PERCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW PHONEME CONTRAST BY ADULT AND 12-YEAR-OLD LISTENERS

Similar documents
Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Learners Use Word-Level Statistics in Phonetic Category Acquisition

A Comparison of the Effects of Two Practice Session Distribution Types on Acquisition and Retention of Discrete and Continuous Skills

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Effects of Open-Set and Closed-Set Task Demands on Spoken Word Recognition

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Rhythm-typology revisited.

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Eli Yamamoto, Satoshi Nakamura, Kiyohiro Shikano. Graduate School of Information Science, Nara Institute of Science & Technology

Perceptual Auditory Aftereffects on Voice Identity Using Brief Vowel Stimuli

Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience

Evolution of Symbolisation in Chimpanzees and Neural Nets

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

1. REFLEXES: Ask questions about coughing, swallowing, of water as fast as possible (note! Not suitable for all

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Investigating speech perception in children with dyslexia: is there evidence of a. consistent deficit in individuals? Abstract

Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies Data: 18/11/ :52:20. New Horizons in English Studies 1/2016

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

Enhancing Van Hiele s level of geometric understanding using Geometer s Sketchpad Introduction Research purpose Significance of study

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Processing Lexically Embedded Spoken Words

School Size and the Quality of Teaching and Learning

Speech Perception in Dyslexic Children. With and Without Language Impairments. Franklin R. Manis. University of Southern California.

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD

How do adults reason about their opponent? Typologies of players in a turn-taking game

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

Journal of Phonetics

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (3), September /97/102(3)/1891/7/$ Acoustical Society of America 1891

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Acoustic correlates of stress and their use in diagnosing syllable fusion in Tongan. James White & Marc Garellek UCLA

Speech Segmentation Using Probabilistic Phonetic Feature Hierarchy and Support Vector Machines

**Note: this is slightly different from the original (mainly in format). I would be happy to send you a hard copy.**

Probabilistic principles in unsupervised learning of visual structure: human data and a model

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Phonological encoding in speech production

Sex Differences in Self-Efficacy and Attributions: Influence of Performance Feedback

Universal contrastive analysis as a learning principle in CAPT

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voiced-voiceless distinction in alaryngeal speech - acoustic and articula

Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge of a Mathematics Problem: Their Measurement and Their Causal Interrelations

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Psychology of Speech Production and Speech Perception

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants

Perceptual scaling of voice identity: common dimensions for different vowels and speakers

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

A. What is research? B. Types of research

Rachel E. Baker, Ann R. Bradlow. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Voice conversion through vector quantization

A NOTE ON THE BIOLOGY OF SPEECH PERCEPTION* Michael Studdert-Kennedy+

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

P. Belsis, C. Sgouropoulou, K. Sfikas, G. Pantziou, C. Skourlas, J. Varnas

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

ANALYSIS: LABOUR MARKET SUCCESS OF VOCATIONAL AND HIGHER EDUCATION GRADUATES

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

A Case-Based Approach To Imitation Learning in Robotic Agents

Speech Perception Deficits in Poor Readers: Auditory Processing or Phonological Coding?*

DEVELOPMENT OF LINGUAL MOTOR CONTROL IN CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS

Different Task Type and the Perception of the English Interdental Fricatives

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Online Publication Date: 01 May 1981 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Student Perceptions of Reflective Learning Activities

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Effect of Cognitive Apprenticeship Instructional Method on Auto-Mechanics Students

SARDNET: A Self-Organizing Feature Map for Sequences

The Effectiveness of Realistic Mathematics Education Approach on Ability of Students Mathematical Concept Understanding

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

A Neural Network GUI Tested on Text-To-Phoneme Mapping

An Empirical Analysis of the Effects of Mexican American Studies Participation on Student Achievement within Tucson Unified School District

Linguistics. The School of Humanities

Lecture 1: Machine Learning Basics

Segregation of Unvoiced Speech from Nonspeech Interference

Effective practices of peer mentors in an undergraduate writing intensive course

Capturing and Organizing Prior Student Learning with the OCW Backpack

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. VCV-sequencies in a preliminary text-to-speech system for female speech

Innovative Methods for Teaching Engineering Courses

Instructor: Mario D. Garrett, Ph.D. Phone: Office: Hepner Hall (HH) 100

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

Observing Teachers: The Mathematics Pedagogy of Quebec Francophone and Anglophone Teachers

The Efficacy of PCI s Reading Program - Level One: A Report of a Randomized Experiment in Brevard Public Schools and Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Exploring Dyslexics Phonological Deficit I: Lexical vs Sub-lexical and Input vs Output Processes

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Transcription:

PERCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW PHONEME CONTRAST BY ADULT AND 12-YEAR-OLD LISTENERS Willemijn Heeren UiL OTS, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands willemijn.heeren@let.uu.nl ABSTRACT How does the perception of a new phoneme contrast develop? Are differences found across age groups? In answering these questions, we use two alternative hypotheses: i) Acquired Distinctiveness: before learning, differences between and within phoneme categories are relatively hard to discriminate. Through training, the phoneme boundary is learned. ii) Acquired Similarity: before learning, differences between and within phoneme categories are relatively well discriminated. Due to training, only the phoneme boundary remains discriminable. We will discuss two perception experiments. In experiment I, Dutch adults learned the British-English pseudowords thif and sif (the first consonant in thif, /θ/, is not a phoneme of Dutch). Between pretest and posttest with materials from one speaker, participants were trained with speech from five other speakers. This forced them to form abstract phoneme categories. The results show that trained listeners seemed to perform better than control listeners in the posttest. However, in general, the control group, who received no training, was difficult to distinguish from the trained listeners. In experiment II, a small group of 12-year-old Dutch children was tested on the same contrast. They received a restricted amount of training. With respect to our question of how the perception of a new phoneme contrasts develops, it appeared that similar learning paths were used by both age groups: discrimination levels increased as a result of learning. INTRODUCTION Learning a language implies learning to perceive unknown phoneme contrasts. But how does the perception of a new phoneme contrast develop? We start from two hypotheses that have originally been posed to test learning of native phonemes (Liberman et al., 191). The first, Acquired Distinctiveness, says that, before learning, differences between and within phoneme categories are hardly discriminable. Through training, the phoneme boundary is learned. Jamieson and Morosan (19) report results in line with this hypothesis. Secondly, according to Acquired Similarity, differences between and within phoneme categories are well discriminated before learning. Due to training, only the phoneme boundary remains discriminable. Learning by infants, even though this may not be at the phonemic level, has been mentioned as consistent with this hypothesis (Pisoni, 1991). A second question we want to answer is: do different age groups learn new contrasts in similar ways? Children up to 12 years of age may still be acquiring their native-language phonemes (Hazan & Barrett, 2000), which could influence their learning of foreign languages, as opposed to adults, whose native language is already in place. This paper will discuss the perceptual development of the /θ-s/ contrast from British-English by Dutch adult and 12-year-old listeners. Generally, the Dutch begin learning English as a second language early. But, as Collins and Mees (1999) observe, they often replace /θ/ with /s/ in From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-31

production. The adults are expected to follow Acquired Distinctiveness, which will probably also be the way the 12-year-olds learn the new phoneme contrast. EXPERIMENT I: ADULTS Method Materials Continua of eight stimuli were synthesized from the British-English phonemes /θ/ and /s/ by means of linear spectral interpolation (van Hessen, 1992). The phonemes occurred in the onset positions of a pair of (both Dutch and English) nonsense words: thif sif. Continua were based on speech from six British-English, male and female, speakers. The phoneme boundary in each continuum had been determined in a classification study with 31 native British-English listeners. Participants In the test 34 students participated, all native speakers of Dutch. None of them were students of English. The first 1 subjects received training between pretest and posttest. The other 1, the control group, took only part in pretest and posttest. Design A pretest-posttest design was used. In both pre- and posttest 4IAX discrimination and absolute identification with speech from one male speaker were run. 4IAX discrimination was expected to reflect both phonemic and auditory perception of the stimulus pairs (Pisoni, 19; Gerrits, 2001). Learning by Acquired Distinctiveness would become most apparent in a 1-step test, since the discrimination level in this test was expected to be low from the beginning. Learning by Acquired Similarity would show in a 3-step test, since its initially high discrimination levels could fall as a result of training. In absolute identification the listener has as many response options as there are stimuli. The test reflects the listener s grasp of the continuum. Classification training was chosen to direct the participants attention towards the two categories (e.g. Jamieson & Morosan, 19). Training materials were continua from five speakers other than the test speaker, encouraging robust category formation (Lively et al., 1993). Procedure All experiments were run in a quiet room at the Utrecht Institute of Linguistics OTS. A laptop computer was used both to present the stimuli at random and to register responses. Listeners were tested individually. Stimuli were presented over headphones at a comfortable listening level. The pretest was completed on the first day. On subsequent days, training sessions were run until the listener classified the new phoneme contrast correctly in at least % of the trials. On the final day the posttest took place. In all tests breaks were given at regular intervals, response times were unlimited, and trial order was randomized. 4IAX Discrimination: On each trial, a stimulus pair was presented in one of eight possible orders (AB-AA, AA-BA, BA-AA, AA-AB, BB-AB, BB-BA, AB-BB or BA-BB). This test was given twice, once with 1-step (i.e. 1-2, 2-3, ) and once with 3-step stimulus pairs (i.e. 1-4, 2-, ) and their order was balanced across subjects. Listeners received written instructions asking them to indicate whether the first or second word pair consisted of the same stimuli. It was stressed that From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-32

differences could be small. The task introduction consisted of eight four-step stimulus pairs from the same continuum. ISI s were 300 ms and inter-pair intervals 00 ms. All orders per stimulus pair were each presented four times. Absolute identification: Listeners received written instructions, asking them to indicate which stimulus from the continuum they had heard. The instructions included a picture of eight numbered buttons. Over the first and last button, pictures of men were plotted. It was explained that each button hid a unique word and that words changed in steps from the name of the first man (behind button 1) to the name of the second man (behind button ). Next, the stimuli were introduced five times in various sequences. During testing, stimuli were presented 20 times. Classification training: Listeners had to reach at least % correct identification over two subsequent training tests before proceeding to the posttest. On each training session, one to four training tests of 40 trials each were run. The test was introduced by both endpoint stimuli from each of the five training speakers. Listeners received immediate feedback on each trial, informing them of the correctness of their choice, and were also regularly shown their percentage of correct responses so far. Training tests contained 12 repetitions per stimulus. Results Repeated measures ANOVA s were run with within-subjects factors Test (2) and Stimulus Pair () or Stimulus Number (), and between-subjects factor Listener Group (2). 4IAX discrimination Main effects of Test (F[1,30]=14.9, p=.001) and Stimulus (F[,10]=10.2, p<.001) were found. The results are shown in figure 1. Listeners mean scores increased from pretest to posttest ( to 1%), but participants did not distinguish all stimulus pairs equally well. No effect of Listener Group was found, which means that the trained listeners did not perform significantly better than the controls. A post-hoc ANOVA on the posttest data did reveal an effect of Listener Group (F[1,224]=., p=.00): the trained listeners scored better than those in the control group. 3-step 4IAX discrimination showed similar results. Main effects of Test (F[1,30]=13.4, p=.001) and Stimulus Pair (F[3.4,101.4]=9.3, p<.001) were found. Percentages correct increased from % before to 93% after training. Again, no effect of Listener Group was found. Within the posttest data, trained listeners did perform better than controls (F[1,1]=10., p=.001). Absolute identification A Test x Stimulus interaction (F[4.,13]=3.9, p=.003) was found. At the /θ/-end of the continuum, participants became better at identifying the stimuli, while this was not the case at the /s/-end. Furthermore, main effects of Test (F[1,30]=2.2, p<.001) and Stimulus (F[2.,.3]=94.1, p<.001) were found. But again, no effect of Listener Group was present. The mean response variances also showed main effects of Test (F[1,30]=20., p<.001) and of Stimulus (F[4.3,129.]=., p<.001). Posttest variances were smaller than those in the pretest, which means that participants had become better at identifying the stimuli. In summary, differences between pretest and posttest were present. Trained listeners progress, however, was not easily distinguished from that of controls. The amount of training needed to reach criterion varied among test listeners: some needed only 90 training trials, while others needed as many as 0 trials. It is conceivable that listeners From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-33

who needed more training also show larger differences between pre- and posttest. Therefore, the trained listeners data were retested, this time with Training Amount as a covariate. The findings differing from the effects described earlier are reported here. For 3-step 4IAX discrimination, a Text x Training Amount interaction was found (F[1,1]=4., p=.044), as well as a main effect of Training Amount (F[1,1]=1.4, p=.001). The rise in correct responses between pre- and posttest was generally larger as the amount of training increased. The mean response variances in absolute identification also showed a main effect of Training Amount (F[1,1]=., p=.033). Apparently, the more training a listener needed, the larger the effects were. 0 Trained listeners 0 Control listeners 0 0 % correct 40 1-2 2-3 3-4 4- - - - % correct 40 1-2 2-3 3-4 4- - - - /θ/ Stimulus pair /s/ /θ/ Stimulus pair /s/ Figure 1. 4IAX discrimination of 1-step stimulus pairs by adult listeners in pretest (dashed) and posttest (solid), with a reference line at 0 % correct. Discussion Differences between trained and control listeners were small. Within the posttest data, however, the groups differed in a way consistent with learning by the trained group. But improvement was not restricted to the phoneme boundary; increased perceptual sensitivity was mostly found within phoneme categories. EXPERIMENT II: 12-YEAR-OLDS Method A group of thirteen 12-year-old children participated: eight in the training group, and five controls. The same materials as in experiment I were used. Procedure All experiments were run in a relatively quiet room at the children s school. The procedure was similar to that of the adults with two exceptions: (i) the amount of training children received was restricted to three 20-trial sessions, and (ii) children completed 4I-oddity discrimination with 00 ms ISI s instead of 4IAX discrimination. 4I-oddity has a smaller number of stimulus permutations (ABAA and AABA), making it less time-consuming. According to Gerrits (2001) the 4I-oddity test reflects auditory rather than phonemic discrimination. We therefore expected to find no differences between pre- and posttest discrimination results. From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-34

Results Data from three trained children were not included in the analyses. Two of them did not improve during training. A third child did not complete the tests due to illness. 4I-oddity discrimination A main effect of Test was found (F[1,]=.4, p=.02). On average, posttest scores (9%) were higher than pretest scores (4%). Mean percentages of correct responses in 3-step discrimination did not increase significantly (pretest % to posttest 0%). Mean response Trained listeners 4 3 2 1 1 2 3 4 Mean response Control listeners 4 3 2 1 1 2 3 4 /θ/ /s/ /θ/ /s/ Stimulus Stimulus Figure 2. Absolute identification by 12-year-olds in pretest (dashed) and posttest (solid) with a diagonal representing ideal absolute identification. Absolute identification Figure 2 shows the mean responses to each of the stimuli in absolute identification. The mean responses showed a Test x Stimulus interaction (F[4.0,32.2]=.2, p=.002), as well as a main effect of Stimulus (F[1.,11.]=41.9, p<.001). Furthermore, a marginally significant Listener Group effect was found (F[1,]=4., p=.0). Even though both groups became more accurate at the /θ/-end of the continuum, this effect was larger for the trained listeners. The mean response variances showed a Test x Listener Group interaction (F[1,]=9.9, p=.014) and a main effect of Test (F[1,]=1.2, p=.004). Trained listeners became more accurate at most stimuli, whereas control listeners did not improve. Post hoc ANOVA s revealed main effects of Listener Group in both pre- and posttest (F[1,4]=.4, p=.023, F[1,4]=.2, p=.009, respectively): trained listeners variances were smaller than those of the controls in the posttest data whereas they had been larger in the pretest. Discussion Despite the small number of participants and limited training, differences between trained and control listeners were found, mainly in absolute identification: trained listeners became more accurate in their responses, as reflected by their variances, whereas the controls did not. From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-3

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS Both adults and 12-year-olds discrimination levels increased as a result of learning. Most improvement, however, was found within phoneme categories. The results are most consistent with learning by Acquired Distinctiveness, but do not strongly support the hypothesis since an increase in discrimination scores at the phoneme boundary was not clearly found. This effect may have been caused by relatively high pretest scores by the adults and by the nature of the discrimination tasks. The adult control group has shown that improvements in test performance were reached without training with multiple speakers: to them, the pretest may have been a training in itself. They also had more experience with English opposed to the child controls, who hardly improved without training. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Collection of the materials was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). REFERENCES Collins, B. & Mees, I.C. (1999) The Phonetics of English and Dutch. Brill: Leiden. Gerrits, E. (2001) The Categorisation of Speech Sounds by Adults and Children, Doctoral Dissertation, Utrecht University. Hazan, V. & Barrett, S. (2000) The development of phonemic categorization in children aged - 12, Journal of Phonetics, 2, 3-39. Hessen, A. van (1992) Discrimination of Familiar and Unfamiliar Speech Sounds, Doctoral Dissertation, Utrecht University. Jamieson, D.G. & Morosan, D.E. (19) Training non-native speech contrasts in adults: acquisition of the English / /-/θ/ contrast by francophones, Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 20-21. Liberman, A.M., Harris, K.S., Kinney, J.A. & Lane, H. (191) The discrimination of relative onset-time of the components of certain speech and nonspeech patterns, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(), 39-3. Lively, S.E., Logan, J.S. & Pisoni, D.B. (1993) Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/ II: The role of phonetic environment and talker variability in learning new perceptual categories, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94(3), 1242-12. Pisoni, D.B. (19) Auditory short-term memory and vowel perception. Memory & Cognition, 3, -1. Pisoni, D.B. (1991) Modes of processing speech and nonspeech signals. In Modularity and the Motor Theory of Speech Perception (edited by I.G. Mattingly and M. Studdert-Kennedy), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 22-23. From Sound to Sense: June 11 June 13, 2004 at MIT C-3