SYNTACTIC RELATIONS VERSUS SEMANTIC ROLES WITHIN RELATIONAL FRAMEWORK

Similar documents
Constraining X-Bar: Theta Theory

Argument structure and theta roles

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Inleiding Taalkunde. Docent: Paola Monachesi. Blok 4, 2001/ Syntax 2. 2 Phrases and constituent structure 2. 3 A minigrammar of Italian 3

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

Pseudo-Passives as Adjectival Passives

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Beyond constructions:

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

Chapter 1 The functional approach to language and the typological approach to grammar

Unit 8 Pronoun References

Developing Grammar in Context

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

The Structure of Relative Clauses in Maay Maay By Elly Zimmer

Context Free Grammars. Many slides from Michael Collins

Strategic discourse comprehension

Chapter 4: Valence & Agreement CSLI Publications

DESIGNING NARRATIVE LEARNING MATERIAL AS A GUIDANCE FOR JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS IN LEARNING NARRATIVE TEXT

Part I. Figuring out how English works

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

BASIC ENGLISH. Book GRAMMAR

Unaccusatives, Resultatives, and the Richness of Lexical Representations

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Control and Boundedness

Construction Grammar. Laura A. Michaelis.

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Copyright and moral rights for this thesis are retained by the author

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

Compositional Semantics

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Multiple case assignment and the English pseudo-passive *

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

Writing a composition

The Four Principal Parts of Verbs. The building blocks of all verb tenses.

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

THE MAN BEHIND THE LEGEND

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

music downloads. free and free music downloads like

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Natural Language Processing. George Konidaris

November 2012 MUET (800)

Possessive have and (have) got in New Zealand English Heidi Quinn, University of Canterbury, New Zealand

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Analysis: Evaluation: Knowledge: Comprehension: Synthesis: Application:

Objectives. Chapter 2: The Representation of Knowledge. Expert Systems: Principles and Programming, Fourth Edition

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

ELD CELDT 5 EDGE Level C Curriculum Guide LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT VOCABULARY COMMON WRITING PROJECT. ToolKit

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today!

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 154 ( 2014 )

Handout 2.10a: 24 Operating Principles and the Verbal Behaviors That Go with Them Cultivating Classroom Discourse to Make Student Thinking Visible

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Adjectives tell you more about a noun (for example: the red dress ).

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Theoretical Syntax Winter Answers to practice problems

Modal Verbs for the Advice Move in Advice Columns

Nancy Hennessy M.Ed. 1

Some Principles of Automated Natural Language Information Extraction

Good-Enough Representations in Language Comprehension

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

Update on Soar-based language processing

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Case government vs Case agreement: modelling Modern Greek case attraction phenomena in LFG

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Campus Academic Resource Program An Object of a Preposition: A Prepositional Phrase: noun adjective

Intensive English Program Southwest College

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

THE FU CTIO OF ACCUSATIVE CASE I MO GOLIA *

The Semantics of Prepositions: An exploration into the uses of "at" and "to"

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Korean ECM Constructions and Cyclic Linearization

Tibor Kiss Reconstituting Grammar: Hagit Borer's Exoskeletal Syntax 1

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Sight Word Assessment

Constructions License Verb Frames

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Course Outline for Honors Spanish II Mrs. Sharon Koller

Chapter 9 Banked gap-filling

Developing True/False Test Sheet Generating System with Diagnosing Basic Cognitive Ability

Emmaus Lutheran School English Language Arts Curriculum

Abstractions and the Brain

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Words come in categories

Thornhill Primary School - Grammar coverage Year 1-6

The subject of adjectives: Syntactic position and semantic interpretation

CHAPTER II THEORETICAL REVIEW. knowledge. Learning without reading is less. A student who only

Transcription:

SYNTACTIC RELATIONS VERSUS SEMANTIC ROLES WITHIN RELATIONAL FRAMEWORK Mădălina CERBAN I. Syntactic relations Semantic roles, also called thematic relations or theta roles, refer to the arguments of the predicate. In all languages it is possible to express an event in several ways using different words. For example, if we want to express an event involving mechanic, tools and to fix a car, we can use two constructions: e.g. The mechanic fixed the car or (1.1) The car was fixed by the mechanic. (1.2) In both sentences the doer of the action is the mechanic, the tools and the car being affected by the action, the fixing, but they differ in how NPs referring to the mechanic and the car are realized syntactically. In the first sentence the NP referring to the doer of the action is the subject and the NP referring to the thing being affected is the direct object, while in the second sentence the NP referring to the thing being affected is the subject and the NP performing the action is the object of the preposition by. This contrast is captured in the system of voice: (1.1) is active, (1.2) is passive. There are, then, different types of relations holding between a predicate and its arguments in a sentence: grammatical relations like subject, direct object, and semantic roles like agent and patient. As there is no agreement regarding the correct set of semantic roles, this paper will discuss only the most common semantic roles. This structure will be referred to as argument structure. If we add another NP to the example above the following structures will be obtained: e.g. The mechanic fixed the car with his tools. His tools does not function as subject, direct object or indirect object, being introduced by the preposition with. Its semantic role is called instrument in this case. It can function as subject in a construction such as His tools were used to fix the car in a special case of passive voice which in relational framework is called circumstantial voice. Thus, the verb fix has three arguments: agent, patient and instrument and each of them can function as subject in a particular voice form. Taking into account the discussion above we have to make a terminological distinction. NPs functioning as Subjects and Direct and Indirect Objects will be referred to as terms. The NPs which do not have the grammatical relations mentioned above will be referred to as non-terms. In the example The mechanic fixed the car with his tools, the mechanic and his tools are terms, while in an example such as The tools are used by the mechanic to fix the car, the mechanic is a non-term.

Mădălina CERBAN Another distinction that should be made is that between Direct and Oblique arguments. Direct arguments are those which are not preceded by prepositions, while the Oblique ones are marked by preposition. II. Semantic roles While there are only three grammatical relations, the number of semantic roles is much larger. Semantic roles depend on the meaning of the verb they relate to. (ii.a) Agents are typically animate and normally trigger the action expressed by the verb, both intentionally and unintentionally. e.g. The man murdered his wife. Intentionally The man killed the child in the accident. Unintentionally *The man murdered the child in the accident. The last example is incorrect because the verb murder presupposes an intentional action, while the verb kill presupposes an intentional or unintentional action. The man killed his wife intentionally. (ii.b) Patient arguments are either in a state of condition or undergo a change of state or condition. e.g. The old man is dead. The old man died. 1 (ii.c) Theme arguments are semantically similar to patients because they also refer to entities which are located or which undergo a change of location; they also denote entities which are possessed or which undergo a change of possession (Van Valin Jr. 2001: 24): e.g. The teacher gave the book to his student. In this example the book is a theme, not a patient because it undergoes a change of possession, not a change of state. Similarly, The teacher put the book on the table, the book is a theme, not a patient because it undergoes a change of its location. 2 (ii.d) Recipient arguments are the Noun Phrase that undergo the actions and it can have two syntactic functions: Indirect Object: e.g. Mary gave a book to John, John is the recipient argument. Subject: e.g. Mary received a book from John, Mary is the recipient argument. (ii.e) Goal arguments differ from recipient arguments in the fact that the former has as finality a change of location, while the latter has as finality a change of possession.

Syntactic Relations Versus SemanticRoles Within Relational Framework e.g. Mary sent the book to John. (ii.f) Source arguments are Propositional Noun Phrases that indicate the starting point of the action. e.g. They ran from the school. (ii.g) Benefactive arguments indicate the beneficiary of the action e.g. He bought a bunch of flowers for his teacher. We can notice that goal, location, recipient and source arguments function with Themes due to the fact that Theme arguments indicate a location, a possession, or a change of location or possession. This is why Themes do not occur with patients. These arguments are closely related to the meaning of the verb; i.e. an event of moving needs a change of location. Nevertheless, the properties of verbs can not be explained only in relation to the state of affairs they embody. Different verbs require a different number of elements. Transitive verbs require at least one element, the Direct Object, but there are cases in which only a Direct Object is not enough for the sentence to be grammatically correct. For example, the verb put requires two elements, a Direct Object and an Adverbial Modifier of Place: e.g. I put the book on the table. * I put the book. * I put on the table. Other verbs, such as buy, run, give, drink can function with only one element: e.g. Leslie bought some flowers. Leslie bought some flowers for her teacher. Leslie brought some flowers for her teacher from the corner. Leslie gave the flowers. *Leslie gave to the teacher. Mike drank a beer. Mike drank in the pub. In the case of intransitive verbs, the arguments have one of the above mentioned semantic roles. Nevertheless, some verbs function both as transitive and intransitive verbs, e.g. drink. The Agent in Mike drank a beer and Mike drank in the pub is the same in both transitive and intransitive constructions. This is not the case in intransitive constructions that come from transitive ones through passivization. The Subject in this type of intransitive constructions is Patient, not Actor. e.g. The man broke the glass. The glass broke.

Mădălina CERBAN The Subject is an Agent in the first example, and a Patient in the second example. If the transitive sentence is paraphrased the semantic identity of the transitive Direct Object and the intransitive Subject becomes explicit. The identity of the patient in the two forms in overt. e.g. The man caused the glass to break. Another category is represented by transitive verbs that do not express actions, such as see, understand, like and therefore their Subjects are not Actors. Van Valin jr. (2001) classifies the Subjects of these verbs in: perceivers, cognizers and emoters. Generally they are referred to as experiencers. e.g. The child saw a cat on the roof. Perceiver The child understood the story. Cognizer The child liked the cake. Emoter Semantic roles can be discussed in general terms such as Agent, Patient, Experiencer or in more specific terms, such as runner, giver, thinker, lover, broken, killed. Agents can be divided into: buyer, giver, drinker and so on (see the examples above), Perceivers can be divided into hearer, feeler, taster and so on, Cognizers into: knower, believer, thinker, Emoters into: liker, lover, hater and so on. In order to differentiate the specific semantic roles from the most general ones, the former are referred to as verb-specific semantic roles and the latter are represented by Thematic Relations. Relational grammar identifies one more level of semantic roles that are relevant from a linguistic point of view, namely grammatical relations. If we analyse the following examples we notice that there is an overlap between the thematic roles of the Subject and the thematic roles of the Direct Object: Subject Direct Object e.g. The writer wrote a new novel. Agent Patient The child got a good mark. Recipient Theme Mother gave her son a new car. Agent Recipient The child saw the film. Experiencer Stimulus The clown amused the child. Agent Experiencer From the examples above we can notice that Recipient, the Recipient can have the syntactic functions of Subject as well as of Direct Object. If we take into account the passive counterparts of these sentences, the grammatical relations are different, but the Thematic roles are the same. The role of the Subject of an active voice and the object of by in a passive construction will be referred to as Actor, and the role of the Direct Object of an active construction and the Subject of a passive verb is called Undergoer. Relational framework considers that these roles, Actor and Undergoer form an intermediate level between Thematic relations and grammatical relations, called semantic macroroles. As a result the order is: (1) Verb-specific semantic role; (2) Semantic role;

Syntactic Relations Versus SemanticRoles Within Relational Framework (3) Thematic relations; (4) Semantic macroroles; (5) Grammatical relations If we analyse a construction such as People believe in God, the Noun Phrase people have the following roles and function: (1) believer; (2) cognizer; (3) experiencer, (4) Actor, (5) Subject. Generally, the semantic macrorole of Actor can have the Thematic role of Agent, while the semantic macrorole of Undergoer can have the Thematic role of Patient. As a result, relational grammar identifies two hierarchies which identify the types of Agent-like or Patient-like Thematic relations. Agent and Patient are on the top of these hierarchies. Nevertheless, if a verb does not have an Agent, then it has an Instrument, an Experiencer, or a Recipient. e.g. The man killed his neighbor. [Agent] The key opened the door. [Instrument] The old man felt dizzy. [Experiencer] The students received their grades. [Recipient] In Patient-like hierarchy, if a verb does not have a Patient, then it will have a Theme, a Stimulus, an Experiencer or a Recipient. e.g. The singer was awarded the prize. [Theme] Mike saw the quarrel. [Stimulus] The clown amused the children. [Experiencer] The flowers were given to the teacher. [Recipient] We can conclude that, although semantic macroroles Actor and Patient represent useful generalizations, using them in order to describe the active constructions in which the Actor has the syntactic function of Subject and the Undergoer has the syntactic function of Direct Object and the passive constructions in which the Undergoer functions as Subject and the Actor as non-term, we can not ignore the verb-specific semantic roles and the Thematic relations in these constructions. If we use Thematic relations to analyse the Active sentences, the Subject can be represented by an Agent, an Instrument, an Experiencer or an Recipient; in Passive sentences, The Subject can be expressed by a Patient, Theme, Experiencer, Stimulus, Recipient or Source, while the non-term can be an Agent, an Instrument, an Experiencer or a Recipient. The more roles we use, the more

Mădălina CERBAN complicated this analysis becomes. This is why the generalizations are important in order to understand the relations between the syntactic functions and the semantic roles within relational framework. NOTES 1 Patient arguments often appear with verbs like: kill, destroy, wash, break. 2 Theme arguments occur with verbs like: put, give, place, buy. REFERENCES Blake, Barry J., Case, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1994. Dik, Simon C., The Theory of Functional Grammar, Part 1: The Structure of the Clause, Amsterdam, Walter de Gruyter, 1989. Foley, William, Van Valin, Robert, Functional Syntax and Universal grammar, Cambridge University Press, 1984. Givon,Talmy, Syntax: An Introduction, I. Amsterdam, John Benjamins, 2001. Hopper & Thompson, Transitivity in Grammar and Discourse, in Language 56, 1980, pp. 281-299. Keenan, Edward L., Towards a universal definition of subject, in C. N. Li, eds., New York, Academic Press, 1976, pp. 305-333. Palmer, F. R., Grammatical roles and relations, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1994. Perlmutter, David (ed.), Studies in Relational Grammar 1, Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1983. Van Valin, Jr., R., An Introduction to Syntax, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 2001. (Van Valin Jr. 2001) Van Valin, R., LaPolla, R., Syntax, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1997. ABSTRACT The paper is structured into two parts: the first part discusses the syntactic relations in sentence, while the second part presents the most important types of semantic roles, pointing out that the number of these semantic roles is much larger than the one of syntactic roles. We analyse the most important levels of generality with respect to semantic roles, namely verb-specific semantic roles, thematic relations and grammatical relations, paying attention to the overlapping of these levels. Key words: syntactic relation, semantic role, thematic role