Zwart (1996). Verb Clusters in Continental West Germanic Dialects

Similar documents
Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Treebank mining with GrETEL. Liesbeth Augustinus Frank Van Eynde

Citation for published version (APA): Veenstra, M. J. A. (1998). Formalizing the minimalist program Groningen: s.n.

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Words come in categories

Parasitic participles and ellipsis in VP-focus pseudoclefts. Jan-Wouter Zwart

VERB MOVEMENT The Status of the Weak Pronouns in Dutch

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Developing a TT-MCTAG for German with an RCG-based Parser

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

LNGT0101 Introduction to Linguistics

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Control and Boundedness

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

A comment on the topic of topic comment

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Argument structure and theta roles

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Why Are There No Directionality Parameters?

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Advanced Grammar in Use

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

1/20 idea. We ll spend an extra hour on 1/21. based on assigned readings. so you ll be ready to discuss them in class

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

Direct and Indirect Passives in East Asian. C.-T. James Huang Harvard University

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Som and Optimality Theory

Advanced Topics in HPSG

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Intensive English Program Southwest College

Agree or Move? On Partial Control Anna Snarska, Adam Mickiewicz University

EAGLE: an Error-Annotated Corpus of Beginning Learner German

Writing a composition

Teacher: Mlle PERCHE Maeva High School: Lycée Charles Poncet, Cluses (74) Level: Seconde i.e year old students

Two$Asymmetries$between$Pre0$and$Post0Head$Order$ and$their$implications$for$syntactic$theory$

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Welcome to the Purdue OWL. Where do I begin? General Strategies. Personalizing Proofreading

Pronominal doubling in Dutch dialects: big DPs and coordinations

The Effects of Ability Tracking of Future Primary School Teachers on Student Performance

linguist 752 UMass Amherst 8 February 2017

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Copyright Corwin 2015

Dependency, licensing and the nature of grammatical relations *

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

MA Linguistics Language and Communication

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

Participate in expanded conversations and respond appropriately to a variety of conversational prompts

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

A Usage-Based Approach to Recursion in Sentence Processing

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Heads and history NIGEL VINCENT & KERSTI BÖRJARS The University of Manchester

Disharmonic Word Order from a Processing Typology Perspective. John A. Hawkins, U of Cambridge RCEAL & UC Davis Linguistics

Hindi-Urdu Phrase Structure Annotation

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

On the Notion Determiner

- «Crede Experto:,,,». 2 (09) ( '36

"f TOPIC =T COMP COMP... OBJ

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

18 The syntax phonology interface

The Lexical Representation of Light Verb Constructions

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Long-distance wh-movement. Long distance wh-movement. Islands. Islands. Locality. NP Sea. NP Sea

Freitag 7. Januar = QUIZ = REFLEXIVE VERBEN = IM KLASSENZIMMER = JUDD 115

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

CORPUS ANALYSIS CORPUS ANALYSIS QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

ON THE SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS

Tagged for Deletion: A Typological Approach to VP Ellipsis in Tag Questions

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

Full text of O L O W Science As Inquiry conference. Science as Inquiry

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

University of Groningen. Topics in Corpus-Based Dutch Syntax Beek, Leonoor Johanneke van der

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

Developing Grammar in Context

The distribution of school funding and inputs in England:

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Prediction of Maximal Projection for Semantic Role Labeling

Transcription:

Zwart (1996). Verb Clusters in Continental West Germanic Dialects Denis Brumann <dbrumann@gmail.com> Universität Leipzig Institut für Linguistik 1 1 Introduction (1) a. (Ik wil) dat hij het boek leest I want that he the book reads I want him to read the book b. *(Ik wil) dat hij leest het boek c. (Hij wil) het boek lezen He wants the book read-inf He wants to read the book. d. *(Hij wil) lezen het boek word order in (1a) and (1c) considered to reect the deep structure ordering of the meaningful elements; the SVO order of main clauses derived by movement of the nite verb to the second position in the sentence should be construed as in (2): (1a) and (Ic) may be derived by movement of the object noun phrase to the left (4) a. (Ik wil) dat Jan het boek snel leest I want that John the book fast reads I want John to read the book quickly. Arguments: nite complement clauses invariably follow the verb, also in embedded clauses and innitival constructions: (5) a. (Hij denkt) dat ik wil dat hij het boek leest He thinks that I want that he the book reads He thinks that I want him to read the book. b. *(Hij denkt) dat ik dat hij het boek leest wil Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 1

2 Verb clusters in Continental West Germanic elementary cases: an auxiliary verb has a past participle in its complement domain a modal, causative, or perception verb has an innitive in its complement domain more complex clusters arise by iteration of the processes that give rise to these simple clusters (6) a. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek heeft gelezen I think that Han the book has read-part I think that John has read the book 1-2 b. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek gelezen heeft 2-1 in Standard Dutch verb clusters, the general word order becomes 1-2-3 (9a), while in High German and Frisian the general word order becomes 3-2-1 (10a) (9) a. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek moet kunnen lezen I think that John the book must can-inf read-inf I think John must be capable of reading the book 1-2-3 b. *(Ik denk) dat Jan het boek lezen kunnen moet 3-2-1 (10) a.... wêrom 't ik de hiele dei sitten bliuwe moattan ha why that I the whole day sit-inf stay-inf must-part have-fin... why I have had to remain sitting all day 4-3-2-1 b. *... wêrom 't ik de hiele dei ha moattan bliuwe sitten 1-2-3-4 Exceptions (Standard Dutch): (11) a. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek moet hebben gelezen I Think that John the book must have-inf read-part I think John must have read the book 1-2-3 b. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek moet gelezen hebben 1-3-2 c. (Ik denk) dat Jan het boek gelezen moet hebben 3-1-2 d.?(ik denk) dat Jan het boek gelezen hebben moet 3-2-1 e. *(ik denk) dat Jan het boek hebben gelezen moet 2-3-1 f. *(Ik denk) dat Jan het boek hebben moet gelezen 2-1-3 Innitivus Pro Participio (IPP): clusters of three verbs, the rst of which is an auxiliary, the verb in its immediate complement domain often takes the shape of an innitive, instead of the expected participial shape: Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 2

(12) a. (Ik denk) dat Jan het heeft kunnen/*gekund lezen I think that John the book has can-inf/-part read-inf I think John could have read the book 1-2-3 b. (Ich glaube) dass J. das Buch hat lesen können/*gekonnt I think that John the book has read-inf can-inf/-part 1-3-2 Certain 3-2-1 dialects, on the other hand, show a cluster containing two participles and one innitive next to the matrix auxiliary verb. This is because the IPP-eect is absent in strict 3-2-1 dialects: (14) a. (Ik dacht) dat Jan het boek gelezen had kunnen hebben I thought that John the book read-part had can-inf have-inf I thought John could have read the book 4-1-2-3 b.... omdat zi'j et wei es daon hebben kund had because she it Participle Participle done-part have-inf could-part had... because she may very well have done it. 4-3-2-1 Other double participle constructions do not seem to involve two auxiliary verbs: (15) a. (Ik wist niet) dat Jan ontslagen was (?geworden) I knew not that John red-part was become-part I did not know John had been red 3-1-2 b. Grad wo t abgfaare gsy bisch... just when you taken-o-part been-part are-fin Just after you had left 1-3-2 These constructions are curious, in that in all dialects, no matter what the preferred word order in the verbal cluster, the most deeply embedded participle has to precede the hierarchically higher participle. Descriptive generalization about verb clusters in Continental West Germanic dialects: Verb clusters in a number of dialects may be broken up by material properly belonging to the most deeply embedded verb in the cluster The generalization regarding Verb Projection Raising is that the material breaking up the cluster must be situated to the left of the verb which the material belongs to (in the sense just described). This implies that the phenomenon is absent from those languages that keep to a strict 3-2-1 order in the verbal cluster. (17) a. We zullen der moeten voor zorgen we will there must-inf for care-inf We will have to take care of that 1..2-3 b. (I bi stolz,) das i ha chöne über de see schwüme I am proud that I have-fin can-inf across the lake swim-inf I'm proud that I have been able to swim across the lake 1-2-3 Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 3

The Verb Projection Raising phenomenon in (17) suggests that, properly speaking, the 'ascending' orders (such as 1-2, 1-2-3, 1-3-2, etc.) do not present clusters, since there is a way for independent material to intervene between the members of the string of verbs. 3 Analysis of the verb clusters from an OV point of view Probleme: a. There is no consistent direction of adjunction, either across Continental West Germanic dialects, or even within particular Continental West Germanic dialects b. There is no xed phrase structure level of the category adjoined c. It is not clear what triggers the various movements, in the sense that there is no understanding how particular asymmetries are to be explained d. The parametrization makes no reference to the timing of the movements e. It is unclear why in the Germanic SOV languages multi-verb constructions show such a variety of word orders within the cluster, whereas in the Germanic SVO languages multi-verb constructions invariably show strictly 'ascending' orders f. It is unclear why certain phenomena are sensitive to the surface order of the members of the verb cluster g. The analysis relies on a phrase structural split among the Germanic languages for which there is no independent empirical basis 4 Analysis of verb clusters from an VO point of view embedded verbs are generated in a position to the right of embedding verbs; the noun phrase complement of a verb is moved to the left. In cases of multi-verb constructions, the complement of the most deeply embedded verb will appear to the left of the verb cluster: (19) a.... dat Jan het heeft gelezen [het boek] that John has read-part the book b.... dat Jan [het boek] i heeft gelezen t i These elements, then, will not normally interfere with the verb movements giving rise to verb clustering. the movements must give rise to crossing paths, rather than nesting paths: (20)... dat Jan [het boek] i [uit] j heeft gelezen t i t j (21) *... dat Jan [uit] j [het boek] i heeft gelezen t i t j Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 4

if the functional projection that hosts the object must precede the functional projection that hosts the embedded predicate, it is not excluded that the latter functional projection is generated somewhere between the auxiliary and the participle (22)... dat Jan [het boek] i heeft [uit] j gelezen t i t j it appears that, in the VO-approach, VP-Raising can be described independently of the verb movement that gives rise to the formation of verb clusters we can maintain that verb clustering never involves movement of verb projections. signicantly reduces the range of variation that the system of grammar allows This material breaking up the cluster cannot be placed to the right of the verb which it properly belongs to: (23) a.... dat Jan [het boek] i that John the book [uit] j out gelezen k heeft t k t i t j read-part has b. *... dat Jan [het boek] i gelezen k heeft [uit] j t k t i t j c. *... dat Jan [het boek] i gelezen k [uit] j heeft t k t i t j the intervening material in VP Raising constructions must be construed (in a pretheoretical sense) with the verb it belongs to. As a rst approximation, we could propose that the order in (6a) results from there being no movement at all, while the order in (6b) is the result of adjunction of the participle to the left of the auxiliary the variation in (6) is the result of the absence vs. presence of a particular movement A similar approach could not be as successful if we were to start from the OV-structure in (3) From the VO-perspective, it looks like the basic 1-2 order is xed (i.e., the auxiliary always follows the modal verb), while the 3 verb (the participle) can choose to not move at all, move part way, or move all the way (26a-c): (26) a. [ VP moet [ VP hebben [ VP gelezen ]]] = (11a) b. [ VP moet [ VP gelezen i -hebben [ VP t i ]]] = (11b) c. [ VP gelezen i -moet [ VP hebben [ VP t i ]]] = (11c) d. [ VP gelezen-hebben ] j -moet [ VP t j [ VP t i ]]] = (11d) e. [ VP [ VP hebben [ VP gelezen ] j moet t j ]] = (11e) f. [ VP hebben j -moet [ VP t j [ VP gelezen ]]] = (11f) The extremely sharp ungrammaticality of (11f), across all Continental West Germanic, as far as I have been able to check, is clearly reminiscent of the ungrammaticality of (23b) (where the embedded predicate appears to be left stranded). This suggests that for licensing purposes the participle is dependent on the auxiliary in the same way as the embedded predicate (or another element in the relevant complement domain) is on the verb selecting it. Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 5

The generalization then seems to be that the participle must be licensed somewhere to the left of the auxiliary, which leaves the 1-2 order in (6a) and the 1-2-3 order in (lla) a very curious exception indeed. On the OV-approach, the general picture of the participle 3 moving gradually leftward (yielding the 1-2-3, 1-3-2, and 3-1-2 orders of (11a-c)) is lost. Three entirely dierent derivations are needed to reach the three fully grammatical word orders in (lla-c). In conclusion, the various word orders discussed here can be derived in a simpler and more restrictive way if it is assumed that the verb phrase in Continental West Germanic is structured as in (2). 5 Patterns of optional movement The phenomenon that in certain dialects both the auxiliary-participle order and the participleauxiliary order are possible is described in terms of optional movement of the participle to the left. But the ungrainmaticality of (lit) suggests that such optionality in fact does not exist If participle movement is optional, movement of the auxiliary to the left of the modal should not interfere with the placement of the participle. Yet this is what happens participle movement cannot be optional (30) (30) a.... dat Jan het boek kan hebben gelezen that John the book can-inf have-inf read-part... that John may have read the book b. *... dat Jan het boek hebben i kan t i gelezen When the auxiliary moves, only the position preceding the auxiliary remains as a potential landing site for the participle Kayne (1993) proposes to analyze auxiliary-participle constructions as possessive constructions (have should also be treated as a composite of two heads, which I will call BE and OF). (31) VP specier V' BE VP specier V' OF AgrP AgrP' AgrP XP Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 6

I assume that in possessive constructions, like (32a), the lexical projection XP in (31) is an NP, whereas in auxiliary-participle constructions, like (32b), XP equals VP. Finally, in constructions containing a secondary predicate, like (32c), XP is a Small Clause: (32) a.... dat Jan [een boek] i heeft t i that John a book has... that John has a book b.... dat Jan [het boek] i heeft [ V P gelezen t i ] that John the book has read-part... that John has read the book c.... dat Jan [het boek] i [uit] j heeft [ SC t i t j ] that John the book out has... that John has nished the book (33)... [ het boek] i [ VP BE [ VP OF [ AgrP [ VP gelezen t i ]]]] there are two specier positions associated with have, indicated by the double hyphens in (33). I would like to propose now that the participle gelezen can be licensed in each of these two specier positions (34) a.... [ het boek] i [ VP BE [ VP gelezen j OF [ AgrP [ VP t j t i ]]]] b.... [ het boek] i [ VP gelezen j BE [ VP OF [ AgrP [ VP t j t i ]]]] The variation among the Continental West Germanic languages with respect to the position of the participle can now be described in terms of which of the two specier positions in (33) may be occupied by the participle. This analysis diers from the one entertained in section 4, in that participle movement was described as adjunction to a head in section 4, and as movement to a specier position in this section. Contrary to what one would expect under the scenario under consideration here, the clausal complement does not appear to the left of the auxiliary, as pointed out to me by Daniel Büring (p.c.). I will not discuss this issue here, leaving the phrase structure status of the participle in (34) open. As the facts in (11a-c) show, the optionality in the placement of the participle is not exhausted by the two variants yielded by (34). In particular, the participle may appear at the far left of a multi-verb cluster, and at various positions in between if the cluster contains more than three verbs. Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 7

(37) a.... dat Jan het boek gelezen moet hebben that John the book read-part must-fin have-inf... that John must have read the book. = (11c) b.?... dat Jan het boek moet gelezen kunnen hebben that John the book must-fin read-part can-inf have-inf... that John must have been able to read the book. 1-4-2-3 A rst approximation therefore could be to assume that in (37) the participle is licensed in the specier position associated with the modal verb. This analysis cannot work if the modal verb itself needs the specier position involved to license the verb in its immediate complement domain. Moreover, on the basis of the pair in (38), one would expect to nd the pair in (39) also, if the complement of the modal were to move as a phrase: (38) a.... dat Jan het boek moet lezen that John the book must-fin read-inf... that John must read the book. b.... dat Jan het boek lezen moet (39) a.... dat Jan het boek moet hebben gelezen that John the book must-fin have-inf read-part... that John must have read the book. = (11a) b. *... dat Jan het boek [ hebben gelezen] moet = (11e) I will assume that a modal may 'take over' from an innitival auxiliary (i.e., license the participle in its specier position) on the basis of the licensing relation that exists between the modal and the innitive (40) a. [ VP gelezen i (hebben j )-moet [ VP (hebben j ) [ VP t i ]]] Spelling out the higher copy yields the High German order 3-2-1, spelling out the lower copy yields the Standard Dutch order 3-1-2. (41)... dat Jan het boek gelezen zou moeten kunnen hebben that John the book read-part should-fin must-inf can-inf have-inf... that John should have been able to read the book. 5-1-2-3-4 The capacity to license the participle in a specier position is transferred with each adjunction The distribution of intervening material in Verb Projection Raising constructions and the distribution of participles seem to be regulated in essentially identical ways (43) a.... dat Jan het boek (uit) gelezen (*uit) moet (*uit) hebben that John the book (out) read-part (out) must-fin (out) have-inf... that John must have nished the book. 3-1-2 b.... dat Jan het boek (uit) moet (uit) hebben (uit) gelezen that John the book (out) must-fin (out) have-inf read-part (out)... that John must have nished the book. 1-2-3 Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 8

6 Conclusion the mechanism of the IPP-eect and the word order generalizations associated with it have been glossed over the question of 'Verb Projection Raising material' intervening between an auxiliary and a participle has not been discussed Leipzig, den 27.05.2012 9