Affirmative Action's Affirmative Actions: A Reply to Sander

Similar documents
AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

Ryerson University Sociology SOC 483: Advanced Research and Statistics

NCEO Technical Report 27

Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer

A Comparison of Charter Schools and Traditional Public Schools in Idaho

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

ABILITY SORTING AND THE IMPORTANCE OF COLLEGE QUALITY TO STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT: EVIDENCE FROM COMMUNITY COLLEGES

Physics 270: Experimental Physics

WE GAVE A LAWYER BASIC MATH SKILLS, AND YOU WON T BELIEVE WHAT HAPPENED NEXT

School Competition and Efficiency with Publicly Funded Catholic Schools David Card, Martin D. Dooley, and A. Abigail Payne

Chapters 1-5 Cumulative Assessment AP Statistics November 2008 Gillespie, Block 4

College Pricing. Ben Johnson. April 30, Abstract. Colleges in the United States price discriminate based on student characteristics

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

Cal s Dinner Card Deals

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

MASTER S THESIS GUIDE MASTER S PROGRAMME IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

Peer Influence on Academic Achievement: Mean, Variance, and Network Effects under School Choice

learning collegiate assessment]

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Interpreting ACER Test Results

An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

w o r k i n g p a p e r s

Multiple regression as a practical tool for teacher preparation program evaluation

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

Lahore University of Management Sciences. FINN 321 Econometrics Fall Semester 2017

STT 231 Test 1. Fill in the Letter of Your Choice to Each Question in the Scantron. Each question is worth 2 point.

Lecture 10: Reinforcement Learning

West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition

The Impact of Honors Programs on Undergraduate Academic Performance, Retention, and Graduation

Quantitative analysis with statistics (and ponies) (Some slides, pony-based examples from Blase Ur)

MGT/MGP/MGB 261: Investment Analysis

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Learning But Not Earning? The Value of Job Corps Training for Hispanics

CHMB16H3 TECHNIQUES IN ANALYTICAL CHEMISTRY

Feature-oriented vs. Needs-oriented Product Access for Non-Expert Online Shoppers

PEER EFFECTS IN THE CLASSROOM: LEARNING FROM GENDER AND RACE VARIATION *

STUDYING RULES For the first study cycle at International Burch University

READY OR NOT? CALIFORNIA'S EARLY ASSESSMENT PROGRAM AND THE TRANSITION TO COLLEGE

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

Activities, Exercises, Assignments Copyright 2009 Cem Kaner 1

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Purdue Data Summit Communication of Big Data Analytics. New SAT Predictive Validity Case Study

Mismatch and The Empirical Scholars Brief

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Alberta Police Cognitive Ability Test (APCAT) General Information

Statewide Framework Document for:

TUESDAYS/THURSDAYS, NOV. 11, 2014-FEB. 12, 2015 x COURSE NUMBER 6520 (1)

CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and

GradinG SyStem IE-SMU MBA

Understanding and Interpreting the NRC s Data-Based Assessment of Research-Doctorate Programs in the United States (2010)

Mathematics Scoring Guide for Sample Test 2005

PIRLS. International Achievement in the Processes of Reading Comprehension Results from PIRLS 2001 in 35 Countries

Evaluating Statements About Probability

Introduction to Causal Inference. Problem Set 1. Required Problems

Software Maintenance

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

What is PDE? Research Report. Paul Nichols

Reflective Teaching KATE WRIGHT ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR, SCHOOL OF LIFE SCIENCES, COLLEGE OF SCIENCE

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

Running head: THE INTERACTIVITY EFFECT IN MULTIMEDIA LEARNING 1

12- A whirlwind tour of statistics

Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital. Guidance and Information for Teachers

ReFresh: Retaining First Year Engineering Students and Retraining for Success

Sociology 521: Social Statistics and Quantitative Methods I Spring Wed. 2 5, Kap 305 Computer Lab. Course Website

Sociology 521: Social Statistics and Quantitative Methods I Spring 2013 Mondays 2 5pm Kap 305 Computer Lab. Course Website

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES WOULD THE ELIMINATION OF AFFIRMATIVE ACTION AFFECT HIGHLY QUALIFIED MINORITY APPLICANTS? EVIDENCE FROM CALIFORNIA AND TEXAS

UNESCO Bangkok Asia-Pacific Programme of Education for All. Embracing Diversity: Toolkit for Creating Inclusive Learning-Friendly Environments

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

Go fishing! Responsibility judgments when cooperation breaks down

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

Analysis of Enzyme Kinetic Data

The Efficacy of PCI s Reading Program - Level One: A Report of a Randomized Experiment in Brevard Public Schools and Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Room: Office Hours: T 9:00-12:00. Seminar: Comparative Qualitative and Mixed Methods

Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use: An Alternative Approach 1

White Paper. The Art of Learning

LANGUAGE DIVERSITY AND ECONOMIC DEVELOPMENT. Paul De Grauwe. University of Leuven

Senior Stenographer / Senior Typist Series (including equivalent Secretary titles)

Practical Research. Planning and Design. Paul D. Leedy. Jeanne Ellis Ormrod. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey Columbus, Ohio

Creating Meaningful Assessments for Professional Development Education in Software Architecture

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

TU-E2090 Research Assignment in Operations Management and Services

Introduction. 1. Evidence-informed teaching Prelude

Transfer of Training

AP Statistics Summer Assignment 17-18

Hierarchical Linear Modeling with Maximum Likelihood, Restricted Maximum Likelihood, and Fully Bayesian Estimation

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

On-the-Fly Customization of Automated Essay Scoring

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

Transcription:

Yale Law Journal Volume 114 Issue 8 Yale Law Journal Article 7 2005 Affirmative Action's Affirmative Actions: A Reply to Sander Daniel E. Ho Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.law.yale.edu/ylj Recommended Citation Daniel E. Ho, Affirmative Action's Affirmative Actions: A Reply to Sander, 114 Yale L.J. (2005). Available at: http://digitalcommons.law.yale.edu/ylj/vol114/iss8/7 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Yale Law School Legal Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Yale Law Journal by an authorized editor of Yale Law School Legal Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact julian.aiken@yale.edu.

Reply Affirmative Action's Affirmative Actions: A Reply to Sander Daniel E. Ho I am grateful to Professor Sander for his interest in my work and his willingness to pursue a valid answer to the critical question of the effects of law school tier on bar performance. Sander's readiness to respond to my Comment' demonstrates the importance of the questions at hand and his openness to progress on these issues. Fortunately, progress is possible, because, as I show here, the impressive-sounding points in Sander's Response 2 violate basic methodological principles and are incorrect. Sander points to certain descriptive facts that my Comment does not dispute. Black students appear to fail the bar at higher rates than white students. It also appears that "blacks and whites with similar law school grades (when controlling for school and entering credentials) have virtually identical graduation and bar outcomes." '3 However, these descriptive observations are irrelevant to the causal question of whether going to a higher-tier law school causes black students to fail the bar. As my Comment and this Reply demonstrate, black law students who are similarly qualified when applying to law school perform equally well on the bar irrespective of what tier school they attend. There is no evidence that affirmative action reduces the bar performance of the students it is designed to help. The descriptive facts Sander presents may account for some of the reasons for affirmative action, but they do not address the consequences of affirmative * All analyses presented in this Reply are available at http://www.yalelawjournal.org. 1. Daniel E. Ho, Scholarship Comment, Why Affirmative Action Does Not Cause Black Students To Fail the Bar, 114 YALE L.J. 1997 (2005). 2. Richard H. Sander, Mismeasuring the Mismatch: A Response to Ho, 114 YALE L.J. 2005 (2005). 3. Id. at 2006 (emphasis added). 2011

2012 The Yale Law Journal [Vol. 114:2011 action. Here, I respond to each of Sander's points in turn. First, Sander's control group, as he conceives it, is invalid. As I noted in my Comment, every law school examined practices some form of affirmative action, so the data is not informative about the broad effects of affirmative action. Instead, we may investigate the effects of attending a higher-tier law school. In response, Sander asserts that white students are the control group and black students are the treatment group, because only blacks were admitted under a system of affirmative action. 4 This conception is incorrect, because black students could differ from white students in all sorts of respects that are not due to affirmative action or law school tier. This flaw is akin to assigning estrogen to a group of women and a placebo to a group of men, and inferring the effect of estrogen on health outcomes by comparing women and men. The black/white test score gap has been documented across a host of fields, so other differences (like primary school education, family structure, or culture) may be at work. 5 Comparing blacks and whites in order to infer the causal effect of law school tier ignores all of these other differences between these groups and thereby provides an invalid estimate of the causal effect. 6 Second, Sander asserts that controlling for law school grades does not bias the estimate of the causal effect of law school tier. He does not take issue with the rule of inference that controlling for a consequence of the cause is never justified and will never produce the right causal effect. 7 This rule is undisputedly violated here, because Sander's analysis itself demonstrates that going to a higher-tier law school decreases law school grades 8 and that law school grades are correlated with bar passage. 9 Sander's responses to this basic flaw do not withstand scrutiny. Standardizing grades within schools is no solution, because the same student may receive lower grades in a higher-tier school even if she learned the same amount. Moreover, the fact that law school GPA is the variable with the biggest effect in Sander's regressions l is not surprising at all. In fact, it is an indicator of the problem: This coefficient reflects the fact that his regression is controlling away the consequences of law school tier, the 4. Id. 5. See THE BLACK-WHITE TEST SCORE GAP (Christopher Jencks & Meredith Phillips eds., 1998). 6. Sander himself recognizes this problem, finding it necessary to argue that "being black" is not an "independently significant causal factor" determining bar passage outcomes. Richard H. Sander, A Systemic Analysis of Affirmative Action in American Law Schools, 57 STAN. L. REV. 367, 447 (2004). The coefficient on being black is only insignificant in the original bar passage regression, however, when Sander (incorrectly) controls for law school grades. Id. at 444. 7. See GARY KING ET AL., DESIGNING SOCIAL INQUIRY: SCIENTIFIC INFERENCE IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCH 78 (1994); PAUL R. ROSENBAUM, OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES 73-74 (2d ed. 2002). 8. Sander, supra note 6, at 373. 9. Id. at 443. 10. See Sander, supra note 6, at 443; see also Sander, supra note 2, at 2007.

2005] Affirmative Actions 2013 key causal variable. As my Comment demonstrates, omitting law school grades from the original regressions leads the coefficient on law school tier to be indistinguishable from zero and the coefficient on black students to be substantially negative. This result shows that within Sander's analysis there is no detectable effect of law school tier and that preexisting credentials alone do not account for the black/white bar passage gap. To further investigate this issue, Sander now proposes a structural equations model." This represents a different approach based on different assumptions from the original article, whereas the whole point of my reanalysis was to reduce the role of unfounded and unnecessary assumptions. Sander's discussion fails to do precisely what my Comment aimed to achieve, namely, to clarify the assumptions in substantively meaningful ways in order to validly infer a causal effect. Clarifying the assumptions of Sander's structural equations model shows that it in fact supports each of my Comment's main points: (1) that we cannot control for law school grades in estimating the causal effect of tier, (2) that white students are an invalid control group, and (3) that there is no detectable effect of law school tier on bar passage. To verify this, note that the path diagram itself readily admits post-treatment bias by the arrows from tier to law school GPA to bar passage. As a result, the total effect in this path diagram simply represents the coefficient of tier in a bar passage regression, 12 (correctly) excluding law school GPA. In addition, the model eliminates white students, making it inconsistent with Sander's (invalid) "organiz[ation] around a comparison of 'treatment' blacks... and 'control' whites." 13 Most importantly, applying this exact structural model to the original data set and variables yields a tier effect that is indistinguishable from zero. 14 If this model supports all of the points in my Comment, why has it been introduced? Not only has Sander's Response changed the assumptions and the model, but it has also changed the outcome variable from eventual bar passage, as defined in his original article, to first-time bar passage. 15 Applying this structural model to the original outcome variable of eventual 11. Id. at 2007-08. 12. See Paul W. Holland, Causal Inference, Path Analysis, and Recursive Structural Equations Models, 18 SOC. METHODOLOGY 449, 457 (1988). 13. Sander, supra note 2, at 2006. 14. This is estimated with a linear probability model of bar passage controlling for LSAT score and undergraduate GPA, excluding gender, because that is what is depicted in Sander's path diagram. Using a logistic regression or including gender does not alter the fact that the tier coefficient is insignificant in the original data set. Although Sander has not provided sufficient information, he appears to have applied ordinary least squares for a dichotomous outcome, which is wrong in part because predictions are not even bounded to the unit interval. 15. Although Sander describes the outcome as "whether a person passes the bar on one of her first two attempts," Sander, supra note 6, at 444, the user's guide for the data indicates that this variable actually represents eventual bar result, see LINDA F. WIGHTMAN, USER'S GUIDE: LSAC NATIONAL LONGITUDINAL DATA FILE 15 (1999).

2014 The Yale Law Journal [Vol. 114:2011 bar passage, we find no tier effect. With the recoded outcome, the model does lead to a borderline significant effect of tier, but not at the significance level of.01 that Sander reports for all other coefficients. 6 The marginal effect of going to a higher-tier law school in this model is roughly a 2.4% increase in the probability of first-time bar passage, plus or minus 2.5% at a 99% confidence level.' 7 This effect is neither statistically distinguishable from zero nor close to explaining the substantial black/white bar passage gaps as Sander originally claimed. (Recall that bar passage gaps are as large as 21% in the index range defined by Sander.)' 8 And of course if one conducts enough tests, with enough specifications, measures, models, and recoding, one can induce a statistically significant result even if the relationship is random (classic "Type 1 error"). Indeed, this problem of "model dependence" is precisely what matching methods are designed to remedy. Using exact matching for Sander's newly defined outcome, thereby eliminating unjustified assumptions, the conclusion remains the same: Tier has no detectable effect on first-time bar passage for students who are similar in all other respects. Third, Sander asserts that my analysis "ignores law school performance."' 9 My analysis excludes law school grades precisely because it investigates the causal effect of law school tier on bar passage. I do not dispute Sander's finding that going to a higher-tier law school reduces grades. Such a finding isn't surprising-after all, we would expect that a higher-tier law school presents a more competitive environment. Moreover, I do not dispute that law school grades are correlated with bar passage. Precisely for these reasons, we cannot control for law school performance to assess the causal effect of law school tier. This is a textbook example of bias induced by controlling for a consequence of the cause. Sander also makes two specific criticisms of my analysis, each of which applies equally to his original analysis. First, Sander claims that law school tier is mismeasured and hence that the results of my analysis do not prove much. 2 Yet if law school tier, the key causal variable, is mismeasured, the original analysis fails as well. Sander cannot have it both ways: The assumptions required to interpret the matching estimates causally are uniformly less onerous than those for his regression analysis. Asserting that the assumptions are violated proves the point that Sander's original inference that law school tier causes black students to fail the bar cannot 16. The fact that all individual coefficients in this path diagram are statistically significant at.01 does not mean that the total effect of tier is. 17. This substantive effect is glossed over by standardization, which does not accord with a substantive quantity of interest and does not make the estimates comparable. See Gary King, How Not To Lie with Statistics: Avoiding Common Mistakes in Quantitative Political Science, 30 AM. J. POL. SCI. 666, 669-74 (1986). 18. Sander, supra note 6, at 446. 19. Sander, supra note 2, at 2009. 20. Id.

2005] Affirmative Actions 2015 be sustained. 21 Second, Sander argues that there are unobservable differences between similarly qualified students that invalidate matching. 22 As I stated in my Comment, unobservable differences in these students would invalidate both my reanalysis and Sander's analysis. 23 Both matching and regression assume that there are no remaining unobserved differences in students of different tiers when holding constant pretreatment variables. 24 Unless the researcher gathers more data (as in my accompanying paper, 25 which controls for 170 additional covariates), we have no idea what impact unobservables might have on the estimates. Sander's claim that matching maximizes bias along unobservables compared to regression is a conjecture without any theoretical basis. The claim depends on the correlation between unobserved and observed variables, and as such it cannot be tested in the data. 26 For example, if higher-tier students have a higher (unobserved) underlying propensity to take the bar in "tough" jurisdictions like California or New York, the true causal effect of law school tier could actually be positive if we do not control for region. 27 In short, Sander's charge of unobserved variables concedes that the estimates of his own original analysis are unfounded. If matched students cannot be viewed as anywhere close to randomly assigned to any reliable measure of law school tier, 28 then we cannot put credence in the estimates of the causal effect of law school tier presented by Sander or my Comment. The purpose of outlining the hypothetical experiment is to lay bare such critical assumptions for drawing a causal inference, thereby permitting scholars to assess the credibility of the estimates, which is what my Comment aimed to achieve. 21. Sander asserts that matching "singles out those pairs of students for whom 'tier' is least meaningful and most biased." Id. at 2010. If students differ in unobservable respects, however, we cannot attribute differences in bar passage rates to law school tier at all. 22. Id. 23. Ho, supra note 1, at 2001. 24. To be clear, some regression techniques account for some types of unobserved heterogeneity, but not the logistic regression employed by Sander. 25. Daniel E. Ho, Evaluating Affirmative Action in American Law Schools: Does Attending a Better Law School Cause Black Students To Fail the Bar? (Mar. 9, 2005) (unpublished manuscript), available at http://www.yalelawjoumal.org and http://people.iq.harvard.edu/-dho/ research/sander.pdf. 26. The standard advice in the literature is to first obtain balance on observables and then to conduct sensitivity analyses to examine to what degree inferences would change if some unobserved variable were correlated to the treatment and the outcome. In such an analysis, inferences could certainly change, but the actual truth cannot be ascertained from the data, because the data is by definition observed. See Paul R. Rosenbaun & Donald B. Rubin, Assessing Sensitivity to an Unobserved Binary Covariate in an Observational Study with Binary Outcome, 45 J. ROYAL STAT. SOC'Y SERIES B (METHODOLOGICAL) 212 (1983). 27. Cf P.J. Bickel et al., Sex Bias in Graduate Admissions: Data from Berkeley, 187 SCIENCE 398 (1975) (reversing a finding of gender discrimination in graduate school admissions by controlling for academic department). 28. See Sander, supra note 2, at 2010.

2016 The Yale Law Journal [Vol. 114:2011 To conclude, I want to underscore one basic point of agreement with Sander: The empirical investigation of affirmative action is important and should be subjected to scientific scrutiny. The fortunate fact is that we are not alone in this venture. Tools for analysis have been developed across academic fields, providing some easy fixes to reassess the mismatch hypothesis with more credible and theoretically consistent assumptions. This reanalysis shows that similarly qualified black students perform just as well on the bar irrespective of law school tier. Of course, law school tier may affect many other outcomes, like public service, life satisfaction, and career success, each of which merits independent investigation. This data set thereby provides a great opportunity for legal scholars to capitalize on developments in other academic disciplines, helping us to better understand the effect of law school tier and possibly of affirmative action.