CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Once upon a time... Distiguishing English from not-so-english. English vs. word salad. What speakers know. Knowledge of language

Similar documents
A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

Part I. Figuring out how English works

Language acquisition: acquiring some aspects of syntax.

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Virtually Anywhere Episodes 1 and 2. Teacher s Notes

Effective Practice Briefings: Robert Sylwester 03 Page 1 of 12

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

A Pumpkin Grows. Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Developing Grammar in Context

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Argument structure and theta roles

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Case study Norway case 1

Grammars & Parsing, Part 1:

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today!

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

2 months: Social and Emotional Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Synthesis Essay: The 7 Habits of a Highly Effective Teacher: What Graduate School Has Taught Me By: Kamille Samborski

Houghton Mifflin Reading Correlation to the Common Core Standards for English Language Arts (Grade1)

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

The Four Principal Parts of Verbs. The building blocks of all verb tenses.

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

IN THIS UNIT YOU LEARN HOW TO: SPEAKING 1 Work in pairs. Discuss the questions. 2 Work with a new partner. Discuss the questions.

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

UNIT PLANNING TEMPLATE

11/29/2010. Statistical Parsing. Statistical Parsing. Simple PCFG for ATIS English. Syntactic Disambiguation

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice

Contents. Foreword... 5

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Orange Coast College Spanish 180 T, Th Syllabus. Instructor: Jeff Brown

Prewriting: Drafting: Revising: Editing: Publishing:

2014 Free Spirit Publishing. All rights reserved.

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Tap vs. Bottled Water

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Unit 14 Dangerous animals

A process by any other name

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Classify: by elimination Road signs

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Consequences of Your Good Behavior Free & Frequent Praise

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

Career Series Interview with Dr. Dan Costa, a National Program Director for the EPA

UNDERSTANDING DECISION-MAKING IN RUGBY By. Dave Hadfield Sport Psychologist & Coaching Consultant Wellington and Hurricanes Rugby.

Division Strategies: Partial Quotients. Fold-Up & Practice Resource for. Students, Parents. and Teachers

Basic Parsing with Context-Free Grammars. Some slides adapted from Julia Hirschberg and Dan Jurafsky 1

Section 7, Unit 4: Sample Student Book Activities for Teaching Listening

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

Mission Statement Workshop 2010

English Nexus Offender Learning

Defragmenting Textual Data by Leveraging the Syntactic Structure of the English Language

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Enhancing Unlexicalized Parsing Performance using a Wide Coverage Lexicon, Fuzzy Tag-set Mapping, and EM-HMM-based Lexical Probabilities

Advanced Grammar in Use

evans_pt01.qxd 7/30/2003 3:57 PM Page 1 Putting the Domain Model to Work

Interpretive (seeing) Interpersonal (speaking and short phrases)

BANGLA TO ENGLISH TEXT CONVERSION USING OPENNLP TOOLS

Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

Campus Academic Resource Program An Object of a Preposition: A Prepositional Phrase: noun adjective

Language skills to be used and worked upon : Listening / Speaking PPC-PPI / Reading / Writing

No Parent Left Behind

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Adjectives In Paragraphs

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

SAMPLE PAPER SYLLABUS

Tour. English Discoveries Online

Transfer of Training

The Foundations of Interpersonal Communication

Transcription:

1 CAS LX 522 Syntax I Introduction to the enterprise Once upon a time... Snoopy kissed whats-her-name after Pigpen chased an orange thing. Who s the girl that Snoopy kissed after Pigpen chased the orange thing? What s the thing that Snoopy kissed Lucy after Pigpen chased? What s the thing that Snoopy told Lucy that Pigpen chased? English vs. word salad Distiguishing English from not-so-english 1) Chris looked over the numbers. 2) Pat peeked over the fence. 3) Chris looked the numbers over. 4) Pat peeked the fence over. 1) Slept cat the. 2) Slept the cat. 3) The slept cat. 4) The cat slept. 5) Cat the slept. 6) Cat slept the. When presented with a sequence of English words, any (native English) speaker can tell you whether it makes a sentence of English. How? Knowledge of language Native speakers just know what is part of their language. But it s tacit knowledge we can t just explain what it is that makes a sentence English. It just is. Or isn t. Our task: Exploring and characterizing what the nature of this knowledge is, and how it differs between languages. What speakers know Although we can t explain our own knowledge of our native language, we can deduce it and, in simple cases, we have a kind of intuition about what it is. 1) The cat slept. 2) Slept cat the.

The noun Cat the does not make a good subject of a sentence, it has to be the cat. In fact, the can t really stand anywhere except before a noun. So, we hypothesize that English speakers know something like a general rule: the comes before nouns. Subject verb The noun can be the subject of a sentence. And the subject seems to come before the verb. So, we hypothesize that English speakers know something like a general rule that subjects of a sentence come before the verb. Formalities We can make these hypotheses formal, explicit: A subject is made of the and a noun. A sentence is made of a subject and a verb. 1. S subject V 2. subject the N 3. N cat 4. V slept Have we done it? Perhaps that s it, perhaps we have now described English. Let s see. There are lots of other nouns. Dog for example. And there are other verbs too. Like coughed. If these rules describe English, then The dog coughed, the cat coughed, and the dog slept should be judged to be English sentences. Hooray! Er... This is exciting! Maybe we have done it!! If this is what English speakers know about English, then all and only the sentences generated by these rules should be judged as English. 1) The dog chased the cat. Hmm. What went wrong? Although the dog chased the cat is judged to be English, our rules do not generate this sentence. Just looking at it, we can see that the problem is that some verbs, like slept and coughed describe something performed by just one individual, but chase is something one individual does to another.

Subject Verb Object Subjects and objects New rule: A verb with both a do-er ( subject ) and a do-ee ( object ) (let s call such verbs transitive ) comes between them. 1. S subject V 2. subject the N 3. S subject Vt object 4. object the N 5. N cat, dog 6. V slept, coughed 7. Vt chased We notice that our subject and object rule both look the same. Also, notice that we can also say A dog chased a cat. So, a and the are probably the same kind of thing. We ll call them determiners (though you might have called them articles ). Probably anything that can be a subject can also be an object. So we can simplify our rules. Nouny phrases &c We need a name for these the noun things. More than one word (a phrase, if you will), where the noun seems like the most important part. How about noun phrases? 1. S NP V 2. NP Det N 3. S NP Vt NP 4. N cat, dog 5. Det a, the 6. V slept, coughed 7. Vt chased Right. The grumpy cat chased the terrified dog. Ah. So, NPs can have adjectives like grumpy and terrified in them. Ok, so our theory of English knowledge is still insufficient, but there s a fairly clear way to extend it. Grammar Supposing that we finally get to the end of this procedure, what we will have constructed is a grammar a system that can distinguish strings of words into English and not English. The sort of grammar we ve been constructing is a generative grammar. The theoretical claim is that all and only strings that it generates will be judged by native English speakers as being English. It is a theory or a model of what English speakers know about English. The S NP V neuron? This is not a claim that the actual rules we re coming up with are somehow encoded in the brains of native English speakers. The system we re hypothesizing characterizes the knowledge, but who knows how the neurons are organized. We can still learn a lot about the structure of language though and maybe learn what kinds of things to look for among the neurons.

Intuitions The primary thing we re trying to explain is why people have the intuitions they have about language. For a given string of words, a native speaker can say whether it is part of their language. But probably can t tell you why. These intuitions are quite stable across speakers. We seek the basis for these intuitions. Stars The notational convention for marking a sentence that is not part of the language is putting an asterisk ( star ) in front of it. 1) *Cat the slept. 2) The cat slept. Unacceptability A string (of words) can be unacceptable for a number of different reasons. Some are important for building our model, and some are not. 1) *Big that under staple run the jump swim. 2) My toothbrush is pregnant again. 3) The rat the cat the dog chased caught escaped adeptly. Ambiguity Frequently, a sentence can be used to express more than one meaning. 1) I walked on the bank. 2) You can t stop a philosopher with a thesis. 3) All doors will not open. 4) Nothing cleans better than Bio-Soy Orange. *Ambiguity But sometimes an ambiguity we might have expected to find is not there. So the sentence isn t ungrammatical, but one intended interpretation is. 1) How did John say that Mary used the jackhammer? Answers: a) Incompetently, b) Quietly. 2) How did John ask if Mary used the jackhammer? Answers: a) *Incompetently, b) Quietly. But I would never say that It generally does not matter that you d never (or almost never) use a sentence being judged. Given the circumstances under which the sentence would be appropriate (rare as they may be), would it be English? Quite often one needs to construct rather artificial sentences to test specific hypotheses.

Felicity Another kind of unacceptability, which we generally won t be concerned with in this class, is the kind that arises from a mismatch with the preceding discourse. 1) Who bought the lamp? 2) #Noel bought the lamp. We care about this when studying constraints on the structure of discourse, but here we re studying constraints on the form of sentences in isolation. Mysteries Our knowledge of language is very complex, but not available to introspection. Children acquire language very quickly, in the same way, and with a stable result. Different languages turn out to have a lot in common there are a lot of possible properties language might have, but yet never seems to. How did we get this? Children certainly are not told things like Billy, if I ever catch you using a subject pronoun that matches the reference of a proper name object, you ll be eating only asparagus for a week! Yet they know that He saw John in the mirror can t mean that John saw himself. And really what could they hear that would teach them this? Kids don t just imitate Imitation by itself could never work as a means of acquiring this kind of language knowledge. The knowledge we re talking about differentiates English sentences from non-english sentences. (Or whatever language) Which ones are good? The ones you hear? The first platypus to eat twenty-five maroon gummi bears will win a prize. Things adults don t say Sure, ok. You generalize. Plus, kids say things they ve never heard an adult say. Me playing. What do you think what the puppet has eaten? How would this come about? So, maybe kids hear what parents say and recognize the patterns, and come up with general rules. That s not so hard, is it? Let s try it out.

Making questions 1) Sue should borrow my guitar. 2) Sue borrowed my guitar. 3) Should Sue borrow my guitar? 4) Did Sue borrow my guitar? 5) What should Sue borrow? 6) What did Sue borrow? That s easy Yes-no questions are formed by taking the second word and putting it in the front. Unless there s no word like should, then you just put did in the front, and use a bare (untensed) verb. Wh-questions are formed by removing something and putting who or what in front of the yes-no question form. See? Simple. My roommate should borrow my guitar. Roommate my should borrow my guitar? Roommate my did borrow my guitar? Who did borrow my guitar? What roommate my should borrow? Why person any would think this is hard? Trying something else Sue said that Mary borrowed my guitar. Sue said Mary borrowed my guitar. What did Sue say that Mary borrowed? What did Sue say Mary borrowed? Who did Sue say borrowed my guitar? Who did Sue say that borrowed my guitar? And one more Mary saw her in the mirror. Mary saw her duck in the mirror. Why can t her be Mary? (Except if it s Mary s duck but it can t be Mary who is ducking). How is a kid supposed to deduce this? Poverty of the stimulus The point is: The linguistic input that a child gets is insufficient to determine which of the possible rules of grammar are the right ones. Yet, children always acquire the same rules. 1, 2, 3,,,? 4, 5, 6? 5, 7, 11? 5, 8, 13? 3, 2, 1?

What are we left with? Of course, you know where this is going: Having language = being human. Rocks, ferns, cats, apes don t soak up language when surrounded by it. Birds have wings, people have arms. What determines whether you re a bird is whether your parents are birds. Human language The point: What makes an organism a human is something about what is encoded in the genes. Only humans have language. So something that makes language possible must be encoded in the genes. Universal Grammar This is the idea of Universal Grammar (UG), which we take to refer to the humanspecific cognitive structures underlying language. Languages differ, too. So, apart from the (species-)universal principles of language, there are differences in how they interact and operate. Parameters of variation. Parameters Basic word order: 1) English (SVO): Akira bought a book. 2) Japanese (SOV): John ga hon o katta. Question formation (where what goes): 3) What did Akira buy? 4) John ga nani o katta no? Explanation If languages are all governed by the same principles (which children don t have to learn), then the child s task is really to: Learn the pronunciations and meanings of the words. Determine the settings of the parameters for the language they are acquiring. What are the principles, parameters? This is the big picture take on what we re trying to accomplish in Syntax. How can languages vary? In what ways don t languages vary? Do certain language properties group together? Could they depend in some way on the same parameter?

Prescriptive rules Prescriptive rules We no doubt remember being taught things like this: 1) A preposition is something you should never end a sentence with. 2) It is important to always avoid splitting an infinitive. But there s a reason why these were painful to learn. They aren t rules of English. Prescriptive rules are generally somewhat arbitrary. Somebody s idea of what the language ought to be like, or hanging on to how the language used to be even after it has changed. If these were actually rules of English, they wouldn t need to be taught (to native speakers at least). Mainly, they serve as a secret code that educated people use to identify each other. Where is English? When we speak of English, what are we referring to? Every native speaker has a complete knowledge system of their language. As far as the grammar is concerned, it s all part of a native speaker s cognitive makeup. (Vocabulary is a different thing...) I-language, E-language The notion of English is really an external notion. It s kind of an average of the properties of the (nearly identical) knowledge systems that the individual speakers in the community have. What we re interested in here, in a sense, are the properties of a single speaker s knowledge of language. We might call it English if that speaker is part of the English speech community. But it s really an individual s knowledge. It s just that the community by and large has the same knowledge. Incidentally, re: LX250 You may recall that in LX250, you did some syntax. There, you were told things like: Sentences have structures described in terms of X-bar templates (heads, complements, specifiers), there is a CP, a IP, a VP, and some NPs. In a sense, this was kind of skipping to the middle. We re going to back up to motivate some of these things (and argue for them), and we ll wind up with a system that is a bit different (more modern). (So, don t just draw trees according to your LX250 rules, they won t be right. Close...but not right.)