Subject Verb Agreement Asymmetries and the Distribution of Labor between Syntax and Morphology

Similar documents
Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

On the Notion Determiner

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Som and Optimality Theory

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Control and Boundedness

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

LIN 6520 Syntax 2 T 5-6, Th 6 CBD 234

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Words come in categories

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

The Inclusiveness Condition in Survive-minimalism

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANITIES & SOCIAL STUDIES

A Computational Evaluation of Case-Assignment Algorithms

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Argument structure and theta roles

Intervention in Tough Constructions * Jeremy Hartman. Massachusetts Institute of Technology

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Compositional Semantics

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

In Udmurt (Uralic, Russia) possessors bear genitive case except in accusative DPs where they receive ablative case.

FOCUS MARKING IN GREEK: SYNTAX OR PHONOLOGY? Michalis Georgiafentis University of Athens

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Pronominal doubling in Dutch dialects: big DPs and coordinations

Writing a composition

Second Language Acquisition of Complex Structures: The Case of English Restrictive Relative Clauses

A comment on the topic of topic comment

Language contact in East Nusantara

Gender and defaults *

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Lecture 9. The Semantic Typology of Indefinites

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Advanced Grammar in Use

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Routledge Library Editions: The English Language: Pronouns And Word Order In Old English: With Particular Reference To The Indefinite Pronoun Man

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

An Interface between Prosodic Phonology and Syntax in Kurdish

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Backward Raising. Eric Potsdam and Maria Polinsky. automatically qualify as covert movement. We exclude such operations from consideration here.

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

UC Berkeley Berkeley Undergraduate Journal of Classics

linguist 752 UMass Amherst 8 February 2017

When a Complement PP Goes Missing: A Study on the Licensing Condition of Swiping

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Feature-Based Grammar

Author: Fatima Lemtouni, Wayzata High School, Wayzata, MN

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Linguistic Inquiry, Volume 35, Number 1, Winter 2004, pp (Article)

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

On Labeling: Principle C and Head Movement

Accurate Unlexicalized Parsing for Modern Hebrew

Transcription:

Subject Verb Agreement Asymmetries and the Distribution of Labor between Syntax and Morphology Martin Walkow mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 1 Agreement Asymmetries in Modern Standard Arabic 1.1 The Positional Asymmetry (PA) Preverbal full NP subjects control verbal agreement in number (NUM) and gender (GEND): (1a) (full agreement). Postverbal full DP subjects control verbal agreement only in GEND, (1b) (partial agreement). (1) a. Pal-Paulæ:d-u qadim{*-a/ -u:} DEF 1 -boys-nom came{-3sg.ma/ -3PL.MA} The boys came. b. qadim{-a/ *-u:} Pal-Paulæ:d-u came{-3sg.ma/ -3PL.MA} DEF-boys-NOM The boys came. (Harbert and Bahloul, 2001, 45) 1.1.1 The Status of Preverbal Subjects A popular Idea: preverbal subjects are topics 2 related to pronouns that are the grammatical subjects of the clause and give rise to full agreement. Example (1a) would receive the analysis in (2). I thank Mohammed Hassan, Sam Al-Khatib and Islam Youssef for discussing the data with me and providing judgements. 1 Abbreviations in Glosses: 1, 2, 3: first second and third person, ACC: accusative, COMP: complementizer, DEF: definite marker, FE: feminine, GEN: genitive, IND: indefinite, MA: masculine, NEG: negation, NOM: nominative, PL: plural, PRT: particle, Q: question marker, SG: singular. 2 I use the term topic here for convenience, other terms have been used for this construction. Moutaouakil (1989) calls them theme, Soltan (2007) left dislocated. (2) Topic Pal-Paulæ:d-u DEF-boys-NOM agree Verb qadim-u: came-3pl.ma Subject hum (silent) 3PL.MA e.g. Fassi Fehri (1988, 2009), Plunkett (1993), Soltan (2007) (see Plunkett, 1993, 241, for references to earlier accounts in this tradition). MSA independently has a construction topic construction, (3), where... there is a resumptive pronoun in the clause, -hu in (3a). the sentence initial noun phrase is always in nominative case, (3a), leading to case mismatch between it and the pronoun in the clause. the sentence initial noun phrases cannot be indefinite, (3b). (3) a. Pal-kItæ:b-u DEF-book-NOM case mismatch qarapa read.3sg.ma -hu zaid-un -3SG.MA.ACC Zayd-NOM-IND The book, Zayd read it. b. * kitæ:b-un qarapa -hu zaid-un book-nom.ind read.3sg.ma -3SG.MA.ACC Zayd-NOM.IND The book, Zayd read it. (Soltan, 2007, 51) If preverbal subjects are topics, we understand why they... appear in nominative case. seem to have a particular discourse function. cannot be indefinite: (4). (4) * walad-un kasara l-baab-a boy-nom.ind broke.3s.ma DEF-door-ACC A boy broke the door. (Soltan, 2007, 51) Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 1

An example where that seems right: NPs to the left of wh-phrases in (5), bind a resumptive in the position they associate with (e.g. Moutaouakil, 1989; Plunkett, 1993). (5) Pa Q t:ul:a:b-u PeIna ya-drus-u:na DEF.students-NOM where 3-studying-MA.PL The students where are (they) studying? (Plunkett, 1993, 243) The Topic proposal in summary: Syntax: silent pronoun in subject position controls full agreement. The major motivation for this comes from the fact that we (i) can observe that pronoun in sentences like (3a), and (ii) pronominal subjects can go unpronounced. Discourse function: Preverbal subjects have a particular discourse status (for a discussion see Moutaouakil, 1989, Chapter 3). Beyond the specifics, one might relate preverbal subjects to givenness, and givenness to pronoun like behavior of agreement. To serve as a general explanation for PA on the syntactic side, all preverbal subjects that control agreement have to be related to a lower pronoun. To serve as an explanation in terms of discourse function, all preverbal subjects would have to have a special discourse status. I will argue that neither the syntactic nor the discourse side can be upheld. 1.1.2 Non-Topic Preverbal Subjects Focus Preposing. Contrastively focussed elements can appear in preverbal position (see e.g. Moutaouakil, 1989, Chapter 2). Focus preposed phrases... Do not bind a resumptive pronouns (Moutaouakil, 1989, 96). Retain their case (Moutaouakil, 1989, 52), accusative in (6). Can be indefinite (Moutaouakil, 1989, 87, on nominal sentences), (7). (6) zaid-an Q safaè-tu Zaid-ACC.IND greeted-1.sg It was Zayd that I greeted. (Moutaouakil, 1989, 24) (7) Pa Q walad-un Ãæ:Pa boy-nom.ind came.3sg.ma Did a boy come? (Mohammad, 1999, 37) Preverbal noun phrases in questions with Pa are contrastively focussed (e.g. Moutaouakil, 1989, 28), (8). (8) Pa Q zaid-an ta-ntaqidu Zayd-ACC.IND 2MA.S-criticising Are your criticizing Zayd? (Moutaouakil, 1989, 30) Unlike topics, (9), contrastively focussed constituents cannot precede Pa, (10). (9) zaid-un Pa Pabu: -hu mari: Q d-un Zayd-NOM.IND Q father.nom -his ill-ma.nom.ind Zayd is his father ill? (Moutaouakil, 1989, 30) (10) * Q amr-an Pa rapai-ta c Amr-ACC.IND Q saw-2s.ma Was it c Amr you saw? (Moutaouakil, 1989, 63) Focus preposed noun phrases differ syntactically as well as in their discourse function from topics as discussed above. Most importantly, they do not involve resumption, which underlies syntactic the explanation of PA in (5), and aren t topical or even given. Focus preposing of subjects still gives rise to full agreement, (11). (11) Pa Q Pawalæ:d-un Ãæ:P{ *-a boy-nom.ind come.perf{ *-3S.M Did boys come? Questions with Paj:. Questions with Paj:... / -u: / -3P.M Do not necessarily involve resumption, (12). Display normal case on Paj:, accusative in (12). Their discourse properties are obviously different from topics. } } Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 2

(12) Paj:at-a madi:nat-in zur-ta which.fe-acc city.fe-gen.ind visited-3sg.fe Which city did you visit? (Krahl et al., 1995, 211) Subject questions with Paj: trigger plural agreement in preverbal position, (13). (13) [Paij-u Paulæ:d-In] [which-nom boys-gen.ind] full agreement ja-ãibu 3S.MA-must Pan ju-sæ:fir-u: PRT 3-travel-PL.MA Which boys have to travel? (Lit. Which boys is it necessary that travel? ) (Mohammad, 1999, 100) Preverbal Subjects after wa and fa. Subjects can appear in preverbal position following wa and fa, (14). (14) a. hada wahm-un fa-kati:ru-:na this fantasy-nom.ind mina r:iãæ:l-i on DEF.men PRT-3-tell lies-ma.pl ja-kdab-u:na Qala: r:iãæ:l-i This is pure fantasy, for many men tell lies to other men. (Badawi et al., 2004, 552) b. wa-hwa læ: ja-bdu: mutafæ:pil-an and-he NEG 3SG.MA-seem.IMP.IND optimistic-acc.ind Are these topics? He does not seem optimistic. (Ryding, 2005, 299) Pa follows topics, (9). wa follows Pa, (15). (15) Pa-wa Ãæ:Pa Pal-walad-u Q-and came.3sg.ma DEF-boy-NOM And did the boy come? (Mohammad, 1999, 36) Since the preverbal nominatives in (14) follow wa, they cannot be topics. Discourse prominence: Topics are typically phonologically prominent. The pronouns huwa and hija can be phonologically reduced after wa/fa to wahwa/fahwa and wahja/fahja (Ryding, 2005, 299), (14b). If the absence of phonological prominence is indicative of discourse prominence, pronominal subjects after wa/fa can lack discourse prominence (observed for classical Arabic by Reckendorf, 1895, 379-380). While wa and fa do mark discourse relations between sentences, the descriptions in Badawi et al. (2004) and Ryding (2005) do not suggest that they are consistently associated with discourse prominence of the subject. Preverbal Subjects in Compound Tense Constructions. In compound tense constructions two verbs agree with the same subject, each shows agreement according to its position with respect to the subject: (16). (16) V 1 kæ:na S Pal-Paulæ:d-u V 2 ja-tara:ki Q d-u:na was.3sg.ma DEF-children-NOM 3-race around-pl.ma The children were racing around. (Badawi et al., 2004, 368) The interverbal position in (16) is not the base position of the subject. Indefinite subjects, e.g. (17)-(18), can appear below the lower verb. In that position they expectably do not trigger number agreement, (17). (17) [...] kæ:na qad ma Q da: qarn-æ:ni [... ] be.perf.3sg.ma PRT pass.perf.3sg.ma century-dual.nom Qala mis & ra (18) kæ:mil-æ:ni whole-dual.nom Qala: on èamlat-i attack-gen næ:bulju:na Napoleon on Egypt [... ] two whole centuries had passed over Napoleons attack on Egypt. (Badawi et al., 2004, 368) V 1 lam ja-kun NEG 3SG.MA-was O tuf:æ:èat-an apple-acc.ind No boy had eaten an apple. V 2 qad Pakala S [Paj:-u walad-in] PRT ate.3sg.ma any-nom boy-gen.ind The asymmetry between (16) and (17)/(18) mirrors that of (1a) and (1b). Intuitively the subject in (16) is not a topic like the one in (5): (i) It appears in a different position, (ii) It does not have the same discourse status. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 3

1.1.3 Summary Summing up: Not all preverbal nominatives have the same syntactic status. Not all of them are related to a resumptive pronoun. Not all preverbal subjects have the same discourse status. Subjects assume preverbal positions in a number of different ways giving rise to full agreement. Some of these ways are obviously driven by the discoure function of the subject (focus preposing, questions) others less obviously so (wa/fa and compound tense constructions). There is no consistent association with givenness. Even outside constructions like (5), full agreement is a general fact about how agreement interacts with word order and is not tied to any particular discourse function or factor that drives SV-order. PA in MSA differs in important ways from positional asymmetries in other languages, e.g. the Italian dialects discussed in Brandi and Cordin (1989): There is agreement in gender with the postverbal subject (see 1.3), unlike in these Italian dialects where there is no agreement in VS-order at all. All preverbal subjects trigger full agreement in MSA. In the Italian dialects there is no agreement in subject questions where the question word is preverbal. Questions: Why are number and gender agreement sensitive to different structural configurations? Why is it number agreement that fails in postverbal position, not gender agreement? And why does gender agreement succeed in postverbal position? 1.2 The Subject-type Asymmetry (SA) I follow (Bahloul and Harbert, 1992; Harbert and Bahloul, 2001) in assuming that forms like huwa/hija/hum/pantum... are normal pronouns (contra Fassi Fehri, 1993, among others). Pronouns, unlike full noun phrases, trigger agreement in NUM and GEND in preverbal as well as postverbal position, (1b) vs. (19). (19) Ãæ:P{*-a came{*-3sg.ma / -u:} / -3.PL.MA} hum læ: xud:æ:m-u-hum they NEG servants-nom-their They came, not their servants. (Harbert and Bahloul, 2001, 63) Number agreement is sensitive to the syntactic category of the subject. The role of person: Third person is the default specification (e.g. in contexts where agreement fails (20)) altogether, so the presence of person agreement is not detectable with third person subjects. With 1/2-pronouns there is agreement with more features anyway, so one can t tell whether person agreement is possible for the same reason number agreement is possible. I will not have anything to say about person agreement. 1.3 Absence of Gender Agreement GEND agreement can be absent in V... -S order when material like prepositional phrases, (20a-c), or existential there/hunæ:ka, (20d), are between verb and subject. (20) a. kæ:n{-a was{-3sg.ma / -at / -3SG.FE The boys had a car. (Soltan, 2007, 111) } QInda-l-Pawlæ:d-I saj:a:rat-un } with-def-boys-gen car-nom.ind b. leis{-a / -at } fi:-l-beit-i wasn t{-3sg.ma / -3SG.FE } in-def-house-gen PImraPat-un woman-nom.ind There was no woman in the house. c. {ya- / ta-}ãibu Qala:-Pal-muPmIn-i:na {3SG.MA- / 3SG.FE-}MOD on-def-believer-pl.ma.gen Pa Q s:ala:t-u DEF.prayer-NOM The believers have to pray. (Lit. Prayer must on the believers) (a.-c. Soltan, 2007, 109) Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 4

d. kæ:n-a hunæ:ka muèæ:walæ:t-un li was-3sg.ma there attempts.fe-nom.ind to [... ] [... ] There were attempts to [... ] (Badawi et al., 2004, 317) The nature and extent of the optionality is yet to be understood. Agreement is mostly obligatory when subject and verb are adjacent. Parkinson (2008) a set of exceptions involving nominalizations. 1.4 Agreement with Multiple Verbs Multiple verbs can agree in one sentence, (16) and (17)/(18). There is no dependency between the agreement features on the two verbs, unlike what as has been argued for complementizer-agreement and verbal agreement in other languages (Zwart, 1997; Chomsky, 2005): The verbs can show different φ-features: (16). In interaction with left-conjunct agreement in VS-order, they can express disjoint sets of agreement features: (21) (not acceptable to all speakers). agrees (21) V 1 kæ:n-at [NP i Puxt-i: was.3sg.fe sister.fe-my CONJ wa and NP j ] ConjP Pantum you.pl.ma My sister and you.pl were racing around. agrees V 2 ta-tara:ki Q d-u:na 2-race around-pl.ma In multiple nominative constructions (Doron and Heycock, 1999), the two verbs can agree with different nominative NPs: (22). agrees agrees (22) V 1 Dal: 1 -a NP i [hæ:sim-un] i V 2 ta-taqad:amu 2 kept-3s.ma Hashim-NOM 3S.FE-progress PP-pro i bi-hi i on-him NP j [Pas:In:-u] j DEF.years.FE-NOM Hashim was growing old. (Lit. The years keep progressing on Hashim. ) (Cantarino, 1975, 265) The masculine on Q Dal:a in (22) is not default agreement. When NP i is replaced by a feminine noun, (23), only feminine agreement is possible. See (23) Q Dal:{ *-a/ -at } zeinab-u ta-taqad:amu bi-hæ: kept{ *-3SG.MA/ -3SG.FE } Zainab.FE-NOM 3SG.FE-progressing on-her Pas:In:-u DEF.years.FE-NOM Zainab was growing old. Dissociation of the agreement properties of two agreeing elements is also found in cases of complementizer agreement in Dutch dialects discussed by Haegeman and van Koppen (2009). The resumptive bound by hæ:simun gives this structure the resemblance of topics like (5). Doron and Heycock (1999) argue that MSA has broad subjects, nominatives that are base generated higher than normal subjects and control resumptives, but are different from topics. They for example do not appear in the left periphery of the sentence. I assume that hæ:simun is such a broad subject. No split agreement: V 2 can agree with a higher S, (16). Both NP 1 and NP 2 are possible agreement targets, (22). In constructions like (22), V 2 (tataqad:amu) cannot agree in gender with NP 2 but in number with NP 1, schematically in (24). *[NUM] [GEND] (24) V 1 NP i V 2 PP-pro i NP j The NUM- and GEND-features have to come from the same agreement target. In this respect MSA differs from Georgian (Béjar, 2000), where the same agreement marker can reflect the features of different agreement targets. The possibility of multiple heads agreeing with the subject is a problem in the face of the Activity Condition on AGREE (Chomsky, 2000). Agreement with the lower verb in (16) involves all φ-features. This should deactivate the subject s φ-features and bar agreement with higher heads. 1.5 Conjunct Agreement Preverbal conjoined subjects trigger agreement with the whole conjunction. Mixed gender conjunctions default to masculine, (25). Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 5

(25) Hind-u wa Hind.FE-NOM and -æ: } -3.DUAL } Zayd-un Zayd.MA-NOM Ãæ:P{ *-at / *-a / come.perf{ *-3SG.FE / *-3SG.MA / Hind and Zayd came. (Soltan, 2007, 80) Postverbal conjoined subjects show agreement with the first conjunct, (26). This is independent of whether the first conjunct is a pronoun or a full noun phrase, (26) a. vs b. (26) a. Ãæ:P{ -at / *-a / *-æ: } Hind-u wa come.perf{ -3SG.FE / *-3SG.MA / *-3.DUAL } Hind.FE-NOM and Zayd-un Zayd.MA-NOM Hind and Zayd came. b. {ÃIP -na / {come.perf -3PL.FE / Pæ:bæ:P-u fathers-nom -hun:a -their.fe *Ãæ:P -u: } *come.perf -3PL.MASC } ] ] They and their fathers came. (Soltan, 2007, 80-81) [hun:a wa [they.fe and Full agreement in preverbal and left conjunct agreement in postverbal position arise with the same subject in compound tense constructions, (21). 1.6 The Proposal in Short The phenomena to be accounted for are summarized in Tab. 1. Proposal: The asymmetries arise because (i) sharing of φ-features can happen in syntax as well as PF, (ii) the relevant processes in syntax and PF are sensitive to different structural relations, and (iii) Agreement processes are sensitive to the internal structure of the agreement target. Syntax: AGREE establishes relations between probes and goals. PF: matches feature under structural adjacency (Adger, 2000), allowing functional structure inside a specifier to interact with a following head. PA: SA: Absence of GEND: Multiple Agreement: No Split Agreement: Conjunct Agreement: Number and gender agreement are sensitive to different structural configurations, (1a) vs (1b). Agreement in VS-order is sensitive to the syntactic structure of the subject, (1b) vs (19). In a limited set of contexts, GEND-agreement is optional under non-adjacency, (20). The subject can be target of multiple agreement relations, (16). φ-features of one probe always come from the same goal, (24). Postverbal conjoined subjects trigger agreement with the leftmost conjunct. Preverbal conjoined subjects trigger full agreement. Table 1: Summary of Phenomena. AGREE happening before PF operations restricts the application of matching at PF. The proposal here continues work (e.g. Benmamoun, 2000; van Koppen, 2007; Benmamoun et al., 2009) that investigates how syntactic and PF factors contribute to agreement phenomena. Logical Form (Interpretation) Lexicon Narrow Syntax Morphology Phonological Form (Pronounciation) Table 2: The Y-model of the grammar. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 6

DP Origin of definiteness marker Pal (comp. Ritter, 1995, for Hebrew). a. Pronoun in VS-order: b. Full DP in VS-order: D 0 φ[num] φp DEM 0 DEMP FP AP. NP [GEND] Agreement projection introducing NUM (Ritter, 1995), and mediating DP internal NUM/GEND-concord (Bernstein, 2001). Projection introducing demonstratives (Bernstein, 1997). Functional projection(s) introducing adjuncts. Locus of gender features (Ritter, 1995). [ φpr+v ] YP GEND: [ ] NUM : GEND φp NUM AGREE [ ] GEND NP φ NUM... Y... [ φpr+v ] YP GEND: NUM : DP AGREE * [GEND] [ ] D [GEND] φp GEND NUM NP [GEND] Y... Table 3: Internal structure of DP 2 Subject Type Asymmetry Proposal: φ-agreement has access only to the features in the highest structural layer of the agreement target. Pronouns and full DPs in MSA differ in the φ- features accessible in their highest syntactic layers as a function of differences in internal structure. Assumptions about internal structure of DP: Tab. 3. Nouns surface between demonstratives and adjective, (27). (27) hæ:da this Pal 3 -wæ:qiq-u Pal-mutaöaIj:Ir-u DEF-reality-NOM DEF-changing-NOM this changing reality (Badawi et al., 2004, 127) D is not born with either GEND- or NUM-features. It has to acquire them from somewhere in its complement. Proposal: D in MSA acquires its φ-features in a relation with NP. NP has GEND but no NUM-features. When external agreement triggers access D, they only have access to GEND. 3 I treat the definiteness markers as exponents of D, see Table 3, spelled out on all terminals of the appropriate type in its c-command domain. This captures that fact that they don t appear on nouns that have raised to D, see (35). Table 4: Immediately postverbal subjects. Evidence about the relation between D and its complement comes from two sources: morphology and movement. See Appendix A.2 for more detailed discussion. This analysis assumes that features can be shared between two nodes (i) in the syntax, and (ii) in a way that the the features on D in this case remain accessible for later applications of AGREE. More discussion of this point in Sec. 3 and 5. Pronouns: Déchaine and Wiltschko (2002) argue that pronouns come in three different sizes: DP, φp and NP, each with different properties. MSA pronouns have the properties of φp-pronouns (See Appendix A.1). As φp contains NUM and GEND, both are accessible to agreement, giving rise to full agreement. In combination with AGREE SA is derived as in Tab. 4: Pronouns: φpr+v consisting of GEND and NUM can access φ(p) and AGREE in both features. Full DPs: A probe on φpr+v consisting of both NUM and GEND cannot access the features in φp due to the intervening GEND features on D, which it AGREEs with in stead. Partial agreement follows. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 7

For the AGREE-account to work, checking only GEND has to be sufficient for the probe as well as the goal to be licensed. Failure to check NUM does not lead to a crash in the syntax. In addition, the φ-features of non-thematic nominatives as in (22) have to be accessible to AGREE. 3 Positional Asymmetry AGREE: If movement is AGREE+EPP, and EPP is an instruction to build structure, there is no reason to expect that AGREE should have access to more features in the presence of EPP than in its absence. Adger (2000): Relations between features can be established in the syntax or at PF. Syntax and PF do this in different structural relations: XP WP Spec W W 0 X X 0 SP Spec S ZP... RP......Z 0... x and y are in an adjacency relation iff Z YP Y...Y 0... Syntax: I adopt AGREE, departing from Adger (2000) where SpecHead is assumed. PF: Feature matching happens under structural adjacency in Tab. 5. When a DP moves into the specifier of a head, the functional structure along the right edge of DP becomes adjacent to that head, compare φp in Tab. 6 and SP in Tab. 5. This allows matching between the GEND and NUM features in the subject s φp and those of the verbal probe. Movement does not cause NUM-agreement. It creates a syntactic configuration that gives PF access to syntactic structures than AGREE cannot access. Disconnecting the mechanics of movement and the mechanics of agreement allows full agreement to be independent of the process that brings about SVorder (see (11) and (13)). This is unlike the Italian dialects discussed in Brandi and Cordin (1989), where some SV-orders feed agreement, but those created by wh-movement don t. Silent copies of moved phrases have to be visible to matching at PF. (13): The subject wh-phrase has moved out of the subordinate clause, and controls number agreement on the verb in the subordinate clause. (i) either the first right-branching node dominating x is the first leftbranching node dominating y (dominance to be understood reflexively) or (ii) x=y Direction of branching here is to be understood relative to the node from which the adjacency relation is calculated. (Adger, 2000) Table 5: Adjacency. For agreement under adjacency to account for this, there has to be a silent copy of the moved subject in preverbal position in the downstairs clause and it has to be a possible matching target at PF. Matching targets need to be restricted: Agreement only with nominatives, never with preverbal objects. One might restrict matching targets to nominatives. Restricting matching to nominatives is not enough to ban split agreement, (24). Adjacency allows the lower verb to access the number features of the structurally higher nominative. Without additional restrictions, this would allow split agreement in VNP NOM VNP NOM -order. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 8

D [GEND] DP[GEND] [ ] GEND φp NUM [ ] GEND φ NUM ZP Match NP[GEND] X [ φpr+v ] WP GEND: NUM :... Table 6: Full agreement via matching in SV-order (to be revised). *[NUM] [GEND] [ φpr+v ] GEND: NUM : D [GEND] AGREE DP[GEND] [ ] GEND φ NUM [ ] GEND φp NUM ZP Match NP[GEND] AGREE X [ φpr+v ] WP GEND: NUM :... (24) V 1 NP i V 2 PP-pro i NP j Observation: Both of these problems disappear once AGREE instead of case serves as a filter on matching targets. If matching only has access to feature bundles that contain a feature that has been the target of AGREE,... Agreement with objects is ruled out by the general mechanisms that make objects inaccessible for agreement in MSA. Split agreement is ruled out, because the φ-probe has not AGREEed with the higher nominative. An example: Tab. 6. φpr has already entered into AGREE with the GENDfeatures in the DP-layer of the subject when the subject was in postverbal position. Copies of these GEND-features are also present in φp. As a result, φpr and φp already share GEND-features, allowing matching of their other features at PF. (see also Tab. 7) Proposal: PF uses feature matching under adjacency to resolve the values of features that have not been valued in the syntax, but are part of the same feature complex as the features that syntax has AGREEd with. PA arises because PF has access to structure inside DPs that AGREE does not have access to. AGREE in the syntax feeds matching in the morphology. Table 7: Agreement with two verbs in VSV-order, (16). Failure to check features in syntax does not lead to crash, as independently argued by Preminger (2009). PF has means of assigning interpretations to features that have been left unchecked in the syntax. 4 Agreement with Multiple Verbs The φ-features on the several agreeing verbs are independent of each other: They show different agreement features, (16) and (21), depending on their local relation to the subject. Two verbs can agree with different nominatives, (22). There is a φ-probe for each verb. The assumptions introduced so far account for agreement with multiple verbs as in Tab. 7. φ-features are accessible for multiple instances of AGREE. They must not delete or become inactive after the lowest φ-probe has AGREEd with them. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 9

Number: Valued and interpretable, they shouldn t delete anyway. Gender: Bošković (2009) observes that not all gender features are treated the same in agreement with conjunctions in Serbo-Croatian. Semantically meaningful gender (on nouns like man and woman) behaves as if it does not delete after AGREE. Feminine behaves differently from masculine. He suggests that this is due to the fact that it is potentially semantically meaningful rather than just an arbitrary grammatical feature. In MSA, only the presence of multiple feminine agreement can be confirmed, as masculine agreement could be default. Feminine agreement is frequently interpretable: On animate nouns it typically indicates feminine sex. Inanimate plurals systematically trigger feminine singular agreement. There are suggestive parallels between Serbo-Croatian and MSA, but I leave this question for further research. See Carstens and Diercks (2009) for related argument that φ-features remain active in some Bantu languages. 5 Absence of GEND-Agreement Absence of agreement has been occasionally suggested (Ouhalla, 1994; Soltan, 2007) to be due to morphological effects. Adjacency as in Tab. 5, however, predicts the wrong pattern of absence of agreement: Agreement should be absent whenever there is material between the subject and the verb, instead it is optionally possible. The possibility of not agreeing seems to be limited. Agreement can arise over intervening material (e.g. (20), V 2 in (22), (28) is a variant of (22) where V 1 agrees with the lower nominative j). (28) V 1 Dal: 1 -at NP i [hæ:sim-un] i V 2 ta-taqad:amu 2 P-pro i bi-hi i NP j Pas:In:-u j kept-3sg.fe Hashim-NOM 3SG.FE-progress on-him DEF.years.FE-NOM Hashim was growing old. (Lit. The years keep progressing on Hashim. ) The presence of agreement in V... S-contexts cannot accounted for by assuming that lower, unpronounced copies of the verb access the features of the subject. For transitive verbs, there will always be a copy of the verb in v that has the subject in its specifier, which should give rise to full agreement. AGREE: Possibility of agreement at a distance is expected. Absence of agreement could be treated as defective intervention, see Preminger (2009) for a discussion of apparently optional agreement in terms of intervention and ways of evading it in Hebrew. This would be a somewhat unusual form of defective intervention as it would involve PPs as interveners in (20a-c). The absence of agreement in existential constructions, (20d), might indicate that person is covertly involved in gender agreement, as existential there is sometimes assumed to be an intervener for person (e.g. Chomsky, 2000). A further difficulty from the perspective of intervention comes from the fact that non-thematic nominatives as in (22) can be agreement targets, but don t prohibit agreement across them with the thematic subject, (28). 6 Conclusions Conjunct Agreement: The current proposal does not give an account of the conjunct agreement facts. An account of left conjunct agreement with full DP conjuncts in VS-order can be given with a combination of Marušič et al. s (2007) argument that gender features cannot be compositionally computed in mixed gender conjunctions. The argument about the distribution of φ-features in DPs from Sec. 2. The assumption that only the features in the highest projections of the conjuncts are accessible for computing the features of CONJP,. van Koppen s (2007) account of left conjunct agreement where AGREE has access to CONJP as well as the first conjunct, and PF chooses which one s features to express based on which will give a more specific morphology Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 10

CONJP has neither GEND nor NUM features, because (i) NUM-features are not present in the D-layer of the conjuncts, hence inaccessible for the computation CONJP s φ-features, and (ii) GEND-features are absent in CONJP at least in mixed gender conjunction because they cannot be compositionally computed. As a result, in mixed gender conjunctions, spelling out the φ-features of the first conjunct always gives rise to more specific morphology because it has at least GEND-features. In same gender conjunctions, both the D of the first conjunct and the CONJP have the same features, only GEND. Spelling out either gives the same result. This line of analysis makes incorrect predictions for coordinations with pronouns. Since the NUM-features of pronouns are accessible in their highest projections, the CONJP of a conjunction of a feminine full DP and a feminine plural pronoun should contain NUM as well as GEND-features and be spelled out qua being more specific that the features of the first conjunct. Full agreement in SV-order is difficult to derive in the current system. A caveat: An account of the conjunct agreement must not be tied too closely to the specifics of MSA. Aoun et al. (1994): Levantine and Moroccan Arabic have the same pattern of conjunct agreement, but lack the positional asymmetry, optionally allow full agreement in postverbal position, and show an interaction between the semantics of the verbal predicate and the availability of conjunct agreement. van Koppen (2007): Some Dutch and German dialects show the same conjunct agreement pattern as MSA, but again their agreement system is quite different. They lack silent pronominal subjects. Agreement asymmetries receive an explanation via the divided and joint labor of syntax and PF. AGREE establishes relations between probes and goals, its result is dependent on the structure of the agreement target, accounting for SA. Feature matching at PF is sensitive to different structural relation, but restrained by AGREE. It provides morphological expression to features of a probe that core syntax did not have access to. Division of labor between syntax and morphology, and close attention to the structure of agreement targets gives a simpler picture of syntactic operations: A Internal Syntax: D-syntax; Morphologically complex Structure: DP Pro-DP: Pro-φP: Pro-NP: D φ φp NP Neither D-Syntax, nor N-Syntax N syntax N Distribution: Argument Argument, Predicate Predicate Semantics: Definite Constant Binding: R-expression Variable Table 8: Déchaine and Wiltschko (2002, 410): Typology of Pronoun Types. The result of AGREE is independent of whether there also is EPP. Differences between pronouns and full DPs follow from differences in their internal structure, not the way the grammar interacts with them (contra previous accounts of SA e.g. Fassi Fehri, 1993; Benmamoun, 2000; Soltan, 2007) Failing to check φ-features in the syntax does not lead to a crash (also Preminger, 2009), but the grammar will use other mechanisms at PF to provide an expression for features that have gone unchecked. φ φp NP Subject Type Asymmetry in More Detail A.1 Pronouns What kind of structure are pronouns in MSA? Déchaine and Wiltschko (2002): Typology of pronouns in Tab. 8. Different types of pronouns have different maximal projections, which determines their differing properties. MSA pronouns are Pro-φP. N NP N Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 11

φp is an agreement projection hosting number and gender features. Pro-φPs... Appear in argument position, (19). Appear in predicate position, (29). Are bindable, (30). (29) Predicate Position: a. ha:da: huwa this pron.3sg.ma This is he. (30) Binding: b. PantI hiya you.sg.fe pron.3sg.fe You are she (Ryding, 2005, 300) Pa-xæ:fu Pana: wa [kul:-u zami:l-in l-i:] 1 Pan 1SG-fear I and [every-nom colleague-gen.ind for-me] 1 COMP Pa- tanæ:fis-a 1SG-compete-SUBJ Pana: I wa and huwa 1 he 1 Qala: for nafs-i same-gen l-wa Q Da:PIf-I DEF-jobs-GEN I and every colleague of mine are afraid that I and he will be competing for the same job. Both GEND and NUM are present in the highest projections of Pro-φP, hence both are accessible to agreement, leading to full agreement. A.2 Full DPs D is not born with either GEND- or NUM-features. It has to acquire them from somewhere in its complement. Proposal: D in MSA acquires its φ-features in a relation with NP. NP has GEND but no NUM-features. Partial agreement results. Evidence about the relation between D and its complement comes from two sources: morphology and movement. A.2.1 Morphology in D Definite and indefinite markers Pal- and -n, show no φ-features (unlike e.g. in German). Most quantifiers show no φ-features (e.g. kul: all, baq Q d some ), at least one has inherent gender specification (kæ:f:at) all, see (31) for a combination with a masculine noun. (31) Pamæ:ma kæ:f:at-i Pal-mustaTmIr-i:na in front of all-gen DEF-investor-MA.PL.GEN in front of all investors (ArabiCorpus, Reference: 070399ECON05) Some quantifiers do show φ-morphology: bidq(at) some, several shows the gender opposite of that of its complement, (32). The opposition of gender agreement, also found with low numerals, remains to be explained, but there is definite evidence for φ- morphology on D, determined by the properties of the nominal complement. PaIj:(at): wh-word which, (33a), free choice item and negative polarity item, (33b). Optionally shows gender concord. (32) a. bidq-at-u Q tul:a:b-in some-fe-nom students.masc-gen.ind some students(masc.) b. bidq-u Q ta:lib-æ:t-in some-nom students-pl.fem-gen.ind some students(fem.) (33) a. PaIj:at-u ra:piè-at-in hæ:dihi which.fe-nom smell-fe-gen.ind this.fe What smell is this? (Badawi et al., 2004, 698) b. læ: tu-ãad-u PaIj:at-u Quru: Q d-in min NEG 3S.FE-exists-IND any.fe-nom goals-gen from (Kremers, 2003, 64) tu:tinhæ:m Tottenham There are no goals from Tottenham. (http://vb.alsultaan.com/175494.html) Importantly: There is concord in gender, but not in number, despite bidq(at) and PaIj:(at) taking plural noun complements, (32) and (33b). A notable exception: kul:, all, has a dual form kul:-æ: meaning both. This dual form also inflects for gender and has a feminine alternate kul:-t-æ:. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 12

Demonstratives agree in number as well as gender with their head nouns. Bernstein (1997) argues that demonstratives are not exponents of D, but originate in a lower projection, see Tab. 3. In this lower position, they can inherit their features from φp. Their morphology is this no evidence about the relation between D and its complement. MSA internal evidence for [φp [DEM... -order comes from the combinations of pronouns and demonstratives. If pronouns like huwa and hija occupy φp, as argued in Sec. A.1, demonstratives should follow them, as they do, (34). (34) a. huwa Dæ: he that.ma that one b. hija Di: she that.fe that one (Wehr, 1979, 354) The morphological evidence suggests that the exponents of D have access to GEND but not NUM-features. In order to have access to GEN to the exclusion of NUM, D has to access NP, rather than φp. A.2.2 Movement into D Construct states: Possessive constructions where the head noun appears to the left of the possessor, (35). The head noun appears without definiteness marking, Parkæ:n-u as opposed to definite Pal-Parkæ:n-u or indefinite Parkæ:n-u-n in (35). These constructions are widely assumed to involve movement of the head noun into the D-domain (see Shlonsky, 2004, for an overview). In some languages this movement results in overt φ-morphology reflecting the φ-features of the possessum (e.g. Carstens, 2001, for Bantu) Since the head noun appears before the possessor, the possessor serves as a gauge for how much of the functional structure between D and N moves into D together with the head noun. (35): Adjectives are stranded by movement into D. (36): Demonstratives are stranded by movement into D. (35) Parkæ:n-u Pal-PIslæ:m-I Pal-xams-at-u pillars.fe-nom DEF-islam.MA-GEN DEF-five-FE-NOM the five pillars of Islam (Ryding, 2005, 213) (36) na Q DarIj:at-u Pat:a Q tauw:ur-i tilka theory.fe-nom DEF.evolution.MA-GEN that.fe that theory of evolution (Badawi et al., 2004, 128) Movement into D strands the functional structure below φp, DEMP and adjectives, and therefore φp itself. The movement target therefore does not contain NUM-features. Names: Longobardi (1994) argues that names move into D. (37): Names appear to the left of demonstratives (37) samqæ:n-un hæ:da Sam an-num.ind that.ma this Sam an (Badawi et al., 2004, 128) Movement of names into D strands DEMP, and by transitivity φp. The movement target therefore does not contain NUM-features. A.2.3 Putting the Pieces Together Morphology on D shows GEND, but never NUM features. Movement into D targets constituents that contain GEND-, but not NUMfeatures. Proposal: D acquires its φ-features in a relation with NP. The connection to movement in Sec. A.2.2 suggests that this might be AGREE. If so the φ-features on D must not delete after being checked, but must remain present be accessed by agreement targets. This issue is related to the persistent presence of φ-features discussed in Sec. 4. If the probe on D was a normal φ-probe comprising GEND and NUM, φp should intervene between D and NP. I suggest instead that the feature on D is a categorical N-feature, a syntactic expression of the relation between D and its complement. The sharing of φ-features between D and N is a byproduct of D checking the N-feature. If on GEND is present on D, DP external agreement controllers only have access to GEN, resulting in partial agreement. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 13

References Adger, D. (2000). Feature checking under adjacency and VSO clause structure. In The nature and function of Syntactic Categories. Academic Press, New York. Aoun, J., Benmamoun, E., and Sportiche, D. (1994). Agreement, word order and conjunction in some varieties of Arabic. Linguistic Inquiry, 25(2):195 220. Badawi, E., Carter, M. G., and Gully, A. (2004). Modern Written Arabic, A comprehensive grammar. Comprehensive Grammars. Routledge, London. Bahloul, M. and Harbert, W. (1992). Agreement asymmetries in Arabic. In Proceedings of the Eleventh West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Béjar, S. (2000). Locality, cyclicity, and Georgian verbal morphology. Ms. University of Toronto. Benmamoun, E. (2000). The Feature Structure of Functional Categories, A comparative study of Arabic dialects. Oxfors Studies in Comparative Syntax. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Benmamoun, E., Bhatia, A., and Polinsky, M. (2009). Closest conjunct agreement in head final languages. ms. LingBuzz. Bernstein, J. B. (1997). Demonstratives and reinforcers in Romance and Germanic languages. Lingua, 102:87 113. Bernstein, J. B. (2001). The DP hypothesis: Indetifying clausal properties in the nominal domain. In Baltin, M. and Collins, C., editors, The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory, chapter 17, pages 536 561. Blackwell. Bošković, Željko. (2009). Unifying first and last conjunct agreement. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 27:455 496. Brandi, L. and Cordin, P. (1989). The Italian dialects and the Null Subject Parameter. In Jaeggli, O. and Safir, K., editors, The Null Subject Parameter, pages 111 142. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Amsterdam. Cantarino, V. (1975). Syntax of Modern Arabic Prose, volume 3. Indiana University Press, Bloomington. Carstens, V. (2001). Multiple agreement anc case seletion: Agains φ-(in)completeness. Syntax, 4(3):147 163. Carstens, V. and Diercks, M. (2009). Parameterizing case theory: Raising verbs in bantu. Talk given at NELS 40, 11/15/2009. Chomsky, N. (2000). Minimalist inquiries. In Martin, R., Michaels, D., and Uriagereka, J., editors, Step by step: Essays on Minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, pages 89 155. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Chomsky, N. (2005). On phases. Ms. MIT. Déchaine, R.-M. and Wiltschko, M. (2002). Decomposing pronouns. LI, 33(3):409 442. Doron, E. and Heycock, C. (1999). Filling and licensing multiple specifiers. In Specifiers. Minimalist Approaches, chapter 4, pages 69 89. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Fassi Fehri, A. (1988). Agreement in arabic, binding and coherence. In Barlow, M. and Ferguson, C. A., editors, Agreement in Natural Language, Approaches, theories, descriptions, chapter 6, pages 107 158. CSLI. Haegeman, L. and van Koppen, M. (2009). The non-existence of a phi-feature dependency between t 0 and c 0. Talk given at NELS 40, 11/13/2009. Harbert, W. and Bahloul, M. (2001). Postverbal subjects in Arabic and the theory of agreement. In Themes in Arabic and Hebrew Sytnax, pages 45 70. Kluwer Academic Publishers. Krahl, G., Reuschel, W., and Schulz, E. (1995). Lehrbuch des modernen Arabisch. Leipzig. Langenschiedt. Kremers, J. M. (2003). The Arabic Noun Phrase. A minimalist approach. PhD thesis, Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen, Nijmegen. Longobardi, G. (1994). Reference and proper names. Linguistic Inquiry, 25:609 665. Marušič, F., Nevins, A., and Saksida, A. (2007). Last-conjunct agreement in Slovenian. In Compton, R., Goledzinowska, M., and Savchenko, U., editors, Annual Workshop on Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The Toronto Meeting 2006, FASL 15, pages 210 227, Ann Arbor. Michigan Slavic Publications. Mohammad, M. A. (1999). Word Order, Agreement and Pronominalization in Standard and Palestinian Arabic. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, Philadelphia. Moutaouakil, A. (1989). Pragmatic Functions in a Functional Grammar of Arabic. Foris Publications, Dordrecht. Ouhalla, J. (1994). Verb movement and word order in arabic. In Lightfoot, D. and Hornstein, N., editors, Verb Movement, chapter 2, pages 41 72. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Parkinson, D. (2008). Sentence subject agreement variation. In Ibrahim, Z. and Makhlouf, S. A. M., editors, Linguistics in an Age of Globalization: Perspectives on Arabic Language and Teaching, pages 67 90. American University Cairo Press, Cairo. Plunkett, B. (1993). The position of subjects in Modern Standard Arabic. In Eid, M. and Holes, C., editors, Perspectives in Arabic Linguistics V, Papers from the fifth annual symposium on Arabic Linguistics, pages 231 260. John Benjamins, Amsterdam. Preminger, O. (2009). Failure to Agree is not a Failure: φ-agreement with Post-Verbal Subjects in Hebrew. Ms. MIT. Reckendorf, H. (1895). Die Syntaktischen Verhältnisse des Arabischen. E. J. Brill. Ritter, E. (1995). On the syntactic category of pronouns and agreement. NLLT, 13:405 443. Ryding, K. C. (2005). A reference grammar of modern standard Arabic. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Shlonsky, U. (2004). The form of Semitic noun phrases. Lingua, 114:1465 1526. Soltan, U. (2007). On Formal Feature Licensing in Minimalism: Aspects of Standard Arabic Morphosyntax. PhD thesis, University of Maryland. van Koppen, M. (2007). Agreement with coordinated subjects. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 7:121 161. Wehr, H. (1979). Arabic-English Dictionary, The Hans Wehr dictionary of Modern Written Arabic. Harrassowitz, Wiesbaden. Zwart, J.-W. (1997). Morphosyntax of Verb Movement. Kluwer, Dordrecht. Fassi Fehri, A. (1993). Issues in the structure of Arabic clauses and words. Kluwer Academic Press, Dortrecht. Fassi Fehri, A. (2009). Arabic silen pronouns, person, and voice. In Brill s Annual of Afroasiatic Linguistics, volume 1. Brill. Martin Walkow, mwalkow@linguist.umass.edu 14