STUDENTS (GRADES 6-8) UNDERSTANDING OF GRAPHS

Similar documents
Mathematics Success Level E

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Minitab Tutorial (Version 17+)

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

Mathematics Success Grade 7

(I couldn t find a Smartie Book) NEW Grade 5/6 Mathematics: (Number, Statistics and Probability) Title Smartie Mathematics

AP Statistics Summer Assignment 17-18

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

Radius STEM Readiness TM

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half

Spinners at the School Carnival (Unequal Sections)

Math Grade 3 Assessment Anchors and Eligible Content

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

First Grade Standards

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

South Carolina English Language Arts

Standard 1: Number and Computation

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

Mathematics process categories

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

On-Line Data Analytics

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

FOR TEACHERS ONLY. The University of the State of New York REGENTS HIGH SCHOOL EXAMINATION PHYSICAL SETTING/PHYSICS

Degree Qualification Profiles Intellectual Skills

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Measurement. When Smaller Is Better. Activity:

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Functional Skills Mathematics Level 2 assessment

Paper 2. Mathematics test. Calculator allowed. First name. Last name. School KEY STAGE TIER

Interpreting ACER Test Results

Further, Robert W. Lissitz, University of Maryland Huynh Huynh, University of South Carolina ADEQUATE YEARLY PROGRESS

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

Teacher Action Research Multiple Intelligence Theory in the Foreign Language Classroom. By Melissa S. Ferro George Mason University

Students Understanding of Graphical Vector Addition in One and Two Dimensions

Statewide Framework Document for:

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

Arizona s College and Career Ready Standards Mathematics

PROGRESS MONITORING FOR STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES Participant Materials

Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

Table of Contents. Introduction Choral Reading How to Use This Book...5. Cloze Activities Correlation to TESOL Standards...

Lesson 17: Write Expressions in Which Letters Stand for Numbers

Classroom Connections Examining the Intersection of the Standards for Mathematical Content and the Standards for Mathematical Practice

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

November 2012 MUET (800)

success. It will place emphasis on:

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

The lab is designed to remind you how to work with scientific data (including dealing with uncertainty) and to review experimental design.

Facing our Fears: Reading and Writing about Characters in Literary Text

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards

1.11 I Know What Do You Know?

Lesson M4. page 1 of 2

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Unit 3: Lesson 1 Decimals as Equal Divisions

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

MADERA SCIENCE FAIR 2013 Grades 4 th 6 th Project due date: Tuesday, April 9, 8:15 am Parent Night: Tuesday, April 16, 6:00 8:00 pm

The following shows how place value and money are related. ones tenths hundredths thousandths

Introduction to Questionnaire Design

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

2013 TRIAL URBAN DISTRICT ASSESSMENT (TUDA) RESULTS

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Introduction to the Practice of Statistics

Coimisiún na Scrúduithe Stáit State Examinations Commission LEAVING CERTIFICATE 2008 MARKING SCHEME GEOGRAPHY HIGHER LEVEL

FIRST-YEAR UNIVERSITY BIOLOGY STUDENTS DIFFICULTIES WITH GRAPHING SKILLS

Abstractions and the Brain

Contents. Foreword... 5

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

Are You Ready? Simplify Fractions

Primary National Curriculum Alignment for Wales

NORTH CAROLINA VIRTUAL PUBLIC SCHOOL IN WCPSS UPDATE FOR FALL 2007, SPRING 2008, AND SUMMER 2008

with The Grouchy Ladybug

Answer Key For The California Mathematics Standards Grade 1

WiggleWorks Software Manual PDF0049 (PDF) Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

Evaluating the Effectiveness of the Strategy Draw a Diagram as a Cognitive Tool for Problem Solving

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

This scope and sequence assumes 160 days for instruction, divided among 15 units.

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Grade Five Chapter 6 Add and Subtract Fractions with Unlike Denominators Overview & Support Standards:

Word Segmentation of Off-line Handwritten Documents

Creating a Test in Eduphoria! Aware

Teaching a Laboratory Section

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Transcription:

STUDENTS (GRADES 6-8) UNDERSTANDING OF GRAPHS George W. Bright, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, USA Susan N. Friel, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA This study focused on middle grades students reasoning in response to questions about a bar graph. There were three levels of questions, and responses were coded to identify knowledge of structural components of graphs, accuracy of graphical-perception tasks, and development of graph schema. At each grade level, students (N=76 in grade 6; N=71 in grade 8) were taught by a single teacher who had been certified as a statistics educator. The teachers taught an experimental graphing unit, with a paper-and pencil task administered before and after the unit. Students seemed to learn to coordinate the components of graphs, performed graphical-perception tasks more accurately, and developed more coherent graph schema. There was some evidence to support the existence of a (perhaps developmental) sequence of reasoning strategies. This study investigated the ways that students in the middle grades (grades 6-8 in USA) interpret data presented in a bar graph. Of particular interest was the nature of the reasoning that students used as they explained their answers to questions. THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES Two theoretical perspectives framed this study. First, a number of authors (Bertin, 1973; Curcio, 1987; McKnight, 1990; Wainer, 1992) have characterized the kinds of questions about data in graphs and, to some extent, the nature of the responses to these questions. Three levels of questioning emerge: an elementary level focusing on extracting data from the graph, an intermediate level involving interpolating and finding relationships in the data shown in the graph, and an overall level involving extrapolating from the data and interpretation of the relationships identified in the graph. Second, structures are emerging for the study of graphical understanding. Although there is not yet universal agreement, there is overlap in the features that command the attention of theorists. Kosslyn (1989, 1994) has proposed language for discussing four structural components of graphs. The framework (e.g., axes, scales, grids, reference markings) provides information about what kinds of measurements are being used and what things are being measured. The simplest frame-work has an L shape; one leg, the x-axis, stands for the things being measured and the other, the y-axis, for the measurements. Visual dimensions called specifiers are used to represent data values. Specifiers are the lines, bars, point symbols, or other marks that specify particular relations among the things represented within the framework. In an L-shaped framework, each leg of the framework bears a label naming the type of measurement being made (a dependent variable) or the entity to which the measurement applies (an independent variable). Labels include coding techniques for representing category membership. The 656

title of the graph is itself a kind of label. The background of a graph includes any coloring, grid, or picture over which the graph may be superimposed. Cleveland and McGill (1984) have identified ten graphical-perception tasks that form the basic perceptual judgments that a person performs to decode visually quantitative information encoded on graphs (e.g., specifiers), though it is possible that these tasks might be grouped into fewer distinct categories. The tasks are ordered, based on what is known about the accuracy with which a person performs them, from most accurate to least accurate; some tasks are identified as being at the same levels. For example, finding positions on a common aligned scale (e.g., bar graphs) can be processed more easily and more accurately than determining area (e.g., pie charts). Pinker s (1990) theory of graph comprehension is based upon general perceptual and cognitive assumptions. His notion of graph schema is important here. A graph schema includes a person s knowledge of relationships between aspects of the visual description as they are connected to aspects of the conceptual information. A graph schema is an active, interrelated knowledge structure, a network of nodes and relations, embodying knowledge of what graphs are for and how they are interpreted. It is useful for translating the information found in the visual description into conceptual information and directing the search for desired pieces of information. A graph schema contains three key pieces of information: the display s pictorial content of some objects or geometrical figures, such as bars or lines, which are described in terms of visual attributes; the L- shaped framework of a coordinate system, specifying the ratio of magnitudes of attributes; and the textual material which specifies the real-world referents. METHOD Subjects Subjects were students in grade 6 (N = 76) from three classes taught by one teacher and students in grade 8 (N = 71) from three classes taught by a different teacher in a different middle school in North Carolina. The students had had little experience with statistics prior to this study. Both teachers were statistics educators from the Teach-Stat project (Friel and Bright, in press). These teachers had participated in a three-week statistics education inservice, used the techniques from this inservice in their own teaching, and co-taught two or more statistics education inservice programs to other teachers. Thus, their knowledge of statistics content and pedagogy was considerably greater than that of most middle grades mathematics teachers. Instrumentation 657

A paper-and-pencil task (Figure 1) was used; the four questions reflected the three levels of questions described earlier, with Questions 2 and 3 used to assess students at the second level. Answering these questions calls on knowledge of structural components of graphs, graphical-perception tasks, and graph schema. Instruction and data gathering Both classroom teachers organized instruction around the same unit on graphing (Friel and Bright, 1995), developed specifically to highlight connections between pairs of graphs. The unit was not provided as a definitive model of instruction. Rather, its use was intended to give students opportunities to gain experience with the process of statistical investigation and with some key concepts (including graphs) in statistics. The task was administered both before and after instruction during October and November, 1994. Figure 1. Task: Lengths of Cats A group of students has been investigating information about their pets Several students have cats. They decided to collect some information about each of the cats. One set of data they collected was the length of the cats measuring from the tips of the cats noses to the tips of their tails. Here is a bar graph showing the information they found: 5 Lengths of Cats in Inches 4 3 2 1 0 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 Inches 1. How many cats are 30 inches long from nose to tail? How can you tell? 2. How many cats are there in all? How can you tell? 3. If you added up the lengths of the three shortest cats, what would the total of those lengths be? How can you tell? 4. What is the typical length of a cat from nose to tail? Explain your answer. RESULTS Based on analysis of the task within the theoretical perspectives discussed earlier, coding categories (Figure 2) were created for the questions; category numbers are arbitrary. All responses were double coded to assure reliability of coding. (Because of space limitations, data are reported only for questions 2 and 3.) When categories generated unique answers, they were split into pairs (Figure 3); the a part reflects a response accompanied by an appropriate explanation, and the b 658

part reflects a response either without any explanation or with an inappropriate explanation. In each of the tables in Figure 3, the totals down the right side are pretest totals, and the totals across the bottom are posttest totals. Since the emphasis of this analysis was on students reasoning, some leeway was allowed in categorizing numerical responses. It seemed more important to categorize evidence of reasoning than skill at arithmetic. Of course, no leeway was allowed for the b part of a pair of categories. For Question 2, 27 out of 76 students (about 36%) in grade 6 and 40 out of 71 students (about 56%) in grade 8 gave pretest and posttest responses that were coded in the same category. For Question 3, the corresponding data were 22 out of 76 students (about 29%) in grade 6 and 18 out of 71 students (about 25%) in grade 8. The most common change in categorization seems to have been shifts from incorrect to correct reasoning (columns 1a and 1b, below rows 1a and 1b). Figure 2. Coding Categories and Sample Responses for Questions 2 and 3 Question 2: How many cats are there in all? How can you tell? 1: Properties of the graph; considers both the range of data and the frequencies 25 cats in all. You add all numbers represented by bars together. 2: Focus on range of data or indicated range on x-axis 37 because it is the last number marked. 3: Uses frequencies of heights of bars 12 cats, I could tell because there are twelve bars on the graph. 4: Incorrect or incomplete or not understandable 24, because you add them all up and see that it is 24. Question 3: If you added up the lengths of the three shortest cats, what would the total of those lengths be? How can you tell? 1: Choose correct lengths of 16, 25, and 25 to get 66 16 one and two 25. Read the first two bars. 2: Added 16 and 25 to get 41, ignoring that there are two cats of length 25 It would be 41. You add up the 2 shortest cats which are 16 in + 25 in = 41 in. 3: Added lengths associated with three left-most bars of height one (16+27+32=75) The length of the 3 shortest cats is 73 inches in legth (sic). I added 16 to 27 and 32 to 41 [which is the incorrect sum of 16 and 27] and got my answer 73 inches. 4: Added lengths associated with three right-most bars of height one (27+32+37=96) 27, 32, 37, the graph showes (sic). 5: Added lengths associated with all four bars of height one (16+27+32+37=112) 112, I can tell by adding up the inches that are the shortest shown on the bar graph. 6: Identified or added frequencies of shortest bars to get answer of 1, 3, or 4. 3, all you got to do is count the number of sort (sic) black lines. 7: Added lengths associated with three left-most bars (16+25+27=68) 68 inches in all. I looked at the first 3 bars and added the inches. 8: Added frequencies for three left-most bars (1+2+1=4) 4 go to the left side of the scale find the first three marks & add them up. 9: Incomplete or not categorizable 3, I added from the side of the chart. DISCUSSION For both grades, across the two administrations of the task, there was an increase 659

(Figure 3) in the number of students responding correctly using the properties of a graph and a decrease in the number of students interpreting the heights of the bars as lengths of cats. These results are consistent with the notion that students learned to coordinate the components of graphs (e.g., framework, specifiers, labels), performed graphicalperception tasks more accurately, and developed more coherent graph schema. Students seemed better able to understand the nature of data reduction evident in the graph in the sense that they moved away from interpreting each bar as representing a single cat. Too, the number of non-categorizable responses seemed to decrease, suggesting that students internalized a vocabulary for discussing their reasoning. These skills are critical ones for appropriate interpretation of data in graphs. In general, the questions seemed somewhat easier for grade 8 students than for grade 6 students, especially on the first administration. This may reflect the greater experience of grade 8 students with graphs. Similarly, Question 2 seemed easier than Question 3, even though they were at the same level (e.g., Bertin, 1983, Curcio, 1989). The difficulty with Question 3 seems to have been in knowing how to identify the shortest cats from the graphical presentation of the data. The largest subset of students who gave identically coded answers on both administrations were students who gave correct answers both times. It seems important that among students who gave incorrect answers on the first administration, the largest shift was to correct reasoning. Instruction did appear to be effective at helping them restructure their knowledge of graphs. Although the large number of categories for Question 3 makes pattern finding difficulty, there was a suggestion about how reasoning might develop. For grade 6, 16 pretest and 15 posttest responses were categorized as 7a/7b. For grade 8, 16 pretest and 13 posttest responses were categorized as 7a/7b. However, few students gave both pretest and posttest responses that were categorized this way. Focusing on the three left-most Figure 3. Summary of Responses for Questions 2 and 3 Grade 6, Question 2 Grade 8, Question 2 Code 1a 1b 2 3a 3b 4 Totals Code 1a 1b 2 3a 3b 4 Totals 1a 13 5 0 0 0 1 19 1a 16 5 0 0 0 1 22 1b 6 10 0 0 1 1 18 1b 3 18 0 0 1 4 26 2 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 3 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 3a 8 5 0 1 2 3 19 3a 0 2 0 2 1 3 8 3b 1 1 0 0 0 1 3 3b 0 1 0 1 0 0 2 4 6 7 0 0 0 3 16 4 2 6 0 0 0 4 12 Totals 34 29 0 1 3 9 76 Totals 21 33 0 3 2 12 71 NOTE: Codes down the left side are pretest codes; codes across the top are posttest codes. Grade 6, Question 3 Code 1a 1b 2a 2b 3a 3b 4a 4b 5a 5b 6a 7a 7b 8a 9 Totals 660

1a 8 2 5 15 1b 2 1 1 1 1 6 2a 2b 1 1 3a 1 2 1 4 3b 1 1 4a 1 1 2 4b 1 1 1 3 5a 1 1 5b 1 1 6a 2 1 2 2 7 7a 5 1 1 1 3 1 3 15 7b 1 1 8a 1 9 2 1 1 1 3 1 3 6 18 Totals 20 6 1 1 1 3 1 2 8 9 6 18 76 NOTE: Since codes 6a and 8a generate similar answers, categories 6b and 8b could not be coded. NOTE: Codes down the left side are pretest codes; codes across the top are posttest codes. Grade 8, Question 3 Code 1a 1b 2a 2b 3a 3b 4a 4b 5a 5b 6a 7a 7b 8a 9 Totals 1a 12 1 1 1 15 1b 1 1 3 5 2a 2b 3a 1 1 2 3b 2 1 1 4 4a 1 1 4b 1 1 5a 1 1 2 5b 1 1 6a 1 1 1 1 4 7a 5 2 2 1 1 3 14 7b 1 1 2 8a 9 3 2 4 1 2 3 3 2 20 Totals 22 6 3 6 1 6 6 7 14 71 NOTE: Since codes 6a and 8a generate similar answers, categories 6b and 8b could not be coded. NOTE: Codes down the left side are pretest codes; codes across the top are posttest codes. bars seems to hold considerable appeal for students, independent of instruction. In both grades, however, the most common shift away from category 7 was to category 1, and the most common shift to category 7 was away from the uncategorizable category 9. This suggests that category 7 reasoning may act as a transition from uninterpretable responses to correct reasoning. The data in this study support the notion that emerging theoretical structures for 661

interpreting students responses provide a way for both researchers and teachers to begin to understand students reasoning about graphs. Further study of the patterns of reasoning is needed, along with more trials of instructional materials which are based on these emerging theoretical structures REFERENCES Bertin, J. (1983). Semiology of graphics (2nd ed., W. J. Berg, Trans.). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. Cleveland, W. S., and McGill, R. (1984). Graphical perception: Theory, experimentation, and application to the development of graphical methods. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 79, 531-554. Curcio, F. R. (1987). Comprehension of mathematical relationships expressed in graphs. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 382-393. Friel, S. N., and Bright, G. W. (1995). Assessing students understanding of graphs: Instruments and instructional module. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, University of North Carolina Mathematics and Science Education Network. Friel, S. N., and Bright, G. W. (in press). Teach-Stat: A model for professional development in data analysis and statistics for teachers K-6. In S. P. Lajoie (Ed.), Reflections on statistics: Agendas for learning, teaching, and assessment in K-12. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kosslyn, S. M. (1989). Understanding charts and graphs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 3, 185-226. Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Elements of graph design. New York: W. H. Freeman. McKnight, C. C. (1990). Critical evaluation of quantitative arguments. In G. Kulm (Ed.), Assessing higher order thinking in mathematics (pp. 169-185). Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. Pinker, S. (1990). A theory of graph comprehension. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Artificial intelligence and the future of testing (pp. 73-126). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wainer, H. (1992, February/March). Understanding graphs and tables. Educational Researcher, 21(1), 14-23. 662