INFINITE AND UNBOUNDED SETS: A PRAGMATIC PERSPECTIVE

Similar documents
Math-U-See Correlation with the Common Core State Standards for Mathematical Content for Third Grade

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Representational Fluency and Symbolisation of Derivative

Mathematics Program Assessment Plan

Students concept images of inverse functions

CAAP. Content Analysis Report. Sample College. Institution Code: 9011 Institution Type: 4-Year Subgroup: none Test Date: Spring 2011

THE ROLE OF TOOL AND TEACHER MEDIATIONS IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF MEANINGS FOR REFLECTION

Montana Content Standards for Mathematics Grade 3. Montana Content Standards for Mathematical Practices and Mathematics Content Adopted November 2011

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

Mathematics subject curriculum

What Do Croatian Pre-Service Teachers Remember from Their Calculus Course?

Page 1 of 11. Curriculum Map: Grade 4 Math Course: Math 4 Sub-topic: General. Grade(s): None specified

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

Dublin City Schools Mathematics Graded Course of Study GRADE 4

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Classroom Connections Examining the Intersection of the Standards for Mathematical Content and the Standards for Mathematical Practice

12 Developing mastery of natural language

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

South Carolina College- and Career-Ready Standards for Mathematics. Standards Unpacking Documents Grade 5

Missouri Mathematics Grade-Level Expectations

Alignment of Australian Curriculum Year Levels to the Scope and Sequence of Math-U-See Program

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

ONE TEACHER S ROLE IN PROMOTING UNDERSTANDING IN MENTAL COMPUTATION

TABE 9&10. Revised 8/2013- with reference to College and Career Readiness Standards

Mathematics Education

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

Empiricism as Unifying Theme in the Standards for Mathematical Practice. Glenn Stevens Department of Mathematics Boston University

GCSE Mathematics B (Linear) Mark Scheme for November Component J567/04: Mathematics Paper 4 (Higher) General Certificate of Secondary Education

Case study Norway case 1

FROM QUASI-VARIABLE THINKING TO ALGEBRAIC THINKING: A STUDY WITH GRADE 4 STUDENTS 1

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

AN INTRODUCTION (2 ND ED.) (LONDON, BLOOMSBURY ACADEMIC PP. VI, 282)

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Digital Fabrication and Aunt Sarah: Enabling Quadratic Explorations via Technology. Michael L. Connell University of Houston - Downtown

Math Grade 3 Assessment Anchors and Eligible Content

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Developing Students Research Proposal Design through Group Investigation Method

Mathematics process categories

Characteristics of Functions

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

HONORS OPTION GUIDELINES

Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School

Statewide Framework Document for:

Introductory thoughts on numeracy

AP Calculus AB. Nevada Academic Standards that are assessable at the local level only.

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Just in Time to Flip Your Classroom Nathaniel Lasry, Michael Dugdale & Elizabeth Charles

Answer Key For The California Mathematics Standards Grade 1

Control and Boundedness

South Carolina English Language Arts

This scope and sequence assumes 160 days for instruction, divided among 15 units.

POLA: a student modeling framework for Probabilistic On-Line Assessment of problem solving performance

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

Abstractions and the Brain

PUBLIC CASE REPORT Use of the GeoGebra software at upper secondary school

arxiv: v1 [math.at] 10 Jan 2016

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program

Exploring Derivative Functions using HP Prime

Evaluating the Effectiveness of the Strategy Draw a Diagram as a Cognitive Tool for Problem Solving

Introducing the New Iowa Assessments Mathematics Levels 12 14

EXPLORING INSIGHT: FOCUS ON SHIFTS OF ATTENTION

Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 (2010)

Guest Editorial Motivating Growth of Mathematics Knowledge for Teaching: A Case for Secondary Mathematics Teacher Education

ACADEMIC AFFAIRS GUIDELINES

Designing a Rubric to Assess the Modelling Phase of Student Design Projects in Upper Year Engineering Courses

Mathematics. Mathematics

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Reading Horizons. Organizing Reading Material into Thought Units to Enhance Comprehension. Kathleen C. Stevens APRIL 1983

University of Exeter College of Humanities. Assessment Procedures 2010/11

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

Documenting the Knowledge of Low-Attaining Third- and Fourth- Graders: Robyn s and Bel s Sequential Structure and Multidigit Addition and Subtraction

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

Do students benefit from drawing productive diagrams themselves while solving introductory physics problems? The case of two electrostatic problems

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

The recognition, evaluation and accreditation of European Postgraduate Programmes.

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

Primary Teachers Perceptions of Their Knowledge and Understanding of Measurement

10.2. Behavior models

Focus of the Unit: Much of this unit focuses on extending previous skills of multiplication and division to multi-digit whole numbers.

Mental Computation Strategies for Part-Whole Numbers

By. Candra Pantura Panlaysia Dr. CH. Evy Tri Widyahening, S.S., M.Hum Slamet Riyadi University Surakarta ABSTRACT

P. Belsis, C. Sgouropoulou, K. Sfikas, G. Pantziou, C. Skourlas, J. Varnas

Artemeva, N 2006 Approaches to Leaning Genre: a bibliographical essay. Artemeva & Freedman

Language properties and Grammar of Parallel and Series Parallel Languages

Pre-Algebra A. Syllabus. Course Overview. Course Goals. General Skills. Credit Value

Approaches to Teaching Second Language Writing Brian PALTRIDGE, The University of Sydney

TEACHER'S TRAINING IN A STATISTICS TEACHING EXPERIMENT 1

On-Line Data Analytics

Transcription:

INFINITE AND UNBOUNDED SETS: A PRAGMATIC PERSPECTIVE Cristina Bardelle University of Eastern Piedmont A. Avogadro, Alessandria, Italy This paper explores some of the ambiguities inherent in the notions of finite/infinite sets and bounded/unbounded sets for what concern Euclidean spaces. The study, carried out with seven mathematics students, shows that wrong attitude towards mathematical language is a major source of misconceptions about cardinality and boundedness. In particular, everyday use of mathematical terms combined with a lack of coordination of different representations proves to be a hindrance in the conceptualization of basic properties of subsets of metric spaces. INTRODUCTION In the last years research in mathematics education has pointed out the central role played by language in the learning of mathematics and has addressed this topic from a variety of different perspectives. The complexity of mathematical language does not refer to the symbolic component only, but it depends on all semiotic representations used in mathematics such as symbolic notations, diagrams, figures and so on, including the verbal component. From the point of view of semiotics many studies stressed the importance of treatment within the same semiotic system and of the translation between different representations of mathematical objects as well (Janvier et al., 1987; Duval, 1993). From the functional linguistic viewpoint as many studies developed to highlight the role of context in the learning of mathematics. Some constructs and ideas from pragmatics were exploited by authors (see e.g. Ferrari, 001, 004; Morgan, 1998; Pimm, 1987) in order to provide theoretical frameworks to interpret difficulties in the learning of mathematics. For example Ferrari (004, p. 384) claimed that students competence in ordinary language and in the specific languages used in mathematics are other sources of troubles. In particular some studies pointed out difficulties arising from the overlapping of everyday language and mathematical language (see e.g. Bardelle, 010; Cornu, 1981; Ferrari, 004; Kim et al., 005; Mason & Pimm, 1984; Tall 1977). But not enough attention has been paid to the fact that mathematical terms are often borrowed from everyday language and used with meanings different from everyday-life usage. The aim of this paper is to further investigate this subject. In particular here we deal with the concept of boundedness and infiniteness of subsets of Euclidean spaces. The study of metric spaces and their properties is basic in the curricula of mathematics undergraduate students and the

concept of boundedness is fundamental for the learning of other topological properties such as compactness. Research questions The presented research investigates some typical students behaviours concerning the recognition of bounded or unbounded subsets of n, endowed with the Euclidean distance. For example, some students seemingly justify that a set is unbounded because it has infinite elements. In particular the study explores the interplay of pragmatic aspects with the flexibility in switching between different representations of bounded/unbounded sets and finite/infinite sets. Do the everyday meanings of the words finite/infinite and bounded/unbounded influence students behaviour in the resolution of problems involving such words? Which is the role of the formal definition of bounded/unbounded set and finite/infinite set? What are the elements (problem formulation, system of representation of the set, context, etc.) that could influence the meaning of bounded/unbounded set and finite/infinite set used by students? THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK Functional approach Functional linguistics is a part of linguistics that studies language in relation to its functions rather than to its form, and pragmatics is a general functional (i.e. cognitive, social and cultural) perspective on language and language use, aimed at the investigation of processes of dynamic and negotiated meaning generation in interaction. Here I adopt Halliday s definition of register (Halliday 1985), which has been thoroughly discussed by Leckie-Tarry (1995). A register denotes a linguistic variety based on use that is a conventional pattern or configuration of language that corresponds to a variety of situations or contexts. Ferrari (004), following Leckie- Tarry (1995), distinguishes between colloquial registers and literate registers. The first ones refer to linguistic resources adopted in spoken communications prevalently but also in informal written communication such as sms messages, e-mails, etc. whereas the second ones refer to written-for-others texts mainly such as books but also to formal spoken communication such as in academic lessons. Ferrari (004, p.387) argues that the registers customarily adopted in advanced mathematics share a number of features with literate registers and may be regarded as extreme forms of them.

Semiotic representations in the concept image and concept definition As mentioned before, registers used in mathematics are highly literate; in fact, the development of mathematics but also its teaching and learning requires the introduction of a variety of representations such as symbolic notations, verbal texts, geometrical figures, diagrams and so on. According to Duval (1993) there cannot be noésis without sémiosis, where sémiosis denotes the production of a semiotic representation and noésis denotes the conceptual learning of an object. Moreover Duval states that the cognitive functioning of human thought needs multiple semiotic systems and he applied his ideas to the learning of mathematics where very different representations occur. Duval makes a distinction between treatment of a representation, which is a transformation (manipulation) within the same semiotic system, and conversion (translation) between different semiotic systems 1. For example, computing the sum of two fractions is an example of treatment, whereas translating a fraction into an equivalent decimal expansion is an example of conversion. A good coordination of semiotic representations is, in my opinion, fundamental for the development of a proper concept image (Tall & Vinner, 1981). This term is used to describe the total cognitive structure that is associated with the concept, which includes all the mental pictures and associated properties and processes (Tall & Vinner, 1981, p. 15). The generation of the concept image of an individual can be influenced also by the concept definition that is a form of words used to specify that concept (Tall & Vinner, 1981, p. 15). The concept definition can be different from the formal concept definition, i.e. a definition accepted by the mathematical community at large. Moreover Tall and Vinner introduced the evoked concept image as the portion of the concept image which is activated at a particular time. The concept image may have conflicting aspects that may be evoked at different times. When such conflicting aspects are evoked simultaneously they cause a cognitive conflict. My claim is that the ability in the use of mathematical language in particular for what concerns the understanding of its purposes and a good coordination of semiotic representations is fundamental for a proper coherent generation of the concept image without conflicting factors. THE EXPERIMENT The research involved seven second year undergraduate mathematics students at the University of Eastern Piedmont in Italy. The students were attending a Geometry course focused on point set topology and on introductory algebraic topology. Students already encountered the concept of finite/infinite set and of 1 Duval used the term register referring to a semiotic system; here register is used with its pragmatic meaning only.

bounded/unbounded set in n, endowed with the Euclidean metric, during the first year. In this second year course the definition of finite/infinite set is considered well known, whereas the definition of boundedness of a set is generalized to an arbitrary metric space, paying particular attention for n with Euclidean metric. This research focused on Euclidean spaces only. The data were collected from individual interviews. The interviews were semi-structured and based on two different kinds of questions. Q1-questions n The first kind of questions were aimed to recognize if a given set in with Euclidean metric is unbounded and infinite. The sets were given by their graphical representation or by symbolic notations and in this case, whenever it was useful and possible, students were encouraged to represent it in the Cartesian coordinate system. Some examples of given sets are 1 nn, 0, ( x, y) : xy 1, 3 ( x, y, z) : x y 1, z 3, etc. Q-questions ( x, y) : x y 1, ( x, y) : y x, ( x, y) : y sin x, 3 ( x, y, z) : x y 3, The second kind of questions were aimed to understand the meaning of finite/infinite set and bounded/unbounded set used to answer to Q1-questions. Students were asked to say what is an infinite and unbounded set or to produce some examples. The interviews were carried out in order to help students to achieve a proper knowledge about the topics as well as to identify their behaviours and their concept image. In particular, Q-questions investigated their concept definition and Q1- questions were aimed to study the influence of the symbolic and graphical representation on their concept image. SELECTED FINDINGS Just two students (students A, D) out of seven showed no kind of problems dealing with the topic of this study. The remaining students presented difficulties that seem, as we shall see shortly, to be due to their lack of coordination of representations of subsets of n and to the adoption of colloquial registers. In what follows only the responses about the sets ( x, y) : x y 1 and 1 nn, 0 are presented. This choice is due to the fact that the students are familiar with them and the conversion in their graphical representation is within their reach and without requiring a previous treatment of the symbolic representation, which would be beyond the purpose of this study. Some results concerning more

examples of sets, which were provided ad hoc to some of the students in order to better grasp their concept image, are also presented. Table 1 summarizes the students answers concerning the boundedness of the set ( x, y) : x y 1. Student ( x, y) : x y 1 Explanation A bounded ( x, y) : x y 1 1,1 1,1 B unbounded x y 1 has infinite solutions, therefore the set is unbounded C bounded It is a circumference, it is bounded D bounded It is a circumference and hence it is bounded E bounded ( x, y) : x y 1 1,1 1,1 F bounded It is a circumference, it is bounded G bounded It is a circumference, it is bounded Table 1: Answers to the question on the boundedness of x, y : x y 1 The first thing to say is that all students, except B, recognized at a first glance that ( x, y) : x y 1 is a circumference in the plane and were able to represent it graphically. Students A and E showed a literate use of mathematical language (literate register) and preferred the symbolic representation. The remaining students, except B, were not able to explain why the set is bounded. Therefore such students, among the others, were encouraged to explain when a set is bounded/unbounded. The set 1 nn, 0 revealed itself to be more troublesome for students. Table summarizes the answers about its boundedness. Students 1, n, n 0 n Explanation A bounded 1, n, n 0 0,1 n B unbounded It is unbounded because n varies in C unbounded It is unbounded on the right the naturals are infinite and hence there is no end for this set

D unbounded It is unbounded because there are infinite n E bounded It is contained in 0,1 F bounded The set is bounded from above and below G unbounded It is not bounded because you get infinite fractions Table : Answers to the question on the boundedness of 1 n, n, n 0 Only three students answered correctly. Since the explanation of student F, even if correct, seemed to be not so usual as a topologic argument, some investigations were conducted in his interview (see below). The remaining three students, among the others, were encouraged also after this question to give the definition of bounded/unbounded set and of finite/infinite set. In this case some students evoked a concept definition which was different from the one evoked after the first question. Table 3 summarized the responses of students about their concept definition of the boundedness of a set. Student What is a bounded/unbounded set in A A set X is bounded if there exists a ball B such that X B B She was not able to give a definition but she gave the closed interval 0,1 as an example of bounded set in unbounded set in n? and the interval 0,1 as an example of. She could not come up with an example in C A set X is bounded if there exists a ball B such that X B D She answered An unbounded set is an infinite set when she referred to sets that she could not visualize She answered A bounded set is a set placed in a limited space when she referred to sets that she could visualize E A set X is bounded if there exists a ball B such that X B F G A set X is bounded if there exists a ball B such that X Bor a product of intervals that contains it She answered An unbounded set is an infinite set when she referred to sets that she could not visualize She answered A set is bounded when, from the drawing, it is in a narrow space when she referred to sets that she could visualize Table 3: Answers to the question on the concept definition of bounded/unbounded set

For what concern the concept of finite/infinite set all students grasped its correct meaning. All students did not give the formal definition but used a more colloquial (colloquial register) but effective argument as showed in Table 4. Student What is a finite/infinite set in all n? All students answered that a finite set is a set with a finite number of points (or elements) Table 4: Answers to the question on the concept definition of finite/infinite set Data shows that students use an everyday meaning for the boundedness of a set even if their concept definition is correct and given with a literate register. Such a meaning is sufficient to give an answer to problems where the set is already given in the Cartesian coordinate system or students can sketch its graphical representation. In this case the definition of bounded used in the everyday meaning in Italian language that is something that has limits referring to space or time clearly fits with the concept definition of students D and G. Moreover, it seems that also students C, F adopted this colloquial meaning since they are not able, at least apparently, to provide an explanation to the boundedness of the circumference (Table 1) even if their concept definition coincide with the formal one (Table 3). One has to highlight that this everyday meaning is not more sufficient in order to answer to problem 1 n, n, n 0 (Table ). In this case all the students who gave an incorrect explanation (B, C, D, G) did not think to sketch the set but worked only with the symbolic system. The symbolic aspect of the representation of this set seemed to evoke in these students the idea of infinite elements connected to the set of natural numbers, indeed a set that tends to infinity and hence with no upper bound. Here the matter is not that students did not understand the concept of infinite set but that the first evoked meaning of infinite in the context of the symbolic representation of this set that they could not visualize at a first glance is a colloquial one that is something that never ends with reference to space and time indeed something which is unbounded. The problem is not even that they could not represent it graphically. Indeed, the students were asked to sketch it and they did it correctly (sometimes with some help). After that they recognized that they were dealing with a bounded set. Notice that student B applied this reasoning also for the circumference since she could not sketch it (Table 1). During the interview students were asked to explain their apparently incoherent behaviour. Some examples of responses are: I used infinite in order to justify that a set was unbounded because I imagined an infinite set as a set stretching to infinity (student G). I think more to the idea of infinity in order to decide about the boundedness of a set (student C).

The research proves also the everyday usage of the term finite by student D as one can see from the following transcript: 1 I: Is ( x, y) : x y 1 unbounded? D: It is a circumference [she draws it]... it is bounded 3 I: It is infinite? 4 D: It is closed, not infinite. 5 I: What does it mean that a set is finite? 6 D: It is limited in space. 7 I: Is the set [in its graphical representation] y x unbounded? 8 D: Yes, it is 9 I: It is infinite/finite? 10 D: It is finite 11 I: Can you find points belonging to it? 1 D: yes, for example 1,1, 1 3,1 3, etcetera, 1 n,1 n..no, then it s infinite! 13 I: Why did you answer finite before? 14 D: Because it seems finite from the drawing. Finally the research highlights another kind of behaviour due to a misleading interpretation of mathematical language. Student F showed no kind of problems with sets like in Table 1,. Problems arose with sets like ( x, y) : y sin x, ( x, y) : xy 1, ( x, y) : y arctg x, etc. where functions are involved. In this case the recognition of well known functions (both graphical and symbolic representations) evoked a meaning of boundedness related to functions rather than sets. The following transcript shows this fact: 1 I: Is ( x, y) : y sin x bounded or unbounded? D: bounded because it can assume values between -1 and 1 After some explanation about his wrong answer he declared I think that the set x y [ ( x, y) : y sin x ] was in.. if I see something simple like 8 8 or y sin x I don t see the other particulars. Finally the formal concept definition of bounded/unbounded set was showed to students (B, D, G) that did not manage to provide it and they were asked if they

remembered it. Student B answered that she could not remember it and the other two that they knew it but they did not think to use it. DISCUSSION AND TEACHING IMPLICATIONS This paper provides an example of a wrong use of mathematical language by some Italian mathematics undergraduate students. In particular, these students do not recognize the importance of mathematical definitions. Mathematical terms, such as bounded/unbounded and finite/infinite, evoke different meanings at different times. This is a typical thinking habit that concerns the everyday-life language. In mathematical language, as well as scientific languages in general, terms are usually coined with one meaning only in order to avoid interpretative problems. This purpose should be shared with the students in order to prevent improper use of mathematical language. The argument.a goal of mathematics education as concerns language is promoting flexibility in the use of registers and awareness of the relationships between linguistic forms and context and purposes. This is by no means spontaneous but must be carefully promoted (Ferrari, 00, p.354) applies to the outcomes of this research. Moreover since data showed that different systems of representation evoke different concept images another goal of mathematics education should be promoting flexibility in switching from one representation to another. In my opinion a good practice of teaching in order to promote an appropriate concept image of the boundedness of sets should include, besides to the formal definition, examples of sets satisfying the definition as well as examples of sets that do not verify it. The choice of examples has to be done in order to evoke possible conflicts between the concept image and the concept definition, for examples such those presented in this paper. Moreover, when possible, sets have to be presented in at least two kinds of semiotic representations. Finally, a good practice should include the assignment of exercises that require, as a first step, the description of the concept definition in order to help students in focusing on the correct meaning of the concept. REFERENCES Bardelle, C. (010). Interpreting monotonicity of functions: a semiotic perspective. In Pinto, M. F. & Kawasaki, T. F. (Eds.), Proc. of the 34th Conf. of the Int. Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. (pp. 105-11). Belo Horizonte, Brazil: PME. Cornu, B. (1981). Apprentissage de la notion de limite: modèles spontanés et modèles propres. Actes du Cinquième Colloque du Groupe Internationale PME (pp. 3-36). Grenoble, France. Duval, R. (1993). Registres de représentations sémiotique et fonctionnement cognitif de la pensée. Ann. de Didactique et de Sciences Cognitives, ULP, IREM Strasbourg. 5, 37-65.

Ferrari, P.L. (001). Understanding Elementary Number Theory at the Undergraduate Level: A Semiotic Approach. In Campbell, S.R. & Zazkis R. (Eds.) Learning and Teaching Number Theory: Research in Cognition and Instruction, (pp.97-115). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing. Ferrari, P.L. (00). Developing language through communication and conversion of semiotic systems. In Cockburn, A.D. & Nardi E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6 th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. (pp.353-360). Norwich, UK: PME. Ferrari, P.L. (004). Mathematical language and advanced mathematics learning. In Høines M. J. & Fuglestad A. B. (Eds.), Proc. 8 th Conf. of the Int. Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. (pp. 383-390). Bergen, Norway: PME. Halliday, M.A.K., & Hasan, R. (1985). Language, context, and text: aspects of language in a social-semiotic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Janvier, C. (Ed.). (1987). Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kim D., Sfard A., Ferrini-Mundy J. (005). Students colloquial and mathematical discourses on infinity and limit. In Chick, H. L. & Vincent, J. L. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. 3 (pp. 01-08). Melbourne: PME. Leckie-Tarry, H. (1995). Language & context. A functional linguistic theory of register. London: Pinter Publishers. Mason, J. & Pimm, D. (1984). Generic examples: seeing the general in the particular, Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15, 77-89. Morgan, C. (1998). Writing Mathematically. The Discourse of Investigation. London: Falmer Press. Pimm, D. (1987). Speaking Mathematically: Communication in Mathematics Classrooms. London: Routledge Kegan and Paul. Tall, D. (1977). Cognitive Conflict and the Learning of Mathematics. Proceedings of the First Conference of The International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Utrecht, Netherlands: PME. Tall, D., Vinner, S. (1981). Concept image and concept definition in mathematics with particular reference to limits and continuity. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 1, 151-169.