category (e.g., dinosaurs), one at a time, in random order, for a fixed duration of 20 s each. On each familiarization trial, a single stimulus exemplar was displayed on one of the two projection screens. The lateral position of the familiarization stimulus was determined randomly on the first trial and alternated on each subsequent trial. Infants were assigned to one of two different between-participant sound conditions. In the Word condition, each familiarization exemplar was presented with a labeling phrase (e.g., Oh look, it s a toma! Do you see the toma? ); in the Tone condition familiarization exemplars were presented with a sequence of pure tones. The auditory stimulus was presented during the initial 4 s of each familiarization trial (M = 3.0 s) and once again approximately 3 s later. Timing of the onset of auditory stimuli varied slightly (SD =.65 s) due to variations in the relay control of the cassette player and the time it took the computer to advance the slide projectors. The same auditory stimulus was presented on all eight familiarization trials. Familiarization stimulus set (i.e., dinosaurs, fish) and auditory stimulus were counterbalanced. The test phase immediately followed the familiarization phase and included one 10 s trial. To ensure that infants looked at the test stimuli, timing of the trial did not commence until the onset of infants first visual fixation. During the test trial, infants were presented with one of two different pairs of novel stimuli, side-by-side, in the absence of any added sound. One test stimulus in the pair was from the familiarized category (e.g., a new dinosaur) and one was from the contrasting category (e.g., a fish). The test stimulus pair to which infants were assigned and the lateral position of the stimuli were completely counterbalanced in the design. Parents were not told the hypotheses of the experiment and were instructed not to talk or to influence their infant s attention in any way during either phase of the procedure. Infants visual fixations were coded from videotape by one of 15 trained observers who were blind to the hypotheses of the experiment as well as to the specific visual stimuli presented. Observers recorded the total amount of time that infants spent looking at a stimulus on a given trial. Eight different observers re-rated 25% of the infants in each age and sound condition. Reliability between observers during the test phase was r =.99. 3. Results We calculated a novelty preference score for each infant by dividing total looking to the out-of-category exemplar by total looking to the out-of-category and within-category exemplars combined. If infants detect the categorical relation among the stimuli, then they should look longer at the out-of-category test stimulus than the within-category one, resulting in a novelty preference score greater than a chance value of.50. Our predictions for the test trial were as follows. If words and tones both promote object categorization, then infants should reveal a novelty preference in both the Word and Tone conditions. If words are specifically linked to object categories, then infants should exhibit a novelty preference in the Word condition but not in the Tone condition. Based on the existing evidence with 9-month-olds (Balaban & Waxman, 1997), we expected 12-montholds to demonstrate categorization in the Word condition but not in the Tone condition. For 6-month-olds, our predictions were less firm. If words and object categories are not yet specifically linked, infants should categorize successfully in the Word and Tone conditions. However, if the link is already tied specifically to names, then infants should categorize successfully in the Word but not the Tone condition. Novelty preference scores for each age and condition are depicted in Figure 2. As predicted, 12-month-olds demonstrated reliable novelty preferences in the Word condition (M =.60, SD =.16; t(15) 2.29, p =.02), but performed at chance in the Tone condition (M =.52, SD =.15, t(15) =.40, p =.35). This constitutes a strong replication and extension of previously published work (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003, in press; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995).

Figure. 2. Mean novelty preference scores (+SE) during the test trial for each age and condition..75.70 * Word Tone Mean Novelty Preference Score.65.60.55.50 *.45 12-Month-Olds 6-Month-Olds Strikingly, an identical pattern of results emerged for 6-month-olds. Six-month-olds in the Word condition demonstrated a reliable novelty preference (M =.67, SD =.22, t(15) = 3.11, p =.004), while 6-month-olds in the Tone condition performed at chance (M =.57, SD =.20; t(15) = 1.37, p =.10). An analysis of individual infants performance was consistent with the group data. We compared the number of infants in each age and condition who exhibited a novelty preference with the number that would be expected by chance. Twelve 12-month-olds exhibited a novelty preference in the Word condition (p =.04), but only 8 did so in the Tone condition (p =.50). Likewise, fourteen 6-month-olds revealed a novelty preference in the Word condition (p =.002), but only eleven 6-month-olds did so in the Tone condition (p =.11). 4. Discussion The current experiment offers two insights into the evolution of the link between object naming and categorization. First, the results reveal that this link between naming and categorization is in place as early as 6 months of age. This is the first demonstration of its kind at this young age. Coupled with previous evidence from 9- and 12-month-old infants (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, 2003, in press; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995), this finding calls into serious question the claim that infants enter the process of word-learning without any a priori linkages between word meanings and conceptual organization (Roberts, 1997; Smith, 1999). Instead, the results suggest that words and object categories are linked even as infants first cross the threshold into word comprehension 6 months (Tincoff & Jusczyk, 1999). Second, the results suggest that there is some degree of specificity to this linkage by 6 months of age: 6- month-olds hearing words formed object categories while those hearing tone sequences did not. This finding suggests that 6-month-olds have made some differentiation in how they link linguistic and non-linguistic stimuli to object categories. However, additional research will be necessary to identify more precisely the scope of this early link. For example, in previous work with infants 9 months of age and older, a wide range of non-linguistic sounds have failed to promote object categorization, suggesting that by 9 months, the link may be specific to linguistic stimuli (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2003). In the present study, the non-linguistic stimuli were pure tone sequences. It will be necessary to test 6-month-olds with a wider range of non-linguistic sounds before we can ascertain how specific this link is in 6-month-olds. Additional research is also needed to determine whether this link between words and object categories is specifically tied to object names. We know that by 12 months this is

indeed the case phrases containing object names facilitate categorization (e.g., Look at the dax ), but general orienting phrases (e.g., Look at what s here ) do not (Fulkerson & Haaf, 1998, in press; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Waxman & Markow, 1995). Whether this is also the case for 6-month-olds remains an open question, and one that we are currently pursuing in our labs. Whatever its initial scope, we propose that the link between object naming and categorization supports both word-learning and conceptual organization, and is an important building block in early language acquisition. From the very onset of lexical acquisition, words and object categories are powerfully linked. This link serves three fundamental functions (Waxman, 1998). First, it helps to guide infants attention to just those aspects of the environment that they should be attending to for the rapid acquisition of words object categories. Second, it promotes conceptual development through the efficient organization of information about objects into cognitivelymanageable categories. Third, it lays the foundation for the acquisition of more fine-tuned linkages between particular types of words (e.g., count nouns, adjectives) and particular types of conceptual relations among objects (object categories, object properties). References Balaban, M. T., & Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 3-26. Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2002). Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. Developmental Psychology, 38, 948-957. Fernald, A., & Thomas, S. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20, 104-113. Fulkerson, A. L., & Haaf, R. A. (1998). New words for new things: The relationship between novel labels and 12- month-olds categorization of novel objects. Poster session presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Atlanta, GA. Fulkerson, A. L., & Haaf, R. A. (2003). The influence of labels, non-labeling sounds, and source of auditory input on 9- and 15-month-olds object categorization. Infancy, 4,349-369. Fulkerson, A. L., & Haaf, R. A. (in press). Does object naming aid 12-month-olds formation of novel object categories? First Language. Graham, S. A., Kilbreath, C. S., & Welder, A. N. (2004). 13-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development, 75, 409-427. Namy, L. (2001). What s in a name when it isn t a word? 17-month-olds mapping of nonverbal symbols to object categories. Infancy, 2, 73-86. Namy, L., & Waxman, S. R. (1998). Words and gestures: Infants interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development, 69, 295-308. Roberts, K. (1997). Linguistic and nonlinguistic factors influencing infant categorization: Studies of the relationship between cognition and language. In C. Rovee-Collier & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 11, pp. 45-108). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Shi, R., & Werker, J. F. (2001). Six-month-old infants preference for lexical words. Psychological Science, 12, 70-75. Smith, L. B. (1999). Children s noun learning: How general learning processes make specialized learning mechanisms. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.) The emergence of language. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10, 172-175. Waxman, S. R. (1998). Linking object categorization and naming: Early expectations and the shaping role of language. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 38, 249-291. Waxman, S. R., & Braun, I. E. (2005). Consistent (but not variable) names as invitations to form object categories: new evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognition, 95, B59-B68. Waxman, S. R., & Lidz, J. (in press). Early word learning. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology, 6 th Edition Volume 2. Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-monthold infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257-302. Woodward, A. L., & Hoyne, K. L. (1999). Infants learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. Child Development, 70, 65-77.