Corrections to and clarifications of the Seri data in Greenberg & Ruhlen s An Amerind Etymological Dictionary

Similar documents
The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Consonants: articulation and transcription

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Sari locative noun classes Contents

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

Dickinson ISD ELAR Year at a Glance 3rd Grade- 1st Nine Weeks

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Underlying Representations

Monticello Community School District K 12th Grade. Spanish Standards and Benchmarks

Conteúdos de inglês para o primeiro bimestre. Turma 21. Turma 31. Turma 41

been each get other TASK #1 Fry Words TASK #2 Fry Words Write the following words in ABC order: Write the following words in ABC order:

source or where they are needed to distinguish two forms of a language. 4. Geographical Location. I have attempted to provide a geographical

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Year 4 National Curriculum requirements

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

Basic concepts: words and morphemes. LING 481 Winter 2011

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Sounds of Infant-Directed Vocabulary: Learned from Infants Speech or Part of Linguistic Knowledge?

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Grade 2 Unit 2 Working Together

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

HISTORY COURSE WORK GUIDE 1. LECTURES, TUTORIALS AND ASSESSMENT 2. GRADES/MARKS SCHEDULE

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

MARK 12 Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Words come in categories

I propose an analysis of thorny patterns of reduplication in the unrelated languages Saisiyat

Primary English Curriculum Framework

Developing Grammar in Context

15 The syntax of overmarking and kes in child Korean

On the final vowel in Kikae

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Grade 3: Module 2B: Unit 3: Lesson 10 Reviewing Conventions and Editing Peers Work

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Set up your desk: Do Now Share-Out 1. Do Now Share-Out 2. Rule the World Essay 10/11/2012

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

ENGLISH LANGUAGE ARTS SECOND GRADE

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Beyond constructions:

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Comprehension Recognize plot features of fairy tales, folk tales, fables, and myths.

Providing student writers with pre-text feedback

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

UKLO Round Advanced solutions and marking schemes. 6 The long and short of English verbs [15 marks]

Pontificia Universidad Católica del Ecuador Facultad de Comunicación, Lingüística y Literatura Escuela de Lenguas Sección de Inglés

INSTANT VOCABULARY 6-10

Welcome to the Purdue OWL. Where do I begin? General Strategies. Personalizing Proofreading

Functional English 47251

NAPLAN State report: Year 3

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Holt Spanish 1 Answer Key Grammar Tutor

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

MARK¹² Reading II (Adaptive Remediation)

Grade 5: Module 2A: Unit 1: Lesson 6 Analyzing an Interview with a Rainforest Scientist Part 1

International Journal of Informative & Futuristic Research ISSN (Online):

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Acquisition vs. Learning of a Second Language: English Negation

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Generative Second Language Acquisition & Foreign Language Teaching Winter 2009

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

The Ontario Curriculum

About this unit. Lesson one

Language contact in East Nusantara

TEKS Comments Louisiana GLE

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay

Transcription:

Corrections to and clarifications of the Seri data in Greenberg & Ruhlen s An Etymological Dictionary Stephen A. Marlett Seri data have been included in comparative studies of Native American languages of North America, especially those that relate to the putative Hokan family and the putative family. Since the publication in recent years of much more analyzed Seri data, including those found in the 2005 dictionary, it is important to reassess the data that has been used in earlier comparative studies. This paper examines the data included in Greenberg & Ruhlen s (2007) An Etymological Dictionary, corrects mistakes and clarifies the data generally. * The first comprehensive presentation of Greenberg s view of (Greenberg 1987) appeared more than twenty years ago. This work generated a great deal of discussion that I do not review here (but see Campbell 1997, especially chapters 3, 7, and 8, for example). Other people have pointed out factual problems with data from specific languages, both before and after Campbell s critique. Greenberg (1987) had access to a limited amount of Seri data, including Moser & Moser (1961) and much poorer data from previous decades. But obviously Greenberg did not review all of the literature on the language that existed by that time. The scope of his project made that difficult for one person or even a small team of people to do. A revised version of the data presented in Greenberg (1987) has now appeared as Greenberg & Ruhlen (version 12: September 4, 2007), An Etymological Dictionary (henceforth AED). The AED includes some new sets, including some Seri data, when compared to Greenberg (1987). The publication of the AED gives me the opportunity to do what I neglected to do for the earlier publication, which is to review the Seri data presented and to indicate where corrections need to be made. Publication of these corrections is intended to help avoid repetition of these errors in the future by other researchers and also help linguists evaluate the proposals of the AED. Table 1 lists the information according to the AED in the first three columns (using the numbering and the order of the AED), including the proposed forms for Proto- and Proto-Hokan (both controversial), and the Seri data that they include (in the third column). In the last two columns I provide corrected data and commentary. The data presented here are primarily from M. Moser & Marlett (2005, Diccionario), indicated in Table 1 as MMD plus a page number. When the data are also available from E. Moser & M. Moser (1961, Vocabulario), I have also included a reference to that work, indicated as MMV. Differences in detail between these two sources are not presented. Obviously progress was made in documenting and understanding the language during the forty-some years that separated these two publications. The same data are found in M. Moser & Marlett (2010) but the pagination is different. Morphological or other information is available from various sources, primarily Marlett (1981), the grammar sections of M. Moser & Marlett (2005, 2010), and Marlett (in preparation). * I thank Lyle Campbell and Merrit Ruhlen for comments and suggestions on an earlier draft of this paper. All remaining deficiencies and errors are my own responsibility. Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session, vol. 51 (2011) http://arts-sciences.und.edu/summer-institute-of-linguistics/work-papers/_files/docs/2011-marlett2.pdf Copyright 2011 by SIL International

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 2 I have converted the transcriptions found in M. Moser & Marlett (2005) to IPA conventions for this paper. As it turns out, Ruhlen converted the IPA-style transcriptions of Greenberg (1987) to (generally) Americanist conventions for the AED. I do not try to harmonize the two transcriptions, but see the explanation of important differences at the beginning of Table 1. I do not take a position here on the reasonableness of the broad hypotheses that are advanced in either Greenberg (1987) or AED, although I have been generally critical of the claims about genetic relationships that have not been supported by solid evidence (Marlett 2007, 2008a) and like many others am especially skeptical about. However, I use shading in some rows in Table 1 to indicate where I think the Seri data definitely do not support the particular reconstruction for which they were listed in AED. If these are excluded, (at best) 33 forms for Seri are found in support of the proposals. In many of these cases, as it might also be pointed out, no data from Yuman languages are included either. It remains to be seen whether additional data from M. Moser & Marlett (2005, 2010) could be or will be added to the comparative database. The lack of serious work on the much more basic (and reasonable, but still undemonstrated) proposal of a Seri-Yuman grouping would suggest that it is premature to be attempting the demonstration of. (Crawford 1976 was an important attempt to seriously demonstrate the evidence.)

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 3 Table 1: Data Explanation of transcriptions: The AED, like Greenberg (1987), uses particular conventions for representing sounds that must be explained and their differences from those in my data. transcription of Seri data in AED transcription of Seri data in this paper Explanation e ɛ slightly more open than open-mid front vowel (functioning as low vowel in the phonology, in which the system contrasts high vs. low despite the skewed phonetics) f ɸ voiceless labial fricative (typically bilabial) ɣ k The AED data represent an unconfirmed impressionistic transcription of what must be /k/. h x voiceless velar fricative k kʔ a sequence of two consonants (voiceless velar stop followed by glottal stop) that occurs at morpheme boundaries ʃ voiceless slightly retroflex postalveolar sibilant x χ voiceless uvular fricative y j palatal approximant For the sake of explicitness, I indicate stress in the data I present (using a superior vertical stroke preceding the primary stressed syllable and an inferior vertical stroke preceding a secondary stressed syllable). Discussion of Seri stress patterns is presented in Marlett (2008b). Other non-ipa conventions in the proto- or proto-hokan data for sounds that do not occur in Seri include <c> for [t s ] (AED, p. 8), superscript y to indicate a palatalized consonant, and <t l > for (apparently) an ejective lateral affricate. The symbol H is unexplained. Geminate consonants in my data represent false geminates (which occur at morpheme boundaries). A <V> in the last column indicates a vowel position of indeterminate quality since it always receives its features from a prefix, as described in Marlett (in preparation), while <VV> indicates that the vowel of indeterminate quality is long. I do not know what the implications of this information may be for historical and comparative studies. Morpheme gloss abbreviations used include: ABS absolute, AUG augment, IRR.ID independent irrealis, PL plural, PON proposition/oblique nominalizer, SN subject nominalizer, TR transitive, 1P first person possessor, 3P third person possessor. The abbreviations K and L in the AED, copied in the present article, are not explained in the source. Other abbreviations: MMD (M. Moser & Marlett, Diccionario, 2005) and MMV (E. Moser & M. Moser, Vocabulario, 1961). The symbol indicates a root boundary.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 4 4 ABOVE 4 *q al 6 ADJECTIVAL/ PARTICIPIAL FORMANT *ki 12 ALLATIVE (MOTION TOWARD) *-k 32 ASHES *pok(us) ~ *pul 54 BEHIND 1 *(a)pi 70 BLACK 1 *pol 88 BONE 3 *cak 93 BREAK (v.) *k at i 100 BROAD *pat l AED Seri Hokan data koɬa upward *k y - ki- (adjective formant) (ʔ)ak (locative) emaɣ (Kroeber), anti-mak (Moser) ˈkoɬa up high (MMD 207, MMV 31) k- (subject nominalizer, nonnegative, non-passive allomorph) ʔak (definite article for locations, etc.) (MMD 311) ˌʔant ˈimak (earth 3P-middle) = ashes (MMD 331, MMV 57) ʔiːp tail ˈiːp (3P-tail) (MMD 407, MMV 79) ko-opoɬ ˈkoːpoɬ (SN-black) (MMD 225, MMV 35) itak ˈitak (3P-bone) (MMD 430, MMV 83) k-ka ˈkkaʃχ (SN-AUGtorn (MMD 169, MMV 20) k-apt-h ˈkaptx (SN-wide), (sing. subject), ˈkaptaɬka (SN-widek-apt-aɬka PL) (MMD 134, (pl. subject) MMV 14) A morpheme i- follows the k- when the verb is transitive, visible only in a limited context. This is believed to be derived from the verb aʔka be located just as other definite articles are derived from positional verbs. The root for middle is Vmak. The root for tail is Vpa. The final vowel shows up only in the derived verb. The root for (be) black is oːpoɬ. The root for bone is Vtak. The root for (be) torn is aʃχ. The root for wide is aptx. Changes are made to this through infixation and suffixation to form the plural.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 5 154 COME 5 *pi 155 COOK 1 *maki 178 DIE 1 *maki 223 EARTH 3 *ama(k) *ama(t) 240 EXCRE- MENT 1 *pa 242 EYE 1 *(i)to(ʔ ) 299 FLY 1 (n.) *k ampa Hokan AED Seri data ifp ˈkaaɸp (SN-arrive) (MMD 50, MMV 4); ˈiiɸp (3P-PON-arrive) * muht ka-motni kaˈmotni (SN-AUGdeformed.by.heat) make cracklings (MMD 123, MMV 13) *(i/a)ma kill, *ma die, *(i/a)mi(ː) sleep -iːm sleep ˈkiːm (SN-sleep) (MMD 502, MMV 96) amt ˈʔamt soil (MMD 323, MMV 54) ʔap ikaːp ˌʔapχ ˈkaap (outside SN-stand) is a modern euphemism for defecate. (MMD 68, MMV 60) *ʔat u eye, head ʔaːto (Kroeber ito) x-komo-h, x-komo-ɬk (pl.) ˈʔato (ABS-eye), ˈito (3P-eye) (MMD 358, 434; MMV 64) ˈχkoːmo (MMD 583, MMV 111) The singular root for arrive is aɸap (sg.), with the second vowel appearing in the imperfective aspect. The first vowel is irregularly long in the citation form. The root for deformed by heat is moton. The second vowel is lost in the perfective, and an epenthetic i inserted. The root for sleep is iːm. This word also seems to be an archaic form of the modern ˈʔant earth. The form ʔamat for earth appears in songs. The root for eye is Vto. There is no reason to segment the χ at the beginning; any reason to segment the x at the end would be rather esoteric. The plural form is a common pattern.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 6 382 HEAVEN *ema 393 HOT *k ec k ac 398 I 2 *ʔi 406 INTERRO- GATIVE 2 *ki *ka *ku 427 LARGE 1 *ta(k) 445 LIVE 2 (v.) *ya 459 LOUSE 3 *ik wa 483 MEAT 3 *mati 498 MOTHER *tati 519 NECK 4 *pak 525 NIGHT 2 *mok Hokan AED Seri data ʔamime ʔaˈmiːmɛ (MMD 322, MMV 54) kko i ˈkkoʃim (SNbe.hot.weather) (MMD 172, MMV 21) i I, *l y e I, we *(a) h i what ʔe I, ʔi my ˈʔɛ (first person pronoun, both singular and plural); ʔi- (1P) (MMD 366, 378; MMV 66) kiʔ who ˈkiːʔ (MMD 497, MMV 94) -atxo many ˈkatχo (SNmany/much) (MMD 151, MMV 17) -iʔ ˈkiːʔ (SNbe.(flexible.item) (MMD 497, MMV 96) *a i, *ahkʷi ʔai ak nit ip a i ip a (pl.) ˌʔait ˈiχak (ABSlouse-PL 3P-nit) (MMD 437, MMV 52) ipˈχa i (3P-flesh), ipˈχaʃ (3P-flesh-PL) (MMD 423, MMV 81) ʔita ˈʔita (1P-mother), ˈata (3P-mother) (MMD 42, MMV 74) k-iphk hang on neck ˈkipxk (SN-TRwear.around.neck) (MMD 525, MMV 99) iʔamok iˈʔamok (3P-PONbe.night), ˈkʔamok (SN-be.night) (MMD 395, 175; MMV 77, 25) This may be an old compound, from something like ʔam iːmɛ (? 3P-home). Compare ʔaˈmak fire (perhaps from ʔam ʔak (? Locative.Article). The root for be hot weather is koʃim. The root for many/much is atχo. The root for be.(flexible.item) is iːʔ. The root for nit is Vχak. The root for flesh is pχa i. The root for mother is Vta. The root for wear around neck is apxk. The root for be night is ʔamok.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 7 620 SAY 3 *koʔe 635 SEED 2 *icu 648 SING *siya 653 SIT 5 *iku 655 SKIN 5 *pakti 666 SLEEP 1 *cima 673 SMALL 3 *k uti 675 SMELL 3 *k ukʷi 699 SPIDER 1 *ʔako Hokan AED Seri data keː kaʔ ˈkɛː (SN-say), ˈkaʔ (IMP-do/say/put), ˈkiʔ (SN-TR-do/say/put) (MMD 472, 488; MMV 91, 10, 94) *iso is fruit ˈis (3P-immature.fruit) (MMD 424, MMV 82) k-oː ˈkoː (SN-sing) (MMD 226, MMV 35) -ix- ˈkiː (SN-sit) (MMD 500, MMV 96) -biɬ iˈnaːiɬ (3P-skin) (MMD 416, MMV 54) (K) *(a/i)sima, (L) *sema sim ˈ iːm (IRR.ID-sleep), ˈkiːm (SN-sleep) (MMD 502, MMV 96) kosot ˈkoː ot (SN-narrow) (MMD 227, MMV 35) k eː stink ˈkʔɛːmt (SN-stink) (MMD 176, MMV 25) *qɔk h e -xox Various terms exist, nothing like what appears in AED. The root for say (sg.) is ɛː. The root for do/say/put (sg.) is aʔ. The root for immature fruit is Vs. The root for sing is oː i. The second vowel is dropped in the singular forms. The root for sit (sg.) is iː. The root for skin is naːiɬ. AED data for Seri must be an error; perhaps they are from another language. There is no b in Seri, and no glottalized consonants. The root for sleep is iːm. It is not clear why there is an s in the AED since in item 178, the same lexeme, it does not appear. The root for narrow is oː ot. It is not clear what happened in the data in the AED. Perhaps data from another language happened to be included here by mistake.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 8 711 STAR 1 *(a)sin 778 THOU 1 *mi *ma 794 TONGUE 3 *paɬ 843 VAGINA 2 *ʔaci 852 WATER/ DRINK *aq ʷa/*uq ʷa 856 WATER 4 *si 885 WING *kampi Hokan AED Seri data a ˈʃaːʔ sun, day (MMD 611, MMV 117) *mi *ma me thou, ˈmɛ (second person you pronoun, both singular and plural) (MMD 444, MMV 85) (i/a-)pal y *ipawa (K), *pel y *pen y (K) lick (ʔe)baHleː (L) * i i penis *ʔaː hw a *aː-x y aː water, *(a/i)k hw ey rain, cloud, *ʔɨk hw i rain (n.) *i/a-si drink ʔapɬ ʔa it ʔa i drink kap (implied to mean wing ) ˈʔapɬ (ABS-tongue), ˈipɬ (3P-tongue) (MMD 421, MMV 59) ˈʔa it (ABS-vulva) (MMV 61) ˈʔaχ (ABS-liquid) fresh water (MMD 359, MMV 65) ˈkisi (SN-TR-drink) (MMD 530, MMV 101) ˈkkap (SN-fly) (MMD 166, MMV 19) The root for tongue is Vpaɬ. The second vowel is dropped in the singular form. The root for vulva is Vsit. The Seri form is the only Hokan data given for this set of proposed cognates in the AED. The root for liquid is Vχa. The vowel a appears in the plural form (followed by a suffix) ˈʔaχa am. The root for drink is asi. The root for fly is kap. The root for wing, branch is VV ɛ a.

Marlett: Seri data in An Etymological Dictionary 9 References Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian Languages: The historical linguistics of Native America. New York: Oxford University Press. Crawford, Judith G. 1976. Seri and Yuman. In Margaret Langdon & Shirley Silver (eds.), Hokan Studies, 305-324. The Hague: Mouton. Greenberg, Joseph H. 1987. Language in the Americas. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Greenberg, Joseph H. & Merritt Ruhlen. 2007. An etymological dictionary. Manuscript on-line at http://www.merrittruhlen.com/files/aed5.pdf. Accessed 11 July 2011. Marlett, Stephen A. 1981. The structure of Seri. Dissertation, University of California, San Diego. Marlett, Stephen A. 2007. Las relaciones entre las lenguas hokanas en México: Cuál es la evidencia? In Cristina Buenrostro et al. (eds.), Memorias del III Coloquio Internacional de Lingüística Mauricio Swadesh, 165-192. Mexico City: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México and Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas. Marlett, Stephen A. 2008a. The Seri and Salinan connection revisited. International Journal of American Linguistics 74.3: 393 99. Marlett, Stephen A. 2008b. Stress, extrametricality and the minimal word in Seri. Linguistic Discovery 6.1:1-14. On-line: http://journals.dartmouth.edu/cgi-bin/webobjects/journals.woa /2/xmlpage/1/article/321. Accessed 8 September 2011. Marlett, Stephen A. In preparation. A reference grammar of Seri. Draft on-line at: http://www.und.nodak.edu/instruct/smarlett/stephen_marlett/grammardraft.html. Accessed 11 July 2011. Moser, Mary B. & Marlett, Stephen A., compilers. 2005. Comcáac quih yaza quih hant ihíip hac: Diccionario seri- español-inglés. First edition. Mexico City and Hermosillo: Plaza y Valdés Editores and Universidad de Sonora. On-line: http://www.sil.org/mexico/seri/g004- diccionario-sei.htm. Accessed 11 July 2011. Moser, Mary B. & Marlett, Stephen A., compilers. 2010. Comcaac quih yaza quih hant ihiip hac: Diccionario seri- español-inglés. Second edition. Mexico City and Hermosillo: Plaza y Valdés Editores and Universidad de Sonora. Moser, Edward W. & Moser, Mary B. 1961. Vocabulario seri: seri-castellano, castellano-seri. Mexico City: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano. Steve Marlett SIL, 16131 N. Vernon Dr. Tucson AZ 85739-9395 steve_marlett@sil.org