The Mass/Count Distinction: Evidence from On-Line Psycholinguistic Performance

Similar documents
An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Writing a composition

Words come in categories

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Morphosyntactic and Referential Cues to the Identification of Generic Statements

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

Developing Grammar in Context

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Phonological encoding in speech production

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

On the Notion Determiner

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Understanding the Relationship between Comprehension and Production

Agreement attraction in comprehension: representations and processes*

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

Processing Lexically Embedded Spoken Words

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Lexical Access during Sentence Comprehension (Re)Consideration of Context Effects

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Let's Learn English Lesson Plan

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Deliberate Learning and Vocabulary Acquisition in a Second Language

The Role of the Head in the Interpretation of English Deverbal Compounds

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Modeling full form lexica for Arabic

Fluency YES. an important idea! F.009 Phrases. Objective The student will gain speed and accuracy in reading phrases.

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Using a Native Language Reference Grammar as a Language Learning Tool

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

GRAMMATICAL MORPHEME ACQUISITION: AN ANALYSIS OF AN EFL LEARNER S LANGUAGE SAMPLES *

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

5/29/2017. Doran, M.K. (Monifa) RADBOUD UNIVERSITEIT NIJMEGEN

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Syntactic types of Russian expressive suffixes

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

Rendezvous with Comet Halley Next Generation of Science Standards

Linking Task: Identifying authors and book titles in verbose queries

A Pumpkin Grows. Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Acquiring verb agreement in HKSL: Optional or obligatory?

(3) Vocabulary insertion targets subtrees (4) The Superset Principle A vocabulary item A associated with the feature set F can replace a subtree X

More Morphology. Problem Set #1 is up: it s due next Thursday (1/19) fieldwork component: Figure out how negation is expressed in your language.

Australia s tertiary education sector

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

Transcription:

Brain and Language 68, 205 211 (1999) Article ID brln.1999.2081, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Mass/Count Distinction: Evidence from On-Line Psycholinguistic Performance Brendan Gillon McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Eva Kehayia McGill University and Jewish Rehabilitation Hospital, Montreal, Quebec, Canada and Vanessa Taler McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Under the hypothesis that the mass/count distinction in English is marked by a monovalent lexical feature, this article investigates whether features, lexical or morphosyntactic, play a role in simple lexical decision. Research findings have yet to settle how many features are accessed during lexical decision and to what extent morphosyntactic features are computed out of context. We used two on-line lexical decision experiments (simple and morphosyntactic priming). Results show that the lexical feature mass is computed in both experiments. However, the morphosyntactic feature plural is subject to task-specific effects and surfaces only where operative. 1999 Academic Press Key Words: mass; count; singular; plural; feature; lexical access; lexical semantics. Psycholinguistic investigations of the mental lexicon have, thus far, shown that an increased processing load, either of grammatical features encoded in the representation or of morphological operations, can affect reaction times (RTs) during word recognition in on-line psycholinguistic experiments (Taft & Forster, 1975; Laudanna et al., 1992; Niemi et al., 1994; Kehayia & Jarema, 1994). Little is known about the role of grammatical features in online word recognition; even less is known about such features as mass for nouns, though neurolinguistic findings have shown dissociations in the performance of patients on different noun classes, including mass and count nouns. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Brendan Gillon, Department of Linguistics, McGill University, 1085 Avenue Docteur-Penfield. Montreal, Quebec H3A 1A7, Canada. 205 0093-934X/99 $30.00 Copyright 1999 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

206 GILLON, KEHAYIA, AND TALER Before describing our experiments which address the mass/count distinction, we briefly outline the morphosyntactic complexity of English nouns. English has four noun classes: proper names, pronouns, mass nouns, and count nouns, the latter two comprising the class of common nouns, the exclusive concern of our study. The mass/count distinction manifests itself in English in a number of ways. For example, count nouns admit a morphological contrast between singular and plural (e.g., table and tables), whereas mass nouns do not (e.g., furniture and *furnitures). Also, cardinal numerals (e.g., two, three, etc.) and quasi-cardinal numerals (e.g., few, several) modify count nouns (e.g., two tables), never mass nouns (e.g., *two furniture). The semantic difference between these two classes of common nouns is this. On the one hand, a singular count noun which applies to an object does not apply to any of its proper parts. For example, the word table applies to a single table, but it does not apply to any of its proper parts, say, one of its legs. In this sense, the members of the denotation of a count noun are indivisible, or atomic. On the other hand, a singular mass noun which applies to an item may apply to some, or even all, of its proper parts. The word oil, for example, applies to every mechanically obtainable portion of a puddle of oil. It is important to stress that while a mass noun which applies to an item may, as in the case of oil, apply to its proper parts, it need not. The word furniture applies to a single table, but does not apply to the table s leg. A member of the denotation of a mass noun such as oil is divisible and hence said to be nonatomic (MN), while a member of the denotation of a mass noun such as furniture is not divisible and hence said to be atomic (MA). In our study, we tracked both kinds of mass nouns. In addition, four subclasses of common count nouns were tracked: duals (DL) such as rope, so-called because they double as count nouns and mass nouns; fossils (FS) such as man, so-called because they have suppletive plurals which are vestiges of early English; invariable plurals (IP), or pluralia tantum, such as goggles, so-called because they never lose the plural suffix -s; and plural mass (MP) nouns such as seconds, so-called because of some of the morphosyntactic properties they share with mass nouns. (See Table 1 below; see Gillon 1992 for further details.) TABLE 1 Noun Classes Tested Singular Plural Regular count nouns (RC) table tables Fossils (FS) man men Invariant: -s (IP) trousers Mass: plural (MP) comics Mass: nonatomic (MN) water Mass: atomic (MA) furniture Dual (DL) rope ropes

THE MASS/COUNT DISTINCTION 207 The singular plural distinction is a morphosyntactic one. It is marked by a suffix or suppletive form and is manifested through the morphosyntactic requirements of agreement. No overt, dedicated suffix marks the mass count distinction in English; rather, it appears indirectly through the morphosyntax of grammatical number and through cooccurrence restrictions. Thus, the mass count distinction is a lexical one. However, both distinctions mass count and singular plural can be easily captured by features, indeed by monovalent features: briefly, mass nouns are assigned the feature M, whereas count nouns are not; plural nouns are assigned the feature P, while singular nouns are not. HYPOTHESIS The fundamental assumption is that during word recognition processing load increases with an increase of features to be computed. A priori, two things are clear. First, it is an open question whether features, lexical or morphosyntactic, play a role in simple lexical decision. To date, research findings are controversial as to how many features are accessed during lexical decision and to what extent morphosyntactic features are computed out of context. Second, features must play a role in the determination of acceptable combinations. Our aim is to proceed step by step so as to isolate whether and where the features come into play. To this effect we used two on-line visual lexical decision tasks. We used a simple lexical decision task to probe lexical features and a morphosyntactic priming task (henceforth called priming task ) to probe morphosyntactic features. We anticipated that if lexical and morphosyntactic features surface during these tasks, they would have an effect on word recognition patterns. Methodology Experiments were run using PsyScope 1.1 (MacWhinney, 1996). Subjects were asked to indicate whether the target on the screen was a real word in English by pressing the designated YES/NO keys on the keyboard. The dependent variables were reaction time (RT) and error rate. The independent variables were: (a) stimulus type, (b) number (singular/plural), and (c) grammaticality (priming task). Subjects EXPERIMENT 1 SIMPLE LEXICAL DECISION Twenty-five native speakers of English, ages 18 to 58 years, with 14 to 18 years of education, participated. Stimuli The total set of stimuli comprised 300 real words: 150 experimental stimuli, mass and count nouns, and 150 fillers, which were verbs in the past tense or in the present tense. There were

208 GILLON, KEHAYIA, AND TALER also 280 nonwords created by changing the first or second phoneme of the real words, while respecting the phonotactic constraints of English. Procedure Stimuli were presented in lowercase. Subjects first saw a mask comprising a series of pound signs (######) in the center of the screen, matching the number of characters of the preceding stimulus. It was presented for 200 ms and was followed by a pause of 150 ms. The target appeared immediately after the pause. The interstimulus interval was set at 200 ms. Stimuli were presented in blocks of 80. The actual test was preceded by a practice session comprising 10 items and a pretest comprising 5 trial items. Results Before proceeding with the analysis, erroneous responses were isolated; they ranged from 0.4 to 3.4% across the different categories, while the IP category yielded the highest error rate, 8.7%. Two types of comparisons were conducted for the experimental items. The first compared mass and count nouns. The second contrasted singular and plural nouns for the DL, FS, and RC categories. With respect to the first comparison, a repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of category (Mass/Count) for the singular nouns (DL, FS, and RC), on the one hand, and MA nouns on the other [F(1, 25) 11.150, p.0001]. A Fischer post hoc analysis also revealed a significant difference between mass and regular count nouns (MA/RC, p.0001). (See Table 2 below.) This finding is not surprising if we assume that processing load increases proportionally to the number of features processed and that during word recognition the lexical feature M is being accessed only for mass nouns. It must be noted that frequency was not found to be a determining factor in the above difference. Within the set of plural nouns, a repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a main effect for the category of invariant plural nouns (IP), where IP nouns were significantly slower than all others [F(1, 25) 33.495, p.0001]. Two factors are thought to play a role here: the low frequency of items TABLE 2 Paired Comparisons between Singular and Plural Nouns in Simple Lexical Decision Category Singular Plural DL (candy) 593.4 597.7 FS (mouse) 592.7 585.7 RC (baby) 572.7 585.1 IP (trousers) 666.7 MA (money) 628.5 MN (sugar) 619.0 MP (comics) 605

THE MASS/COUNT DISTINCTION 209 in the IP category as well as their susceptibility to faulty parsing during word recognition. Thus, an IP noun such as trousers is liable to be parsed as trouser-s, searched for as trouser, and, when not found, searched for as trousers. Further evidence for faulty parsing comes from the fact that IPs have the highest error rate (8.7%) of any nominal class. With respect to the mass/count distinction, Fischer post hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between the category of MP nouns and those of RC and FS nouns (MP/RC, p.0001; MP/RC, p.0001). With respect to the contrast between singular and plural within the DL, FS, and RC noun groups, repeated-measures ANOVAS revealed no significant effect. While this finding is as expected for DLs, given their dual nature, and for FSs, given their fossilized no -s plural, one wonders about RC, where one may have expected significantly longer RTs if parsing of the plural marker -s takes place. However, given the nature of the task, where morphosyntactic features such as P (plural) need not be accessed and given the high frequency of RC nouns, one may hypothesize that instead of parsing the plural -s, subjects opted for a whole-word access recognition process. EXPERIMENT II LEXICAL DECISION WITH MORPHOSYNTACTIC PRIMING In this task, each stimulus was preceded by a determiner or adjective to yield grammatical or ungrammatical combinations, e.g., three babies-*three baby; much mud-*many mud. The same types of determiner primes were used across noun types. Assuming that the morphosyntactic prime activates a number of features and creates expectations for a set of specific features in the target, it is hypothesized that whenever a mismatch occurs between what the prime activates and what the target activates, RTs for the target will be slower. Nevertheless, subjects were simply requested to do a lexical decision on the target and not to judge the grammaticality of the prime target. Subjects Twenty-two native speakers of English, ages 18 to 58 years, with 14 to 18 years of education, participated. Stimuli The total set of stimuli comprised 442 real word targets: 218 experimental stimuli presented in both grammatical and ungrammatical combinations, e.g., three babies three baby and 224 verb filler pairs, e.g., wash washed. There were also 360 nonword targets created by changing the first or second phoneme of real words while respecting phonotactic constraints of English. Nonword targets were preceded by the same morphosyntactic real-word primes as were the real-word targets, e.g., three sabies. Procedure Stimuli were presented in lowercase. Primes were presented for 250 ms. The target was presented starting at 0 and was followed by a mask, consisting of a series of pound signs

210 GILLON, KEHAYIA, AND TALER (######) in the center of the screen, presented for 100 ms. The interstimulus interval was set at 200 ms. Stimuli were in blocks of 80 with a break in between blocks. The actual test was preceded by a practice session comprising 10 items and a pretest comprising 5 trial pairs. Results Erroneous responses were again isolated. They ranged from 0.9 to 2.6% for the grammatical conditions and from 0.5 to 3.8% for the ungrammatical conditions across all categories with the exception of the category of IP nouns that again showed an elevated error rate of 7.3% in the grammatical condition and 8.9% in the ungrammatical condition. Repeated-measures ANOVAS revealed a main effect for category (Mass/ Count) [F(1, 22) 15.688, p.0001]. A post hoc analysis revealed that, as with the simple lexical decision task, within the singular grammatical category, MA nouns were significantly slower than all others (Fischer test of significance, MA/RC, p.01; MA/FS, p.007; MA/DL, p.08). Similarly to the previous experiment, the IPs were responded to significantly more slowly than all other plural nouns in the grammatical condition (Fischer test of significance, IP/RC, p.0001; IP/FS, p.0001; IP/DL, p.0002; IP/MA, p.0001). (See Table 3 below.) We now turn to the comparison between grammatical and ungrammatical conditions. As expected in all cases involving a mismatch between the determiner-prime and the overt morphosyntactic feature -s in the target as in the RC and the DL categories, ungrammatical conditions yielded significantly longer RTs (in the singular condition F(1, 22) 19.979, p.0002, in the plural condition F(1, 22) 14.461, p.001). This pattern is reversed for the MA and the IP nouns (MAg/MAug: F(1, 22).9, p ns; IPg/Ipug: F(1, 22).7, p ns). In both cases, the explanation lies with facilitation arising from semantic priming. Recall that the semantic import for a noun lacking the feature M (i.e., a count noun) is that its denotation is atomic, whereas the semantic import for a noun possessing the feature M (i.e., a mass noun) is that its denotation need not be atomic. Atomic mass nouns TABLE 3 Paired Comparisons between Singular (SG) and Plural (PL) Nouns in Primed Lexical Decision SG Category (Gramm.) SG (Ungramm.) PL (Gramm.) PL (Ungramm.) DL 634.3 668.2 640.5 671.1 FS 608.0 602.0 608.5 618.1 IP 738.2 722.6 MA 676.8 658.9 MP 653.6 680.4 MN 635.1 650.2 RC 612.3 639.7 614.2 643.7

THE MASS/COUNT DISTINCTION 211 are nouns which are exceptional in that their denotations, being atomic, suit them for being count nouns, yet they are in fact mass nouns. We hypothesize that it is this exceptionality which accounts for their longer RTs in the primed grammatical condition. Moreover, it is precisely this exceptionality which facilitates the recognition of mass atomic nouns in the ungrammatical condition, for the plural determiner-prime requires atoms for its interpretation, which fits with the exceptional nature of the mass atomic nouns. Similarly, IP nouns are recognized more quickly in the ungrammatical condition than in the grammatical one. Once again, in the ungrammatical condition the singular determiner-prime facilitates the recognition of the word. In conclusion, our results show that the lexical feature M is indeed computed in both simple and primed on-line word recognition. However, the morphosyntactic feature P is subject to task-specific effects and surfaces only in the second experiment, where it is operative. Further research, using different tasks, is necessary if we are to define the level at which the grammatical features that characterize English nouns surface. REFERENCES Gillon, B. 1992. Towards a common semantics for English count and mass nouns. Linguistics and Philosophy, 15, 597 639. Kehayia, E., & Jarema, G. 1994. Morphological priming (or Prim#ing) of inflected verb forms: A comparative study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 2, 83 94. Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. 1967. Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown Univ. Press. Laudanna, A., Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A. 1992. Processing inflectional and derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 333 348. Niemi, J., Laine, M., & Tuominen, J. 1994. Cognitive morphology in Finnish: Foundations of a new model. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 423 446. Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. 1975. Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 14, 638 647.