FUNCTIONAL OPTIMALITY THEORY. Paul Boersma. Abstract. This paper briefly introduces Optimality Theory from the functionalist viewpoint.

Similar documents
Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Markedness and Complex Stops: Evidence from Simplification Processes 1. Nick Danis Rutgers University

Consonants: articulation and transcription

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

An argument from speech pathology

Manner assimilation in Uyghur

Underlying Representations

Som and Optimality Theory

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Phonetics. The Sound of Language

To appear in the Proceedings of the 35th Meetings of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Post-vocalic spirantization: Typology and phonetic motivations

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

Phonological Processing for Urdu Text to Speech System

Listener-oriented phonology

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

A Level Playing-Field: Perceptibility and Inflection in English Compounds. Robert Kirchner and Elena Nicoladis (U. Alberta)

SOUND STRUCTURE REPRESENTATION, REPAIR AND WELL-FORMEDNESS: GRAMMAR IN SPOKEN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION. Adam B. Buchwald

Canadian raising with language-specific weighted constraints Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Lexical phonology. Marc van Oostendorp. December 6, Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a monolithic

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Precedence Constraints and Opacity

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

SEGMENTAL FEATURES IN SPONTANEOUS AND READ-ALOUD FINNISH

Speech Recognition using Acoustic Landmarks and Binary Phonetic Feature Classifiers

Radical CV Phonology: the locational gesture *

Derivations (MP) and Evaluations (OT) *

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

Eli Yamamoto, Satoshi Nakamura, Kiyohiro Shikano. Graduate School of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science & Technology

Graduate Program in Education

The optimal placement of up and ab A comparison 1

SOME MINIMAL NOTES ON MINIMALISM *

Program in Linguistics. Academic Year Assessment Report

The Acquisition of English Intonation by Native Greek Speakers

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Joan Bybee, Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2001,

Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson

Optimality Theory and the Minimalist Program

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

DOWNSTEP IN SUPYIRE* Robert Carlson Societe Internationale de Linguistique, Mali

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Introduction to HPSG. Introduction. Historical Overview. The HPSG architecture. Signature. Linguistic Objects. Descriptions.

Towards a Robuster Interpretive Parsing

Journal of Phonetics

Consonant-Vowel Unity in Element Theory*

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Applications of memory-based natural language processing

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Partial Class Behavior and Nasal Place Assimilation*

Linguistics 220 Phonology: distributions and the concept of the phoneme. John Alderete, Simon Fraser University

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Sound symbolism in deictic words

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Audible and visible speech

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

An Introduction to the Minimalist Program

Laporan Penelitian Unggulan Prodi

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Design Of An Automatic Speaker Recognition System Using MFCC, Vector Quantization And LBG Algorithm

age, Speech and Hearii

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Using computational modeling in language acquisition research

Process-specific constraints in Optimality Theory

Human Emotion Recognition From Speech

Christine Mooshammer, IPDS Kiel, Philip Hoole, IPSK München, Anja Geumann, Dublin

Contrastiveness and diachronic variation in Chinese nasal codas. Tsz-Him Tsui The Ohio State University

Fix Your Vowels: Computer-assisted training by Dutch learners of Spanish

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

On the Formation of Phoneme Categories in DNN Acoustic Models

L1 Influence on L2 Intonation in Russian Speakers of English

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

On-Line Data Analytics

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Learning Methods in Multilingual Speech Recognition

Perceived speech rate: the effects of. articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. Jacques Koreman. Saarland University

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

On the nature of voicing assimilation(s)

An Interactive Intelligent Language Tutor Over The Internet

Language Acquisition Chart

The analysis starts with the phonetic vowel and consonant charts based on the dataset:

The Odd-Parity Parsing Problem 1 Brett Hyde Washington University May 2008

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Transcription:

Institute of Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Proceedings 21 (1997), 37-42 FUNCTIONAL OPTIMALITY THEORY Paul Boersma Abstract This paper briefly introduces Optimality Theory from the functionalist viewpoint. 1. Introduction The functional hypothesis for phonology (Passy 1890) maintains that sound structures reflect an interaction between the articulatory and perceptual principles of efficient and effective communication. The theory of Functional Phonology (Boersma 1997a) maintains that this interaction is directly reflected in the grammar: it handles substance-related phonological phenomena within the constraint-ranking framework introduced by Optimality Theory (Prince & Smolensky 1993), but without the need for positing innate features and hierarchies; if restricted to gestural and faithfulness constraints, its scope equals that of autosegmental phonology and feature geometry. 2. Grammar model Functional Phonology makes a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual representations and features. Figure 1 shows its concept of the grammatical correlates of the systems, processes, and representations of the speech production and perception systems of a single speaker-listener: I underlying form J recognition system / (recognition grammar) lspecijicationl - - - - --- - - - - - - - - - :. / production system : f (production grammar) [articulatory output] - - - - - - ART I perceptual input/ -lj.. : [acoustic output] perceptuạl cat. system t corn. are (perception grammar) p perceptual cat. system (perception grammar) : [acoustic input] I perceptual output/ FAITH - - - ' LISTENER SPEAKER Figure 1: the grammar model of Functional Phonology. IFA Proceedings 21, 1997 37

In figure 1, we see the following representations: (1) The acoustic input of the speech uttered by another person, as presented to the ear of the listener; written between brackets because it is a language-independent representation. (2) The perceptual input: the speech uttered by another person, as perceived by the listener, in terms of perceptual features (periodicity, noise, spectrum) and their combinations; written between slashes. (3) A perceptual specification in terms of perceptual features, as stored in the language user's lexicon; written between pipes. (4) The articulatory output of the speaker, in terms of articulatory gestures (articulator positions, muscle tensions) and their combinations; written between brackets. (5) The acoustic output of the speaker: an automatic result of her articulatory output; also written between brackets. (6) The perceptual output of the speaker: her acoustic output, as perceived by herself; written between slashes. Figure 1 also shows the following processing systems: The speaker's production system determines the surface form of the utterance from an underlying perceptual specification. The listener's perceptual categorization system determines how a listener converts the raw acoustic input to a more perceptual representation; she uses the system for the acoustic input from other speakers as well as for her own acoustic output. The listener's recognition system converts the perceptual input into an underlying form (and helps the categorization system). A comparison module on behalf of language acquisition. If the learner's output, as perceived by herself, differs from the adult utterance, as perceived by the learner, the learner will take a learning step (Boersma, to appear; Boersma 1997b). The abbreviations ART and FAITH refer to articulatory and faithfulness constraints, as explained below. 3. Constraint-ranking grammars and functionalism Consider the process of place assimilation of nasals in Dutch. The words /trein/ 'train' and /paka/ 'catch' will often be concatenated as /treimpaka/. The process is confined to the coronal nasal: bilabial nasals, velar nasals, and plosives at any place, do not usually assimilate. 3.1. Explanation versus description A phonetic explanation for these facts can readily be given: as compared to the articulation [ treinpaka], the articulation [ treimpaka] saves the speaker one tonguetip gesture, since the bilabial gesture for [ m] was already needed for [ p ]; the perceptual loss of this assimilation is the neutralization of any specified I nl and lml, which could lead to confusions between words that end in these sounds, and to extra required effort in the recognition system. The restriction to nasals can be explained by the fact that e.g. the nasals /m/ and /n/ are perceptually much more alike than the plosives /p/ and /t/, so that the listener will rely less on place information for nasals than for plosives, so that the speaker has more freedom to mispronounce a nasal than 38 IF A Proceedings 21, 1997

a plosive; the restriction to coronals can be explained by the fact that coronals are much more common than labials, so that the listener will have a bias towards recognizing a coronal instead of a labial, so that the speaker will assume that the listener will reconstruct the coronal even if she pronounces it as a labial. These explanations, however, do not tell us what a speaker does when she has to concatenate the words /trein/ and I pak'j/, and this is why phonetic explanations have seldom met with enthusiasm on the part of linguists. Until 1993, linguists tended to describe phonological processes with rules, e.g., they would describe nasal place assimilation with a structure-changing recipe like "n m I _ p'', or with a generalization over the various places, or with a featurefilling recipe like "[Oplace] [aplace] I_ [aplace]", or in an autosegmental and/or feature-geometric formulation. All these notational variants, however, are still recipes and have little explanatory power. So the explanatory and descriptive accounts had been divorced for a long time. 3.2. Constraint-ranking grammars The advent of Optimality Theory (Prince & Smolensky 1993; McCarthy & Prince 1993a, 1993b, 1994, 1995) changed this situation, by making constraints instead of rules central to the grammar. A traditional Optimality-Theoretic account of nasal place assimilation would have that a universal constraint NASSIM ("nasals have the same place as a following consonant") is dominating the universal constraint ldent (place) ("the surface place is equal to the underlying place specification"). Since these constraints are violable, the outcome depends on their rankings, so that we have the following mini-typology: if NASSIM outranks ldent (place), there will be assimilation; if, on the other hand, ldent (place) >> NASSIM, there won't. A constraint like NASSIM still provides no explanation: it is still purely descriptive. But instead of these allegedly universal constraints, we can directly translate the phonetic principles of minimization of effort and perceptual confusion into the grammar, namely, into articulatory constraints ("ART " in figure 1 ), which evaluate articulatory outputs, and faithfulness constraints ("FAITH" in figure 1), which evaluate the similarity between the specification and the perceptual output. For nasal place assimilation, the relevant articulatory and faithfulness constraints would be *GESTURE (tongue tip: close & open): "do not make a tongue-tip opening and closing gesture" *REPLACE (place: coronal, labial I nasal I _ C): "do not implement a perceptual coronal place specification as something that will be heard as labial place, for a nasal, before a consonant" The Dutch assimilation process can then be seen as the result of the following grammar of ranked constraints (I will freely abbreviate constraint names): *REPLACE (cm I plosive) Nasal place assimilation *GEST RE (tip) *REPLACE (car I nasal) (2) IFA Proceedings 21, 1997 39

Because plosives do not assimilate, the constraint *REPLACE (place: coronal, labial I plosive I _ C) must be ranked higher than *GESTURE (tongue tip). Note that the ranking of *REPLACE (place I plosive) above *REPLACE (place I nasal) reflects the asymmetry of perceptual confusion discussed above, so that we may well hypothesize that this ranking is nearly universal. Indeed, if we could find out what rankings are universal and what rankings can be set on a language-specific basis, we would have a typologically adequate account of possible and impossible sound systems, which, in my view, is an important goal of phonological theory. Thus, violable constraints can be expressed in such a general way that they yield to the linguist's requirement of universality and simplicity, and to the phonetician's requirement of explicability in terms of the properties of the human speech mechanism. In Boersma (forthcoming), I identify these functional constraints, investigate their interactions, and show their empirical adequacy..4. The production grammar So I will assume that the speaker's production system can be described by an Optimality-Theoretic production grammar. A typical production process can thus be represented with the following Optimality-Theoretic tableau: lspecj A B [arti] /percif * [ art2] I perc2/ * I (3) This tableau shows the following representations, each of which can be identified in figure 1: ( 1) A perceptual specification spec. (2) Many candidate articulatory outputs arti. (3) For each candidate articulatory output art( the corresponding perceptual output perci. In tableau (3 ), the two constraints A and B both issue a protest against a certain candidate, as shown by the asterisks (the marks). Because A is ranked higher than B, the dishannony associated with its violation is greater than that of B, and its violation becomes the crucial violation for candidate 2, as shown by the exclamation symbol, which is put after the crucial mark. Thus, candidate 1 is more harmonic (less offensive) than 2, so it becomes the winner, as shown by the pointing finger. Some cells are grey because any violations in these cells have not contributed to determining the winner. Our example of nasal place assimilation is written as lan+pal *GESTURE (tip) *REPLACE (cor) [anpa] /anpa/ *! [ampa] /ampa/ * (4) 40 IF A Proceedings 21, 1997

The candidate [ampa] (shorthand for "pharyngeal narrowing plus lip closure and opening plus velum raising... "), which is perceived as /ampa/ (shorthand for "high F1 plus labial place plus nasality... "), is the winner. The fact that phonetic principles can be expressed in an Optimality-Theoretic grammar, was independently discovered by a research group in California (Jun 1995, Flemming 1995, Hayes 1996, Kirchner 1998). The differences between their theories and the view of functional phonology described here, are discussed in Boersma (forthcoming). 5. The perception grammar I will likewise assume that the listener's categorization system can be described by an Optimality-Theoretic perception grammar. We can thus represent a typical categorization process with the following tableau: [ac] A B I@' /cat1 I * /cat2/ *' (5) This tableau shows the following representations (visible twice in figure 1): ( 1) An acoustic input ac. (2) Several candidate perceptual categories cati. For instance, on the perceptual tier F 1 (first formant), the listener may have three categories of 300, 500, and 700 Hz (for high, mid, and low vowels, respectively). If the acoustic input is 440 Hz, a relevant constraint is: *WARP (F1: [440), /300/): "do not initially classify an acoustic input of 440 Hz as a high vowel" The decision of the categorization system can now be described with the following tableau, if the system is trying to initially classify any acoustic input into the "nearest" category: [440] *WARP *WARP *WARP ([440], /700/) ([440], /300/) ([440], /500/) /300/ *' I@' /500/ * /700/ *! (6) The winner is the category /500/, i.e., the input of 440 Hz is initially perceived as a mid vowel (the recognition system may correct this initial categorization on the basis of other information). IFA Proceedings 21. 1997 41

6. Conclusion The hypothesis of Functional Phonology is that the production and categorization systems can be described with Optimality-Theoretic constraint-ranking grammars that contain direct translations of principles of minimization of articulatory effort and perceptual confusion. 7. References Boersma, Paul (1997a). "The elements of Functional Phonology", ms. University of Amsterdam. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 173, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html) Boersma, Paul (1997b). "How we learn variation, optionality, and probability", this volume, 43-58. [an earlier version is Rutgers Optimality Archive 221, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html) Boersma, Paul (forthcoming). Functional Phonology. Doctoral thesis, University of Amsterdam. Boersma, Paul (to appear). "Learning a grammar in Functional Phonology", in Joost Dekkers, Frank van der Leeuw, and Jer'ben van de Weijer (eds.), Optimality Theory: Phonology, Syntax, and Acquisition. Flemming, Edward ( 1995). Auditory Representations in Phonology. Doctoral thesis, University of California, Los Angeles. Hayes, Bruce (1996): "Phonetically driven phonology: the role of Optimality Theory and inductive grounding", Proceedings of the 1996 Milwaukee Conference on Formalism and Functionalism in Linguistics. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 158) Jun, Jongho (1995). "Place assimilation as the result of conflicting perceptual and articulatory constraints", West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 14. Kirchner, Robert (1998). "Some new generalizations concerning geminate inalterability", handout at a meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 240] McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince (l 993a). Prosodic Morphology /: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and Rutgers University, New Brunswick. McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince (1993b). "Generalized alignment", in Geen Booij and Jaap van Marie (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1993, Kluwer, Dordrecht, 79-153. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 7, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html] McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince (1994). "The emergence of the unmarked: optimality in prosodic morphology", in Merce Gonzalez (ed.), Papers of the 24th Annual Meeting of the North Eastern Linguistic Society, Graduate Linguistic Student Association, Amherst, 333-379. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 13, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html] McCarthy, John J. and Alan Prince (1995). "Faithfulness and reduplicative identity", in J. Beckman, S. Urbanczyk, and L. Walsh (eds.), Papers in Optimality Theory, Occasional Papers 18, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, 249-384. [Rutgers Optimality Archive 60] Prince, Alan and Paul Smolensky (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science Technical Report 2. Passy, Paul (1890). Etude sur Les changements phonetiques et leurs caracteres generaux. Librairie Firmin - Didot, Paris. 42 IFA Proceedings 21, 1997