Brian Butterworth a a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London. Available online: 30 Mar 2012

Similar documents
To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Exact Equality and Successor Function : Two Keys Concepts on the Path towards Understanding Exact Numbers

Zealand Published online: 16 Jun To link to this article:

Philip Hallinger a & Arild Tjeldvoll b a Hong Kong Institute of Education. To link to this article:

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

UCLA Issues in Applied Linguistics

A Model of Knower-Level Behavior in Number Concept Development

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Mathematics Education

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

University of Groningen. Systemen, planning, netwerken Bosman, Aart

Abstractions and the Brain

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Document number: 2013/ Programs Committee 6/2014 (July) Agenda Item 42.0 Bachelor of Engineering with Honours in Software Engineering

Published online: 26 Mar 2010.

Andrew S. Paney a a Department of Music, University of Mississippi, 164 Music. Building, Oxford, MS 38655, USA Published online: 14 Nov 2014.

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

MMOG Subscription Business Models: Table of Contents

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

+32 (0)

Using Moodle in ESOL Writing Classes

Computerized Adaptive Psychological Testing A Personalisation Perspective

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Helma W. Oolbekkink Marchand a, Jan H. van Driel b & Nico Verloop b a Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Published online: 24 Jan 2007.

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

Mental Models and the Meaning of Connectives: A Study on Children, Adolescents and Adults

Routledge Library Editions: The English Language: Pronouns And Word Order In Old English: With Particular Reference To The Indefinite Pronoun Man

Extending Place Value with Whole Numbers to 1,000,000

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Spring 2016 Stony Brook University Instructor: Dr. Paul Fodor

AQUA: An Ontology-Driven Question Answering System

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

TAIWANESE STUDENT ATTITUDES TOWARDS AND BEHAVIORS DURING ONLINE GRAMMAR TESTING WITH MOODLE

Cal s Dinner Card Deals

Key concepts for the insider-researcher

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 )

Concept mapping instrumental support for problem solving

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Jeryl D. Benson EdD OTR/L a, Joyce Salls OTD OTR/L b & Cora Perry MS OTR/L c a Occupational Therapy Department, Duquesne University,

THE ROLE OF TOOL AND TEACHER MEDIATIONS IN THE CONSTRUCTION OF MEANINGS FOR REFLECTION

Introduction to Psychology

A Practical Introduction to Teacher Training in ELT

New Venture Financing

TOURISM ECONOMICS AND POLICY (ASPECTS OF TOURISM) BY LARRY DWYER, PETER FORSYTH, WAYNE DWYER

Save Children. Can Math Recovery. before They Fail?

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

Cross Country Comparison of Scholarly E-Reading Patterns in Australia, Finland, and the United States

Journal Article Growth and Reading Patterns

Available online: 03 Nov 2011

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

Second Language Acquisition in Adults: From Research to Practice

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Statistical Analysis of Climate Change, Renewable Energies, and Sustainability An Independent Investigation for Introduction to Statistics

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

Show and Tell Persuasion

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Cognitive Apprenticeship Statewide Campus System, Michigan State School of Osteopathic Medicine 2011

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Daniel Reinholz a a Center for STEM Learning, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA Published online: 11 Feb 2015.

An Asset-Based Approach to Linguistic Diversity

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Advancing the Discipline of Leadership Studies. What is an Academic Discipline?

Summary results (year 1-3)

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

Global Seminar Quito, Ecuador Language, Culture & Child Development. EDS 115 GS Cognitive Development & Education Summer Session I, 2016

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Kendriya Vidyalaya Sangathan

NORMAL AND ABNORMAL DEVELOPMENT OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOUR

Accounting & Financial Management

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

IS FINANCIAL LITERACY IMPROVED BY PARTICIPATING IN A STOCK MARKET GAME?

Evaluation of Hybrid Online Instruction in Sport Management

Approaches to Teaching Second Language Writing Brian PALTRIDGE, The University of Sydney

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

A Metacognitive Approach to Support Heuristic Solution of Mathematical Problems

2 Research Developments

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 12 December 2011 ISSN

Biomedical Sciences (BC98)

Statewide Framework Document for:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

CONSULTATION ON THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE COMPETENCY STANDARD FOR LICENSED IMMIGRATION ADVISERS

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams

10.2. Behavior models

Transcription:

This article was downloaded by: [University College London] On: 04 April 2012, At: 09:07 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Language Learning and Development Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hlld20 Commentary on How Can Syntax Support Number Word Acquisition? by Kristen Syrett, Julien Musolino, and Rochel Gelman Brian Butterworth a a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London Available online: 30 Mar 2012 To cite this article: Brian Butterworth (2012): Commentary on How Can Syntax Support Number Word Acquisition? by Kristen Syrett, Julien Musolino, and Rochel Gelman, Language Learning and Development, 8:2, 186-189 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.609129 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Language Learning and Development, 8: 186 189, 2012 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1547-5441 print / 1547-3341 online DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2011.609129 Commentary on How Can Syntax Support Number Word Acquisition? by Kristen Syrett, Julien Musolino, and Rochel Gelman Brian Butterworth Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London What role does language play in developing the concept of number? This question is at the center of an important current debate. To try to answer it, let us consider what is needed to learn number words and their meaning. First, the learner has to be able to identify number words as such, that is, to distinguish them from other sorts of word. Second, the learner has to have number meanings available. Third, the learner has to be able to map the words on to their meanings. The question addressed in the paper by Syrett, Musolini, and Gelman (SMG) is the first one: How does a child identify number words? In particular, can the child generalize from other words, especially nonnumerical quantifiers? In particular, it is assessment of the claim by Bloom and Wynn (1997) that a collection of linguistic cues [in the input] may play a significant role in children s acquisition of number word meaning (p.514) and that sensitivity to these different linguistic cues brings children to their initial stage of number word acquisition (knowledge that number words pick out numerosities) (p. 515). It has been argued by Carey that the ease of mastering determiners and other quantifiers, as well as evidence for prelinguistic availability of the singularity/plurality distinction, suggests that the meanings that underlie the quantifiers of natural language are also part of the human innate endowment (Carey, 2009, p. 238). However, she also notes that these cannot be the basis for constructing representations of what she calls integers (meaning cardinals), since they do not express exact cardinal values of sets (p. 241). In fact, English quantifiers, such as some, all, many, several, and so on, do not denote a specific number of objects, whereas the counting words one, two, three, etc., do. (An added complication is that quantifiers such as some and all, not to mention determiners such as the, can denote continuous quantities rather than sets and subsets, as in the expression some cake, some of the cake, the flour weighs two kilos.) So the issue here is, can the child get from quantifiers to counting words without the help of pre-existing concepts of cardinality? Carey argues for bootstrapping, that is, pulling oneself up by one s bootstraps. This, of course, is impossible (as a simple experiment will demonstrate), even in its Quineian version. So one must look elsewhere for an account. Correspondence should be addressed to Brian Butterworth, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom. E-mail: b.butterworth@ucl.ac.uk

SYNTAX AND NUMBER WORD ACQUISITION 187 It is important to understand the role of the counting words a child will experience. Of course, the words are used for counting, but the practice of counting involves the integration of two separate meanings: cardinality and ordinality. Cardinality is an abstract property of a set how many things there are in a set. This is in contradistinction to ordinality the order of the number words. In the former, the cardinality fourness includes the cardinalities of the threeness, twoness, and oneness, while in the latter four is the next element after three in a well-ordered sequence but does not include three. Of course, the fact that in English the words for the cardinal four and ordinal four are the same, poses a problem for the learner that takes some unravelling, as many authors have noted (e.g., Fuson, 1988, 1992). This question has become a divisive issue in studies of the development numerical competence. On the one hand, it has been widely claimed that knowing the vocabulary of number words is a necessary condition for developing concepts of cardinality (sometimes misleadingly called exact number, since both cardinals and ordinals are exact (e.g., Carey, 2004; Le Corre & Carey, 2007; Pica, Lemer, Izard, & Dehaene, 2004). On the other hand, as we will see, the cardinal concepts are independent of language, and the child s task is to learn the mapping between the words and concepts (Gelman & Gallistel, 1978). The focus of SMG is the prior issue of how the child comes to be able to identify words as referring to numbers, which of course is necessary on either type of account, but in the former is necessary for the development of cardinal concepts. CARDINALITY There are two aspects to a grasp of the cardinal meanings of number words. First, the learner has to understand that four mean fourness and, second, that cardinals have well-defined properties. Piaget (1952) identified an important one in his conservation tasks, namely, that some transformations affect cardinal value and some do not. In particular, he differentiated transformations that change cardinality, for example, by adding or taking away a member, from transformations that do not change cardinality, such as rearrangement of members. He also stressed that if two sets have the same (cardinal) number, then their members could be put into one-to-one correspondence. In coming to understand the concept of [cardinal] number, knowledge of the counting words seemed to be largely irrelevant. The critical point here is that in this account, knowledge of counting words contributes little to coming to an understanding of cardinality. Several authors have also taken this view. For example, Fodor (1975) argues that learning a language essentially means learning to express concepts already present in their prelinguistic systems for representing the world. Gelman and Gallistel (1978) have also stressed that learning to count using counting words consists of mapping the words on to prelinguistic concepts of cardinality ( numerons ). One might have assumed that a commitment to a nativist account of concepts would lead to a nativist account of cardinal concepts. Bloom and Wynn (1997) as well Carey and her colleagues take a nonnativist position here. They are essentially arguing, as SMG note, for Skills before Principles approach not too different from an essentially empiricist position of the acquisition of number concepts advocated by some developmental psychologists (Briars & Siegler, 1984; Mix, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2002) or philosophers such as Kitcher (1984). See (Butterworth & Reeve, in press) for a discussion of this issue.

188 BUTTERWORTH NOTICING AND APPLYING The criteria for crediting children with knowledge are different for Gelman and Piaget. While Piaget focused on the application required to solve problems, Gelman focused on noticing numerical events. Gelman (1972) was among the first researchers to use so-called noticing tasks to demonstrate children s reactions to number events. In the last 20 years, it has become something of a developmental game to show that younger and younger children are sensitive to particular concepts to illustrate the basis of specific competencies. That is, children notice systematicities or violations of systematicities. We need to consider, though, the epistemological status of noticing indices, including, for example, the relationship between noticing and later application indices of number and the developmental sequence(s) that characterize the transition from noticing to action. Of course, these are not new questions; however, they are worth revisiting as a way of examining claims about the development number competencies (Butterworth & Reeve, in press). SMG tackle the question of whether children notice the syntactic context of quantifiers. The second question is, can they apply that knowledge in interpreting counting words as having a quantificational meaning? Even if the answer to both questions is yes, there is still the question of getting from the nonspecific quantificational nature of words such as some, many, and several to the specific cardinal meanings of one, two, and so on. As noted above, it is true that many and several denote properties of sets of discrete objects. Other quantifiers such as some and all can also denote quantities that are not sets or subsets of them. May I have some cake and Please give me all the cake denote continuous quantities, not members of a set of cakes. Even the partitive of, which is crucial in this study, can denote both continuous quantity, some of the cake, as well as a subset of a previously mentioned or presupposed set, some of the cakes. Also, these quantifiers are not intrinsically ordered, unlike the counting words. EVIDENCE OF THE INDEPENDENCE OF NUMBER AND LANGUAGE The original position taken by (Gelman & Gallistel, 1978) entailed an innate capacity for cardinality that was independent of counting words. There is now extensive evidence from comparative studies that human infants (Antell & Keating, 1983; Wynn, 1992) and other species (Davis & Pérusse, 1988; Gallistel, 1990; Nieder & Dehaene, 2009), lacking counting words, notice changes in numerosity and can sometimes apply this knowledge in guiding behavior. Moreover, cultures without counting words can nevertheless have a sense of the exact equality of sets (Izard, Pica, Spelke, & Dehaene, 2008) and even carry out enumeration and simple arithmetical tasks (Butterworth & Reeve, 2008; Butterworth, Reeve, Reynolds, & Lloyd, 2008). Even though the Amazonian Mundurucu use their few counting words in a rather approximate way, some members do use body-part counting for exact enumeration (Pica et al., 2004, supplementary information), again suggesting the independence of a concept of cardinality from the language resources. Now these languages do have quantifier words. Warlpiri and other central desert languages of Australia have grammatical markers for dual and trial (typically meaning afew) (Bittner & Hale, 1995). If the claim is that the syntax of quantifiers drives an understanding of cardinality

SYNTAX AND NUMBER WORD ACQUISITION 189 and cardinalities, then one might have thought that the Warlpiri would use these quantifiers in a systematic way to enumerate, for example singular definite, dual, trial, several, and many. But there is no evidence that these quantifiers are used in this way in these cultures. SMG demonstrate quite clearly that even the first step, being able to use syntactic context to identify words that could have cardinal meanings for the early learner, lacks convincing support. It seems more plausible to me that counting words are learned in counting contexts, contexts that are specifically cardinal, as in games and activities where the adult or older child says, Here are three dinosaurs and [adding one] now there are fours dinosaurs. REFERENCES Antell, S. E., & Keating, D. P. (1983). Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child Development, 54, 695 701. Bittner, M., & Hale, K. (1995). Remarks on definiteness in Warlpiri. In E. Bach (Ed.), Quantification in natural languages (pp. 81 107). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Bloom, P., & Wynn, K. (1997). Linguistic cues in the acquisition of number words. Journal of Child Language, 24, 511 533. Briars, D. J., & Siegler, R. S. (1984). A featural analysis of preschoolers counting knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 20, 607 618. Butterworth, B., & Reeve, R. (2008). Verbal counting and spatial strategies in numerical tasks: Evidence from indigenous Australia. Philosophical Psychology, 21, 443 457. Butterworth, B., & Reeve, R. (in press). Counting words and a principles-after account of the development of number concepts. In M. Siegal & L. Surian (Eds.), Access to language and cognitive development. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Butterworth, B., Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Numerical thought with and without words: Evidence from indigenous Australian children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105, 13179 13184. doi: 10.1073pnas.0806045105 Carey, S. (2004). On the origin of concepts. Daedulus, 133(1), 59 68. Carey, S. (2009). Where our number concepts come from. Journal of Philosophy, 106(4), 220 254. Davis, H., & Pérusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 561 579. Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. New York, NY: Thomas Y. Crowell. Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children s counting and concepts of number. New York, NY: Springer Verlag. Fuson, K. C. (1992). Relationships between counting and cardinality from age 2 to 8. In J. Bideaud, C. Meljac, & J. P. Fisher (Eds.), Pathways to number, children s developing numerical abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Gelman, R. (1972). Logical capacity of very young children: Number invariance rules. Child Development, 43, 75 90. Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child s understanding of number (2 nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Izard, V., Pica, P., Spelke, E. S., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Exact equality and successor function: Two key concepts on the path towards understanding exact numbers. Philosophical Psychology, 21(4), 491 505. doi: 10.1080/09515080802285354 Kitcher, P. (1984). The nature of mathematical knowledge. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105(2), 395 438. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005 Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. (2002). Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press. Nieder, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32(1), 185 208. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135550 Piaget, J. (1952). The child s conception of number. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate calculation in an Amazonian indigene group with a reduced number lexicon. Science, 306, 499 503. Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749 751.