Ambiguous Novel Compounds and Models of Morphological Parsing

Similar documents
An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Linking Task: Identifying authors and book titles in verbose queries

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Lexical Access during Sentence Comprehension (Re)Consideration of Context Effects

The Perception of Nasalized Vowels in American English: An Investigation of On-line Use of Vowel Nasalization in Lexical Access

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

VOL. 3, NO. 5, May 2012 ISSN Journal of Emerging Trends in Computing and Information Sciences CIS Journal. All rights reserved.

Parallel Evaluation in Stratal OT * Adam Baker University of Arizona

Which verb classes and why? Research questions: Semantic Basis Hypothesis (SBH) What verb classes? Why the truth of the SBH matters

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Aging and the Use of Context in Ambiguity Resolution: Complex Changes From Simple Slowing

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

Phonological Encoding in Sentence Production

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

Processing Lexically Embedded Spoken Words

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

A Case Study: News Classification Based on Term Frequency

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

LING 329 : MORPHOLOGY

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

Derivational: Inflectional: In a fit of rage the soldiers attacked them both that week, but lost the fight.

have to be modeled) or isolated words. Output of the system is a grapheme-tophoneme conversion system which takes as its input the spelling of words,

Syntax Parsing 1. Grammars and parsing 2. Top-down and bottom-up parsing 3. Chart parsers 4. Bottom-up chart parsing 5. The Earley Algorithm

Multiple Route Model of Lexical Processing

Automatization and orthographic development in second language visual word recognition

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

A Derived Transformation of Valence Functions Across Two 8-Member Comparative Relational Networks

Session 2B From understanding perspectives to informing public policy the potential and challenges for Q findings to inform survey design

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

Probability and Statistics Curriculum Pacing Guide

The Strong Minimalist Thesis and Bounded Optimality

Test Blueprint. Grade 3 Reading English Standards of Learning

Evaluation of Teach For America:

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Deliberate Learning and Vocabulary Acquisition in a Second Language

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Summary / Response. Karl Smith, Accelerations Educational Software. Page 1 of 8

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

Speech Recognition at ICSI: Broadcast News and beyond

Retrieval in cued recall

Generating Test Cases From Use Cases

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Parsing of part-of-speech tagged Assamese Texts

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Characteristics of Functions

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 7: Number

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

Writing quality predicts Chinese learning

Strategies for Solving Fraction Tasks and Their Link to Algebraic Thinking

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

Phonological encoding in speech production

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

Abstractions and the Brain

Learning From the Past with Experiment Databases

Instructor: Mario D. Garrett, Ph.D. Phone: Office: Hepner Hall (HH) 100

Introduction to the Practice of Statistics

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Field Experience Management 2011 Training Guides

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

TIMSS ADVANCED 2015 USER GUIDE FOR THE INTERNATIONAL DATABASE. Pierre Foy

raıs Factors affecting word learning in adults: A comparison of L2 versus L1 acquisition /r/ /aı/ /s/ /r/ /aı/ /s/ = individual sound

CS Machine Learning

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Taught Throughout the Year Foundational Skills Reading Writing Language RF.1.2 Demonstrate understanding of spoken words,

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

The Representation of Concrete and Abstract Concepts: Categorical vs. Associative Relationships. Jingyi Geng and Tatiana T. Schnur

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

School Size and the Quality of Teaching and Learning

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

A Bootstrapping Model of Frequency and Context Effects in Word Learning

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

NCEO Technical Report 27

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

Save Children. Can Math Recovery. before They Fail?

(Includes a Detailed Analysis of Responses to Overall Satisfaction and Quality of Academic Advising Items) By Steve Chatman

Chromatography Syllabus and Course Information 2 Credits Fall 2016

Revisiting the role of prosody in early language acquisition. Megha Sundara UCLA Phonetics Lab

Learning Structural Correspondences Across Different Linguistic Domains with Synchronous Neural Language Models

Minimalism is the name of the predominant approach in generative linguistics today. It was first

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France

Sensitivity to second language argument structure

Transcription:

Brain and Language 68, 378 386 (1999) Article ID brln.1999.2093, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Ambiguous Novel Compounds and Models of Morphological Parsing Gary Libben, Bruce L. Derwing, and Roberto G. de Almeida University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada This paper reports on two experiments that investigated the activation of morphemes in English novel compounds. All experiments employed stimuli that we have called ambiguous novel compounds. These words (e.g., clamprod) have two interpretable parses (e.g., clam prod or clamp rod) and thus offer an opportunity to investigate which parses are preferred, whether both possible parses are computed, and whether parsing procedures divide words into their morphological constituents or extract constituent representations. The results suggest that morphological parsing does not simply divide a word into its constituents, but rather generates multiple representations that are subsequently evaluated. 1999 Academic Press Companies that create names for new products such as Powerbar and Dryloft proceed from the assumption that the constituent morphemes of these novel compounds are easily and automatically accessible to readers of English. While this is almost certainly a sound and uncontroversial assumption, identifying the mechanisms that allow for such constituent activation is considerably less straightforward (see Taft & Forster 1976; Taft 1981; Bergman, Hudson, & Eiling 1988; Libben 1998). Because novel compounds do not have whole-word representations in the mind, a prelexical parsing procedure This research was supported by a Major Collaborative Research Initiative Grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada awarded to Gonia Jarema (Coprincipal Investigator and Director), Eva Kehayia (Co-principal Investigator), and Gary Libben (Co-principal Investigator) and by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Research Grant awarded to Bruce Derwing (Principal Investigator), Gary Libben, and Terrance Nearey. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. We also gratefully acknowledge the contributions of Martha Gibson, Patrick Cooper, and Julia Peters in both the planning and testing phases of this research. Address reprint requests to Gary Libben, University of Alberta Linguistics Department, Edmonton, Alberta T6G 2E7, Canada. 0093-934X/99 $30.00 Copyright 1999 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 378

AMBIGUOUS NOVEL COMPOUNDS 379 seems to be required to account for the recognition of their components. Furthermore, this decomposition cannot proceed by removing affixes because, by definition, these words are constructed through the concatenation of morphological roots. In this paper, we report on an investigation of how prelexical parsing is achieved. Our study focuses on a set of tough cases ambiguous novel compounds which were introduced by Libben (1994) as a stimulus type that offers a window into the mechanisms of prelexical parsing. Ambiguous novel compounds are strings such as clamprod which have two possible parses (in this case clam-prod and clamp-rod). Libben (1994) argued that if prelexical parsing were characterized by a simple left-to-right parsing procedure, then the preferred parse for such compounds would always isolate the first possible constituent of the ambiguous compound (i.e., clam-prod). We term this the first possible parse hypothesis and contrast it with the last possible parse hypothesis which, in the case of clamprod, would generate clamp-rod. Libben (1994) concluded that neither of these alternatives characterize the prelexical parsing procedure. Rather, he claimed that ambiguous novel compounds are processed through a recursive parsing procedure that results in the creation of all possible morphologically-legal representations. In the experiments reported below, this claim was tested using a new experimental paradigm that was developed to probe both constituent activation and the mechanisms of prelexical parsing. EXPERIMENT 1: PARSING AMBIGUOUS NOVEL COMPOUNDS Our first experiment addressed the question of whether or not native speakers of English show a tendency to assign a first possible parse to ambiguous novel compounds. The experiment employed the morpheme recall task which was developed to be used with large groups of participants to uncover their spontaneous parsing preferences. Participants Fifty-two undergraduate students in a introductory course in linguistics participated as a single group in the experiment. Procedure All participants were tested in a single session. The experimental stimuli were presented on a large screen in a university amphitheater using an In Focus liquid crystal display that allowed the screen of a Macintosh PowerBook 520 to be displayed to a large group of participants. In the morpheme recall task, each trial had four components: an alert component, a stimulus

380 LIBBEN, DERWING, AND DE ALMEIDA FIG. 1. A single trial in the morpheme recall task. component, a focus component, and a response component (see Fig. 1). The alert component consisted of an auditory beep and the presentation of a in the center of the screen. In the stimulus component, participants saw three stimuli presented in succession. The first was a monomorphemic word presented at the top of the screen. The second was a compound presented in the center of the screen, and the third was another monomorphemic word presented at the bottom of the screen. Each stimulus was centered with respect to the left and right screen margins and remained visible for 750 ms. The stimulus component was followed by a focus indication consisting of an arrow pointing in one of four directions. In the final response component, participants were required to write down the morpheme that corresponded to the arrow direction. If the arrow pointed upward, they were to write the word presented at the top of the screen. If it pointed downward, they were required to write down the word that appeared at the bottom. In the critical left arrow or right arrow conditions, they were required to write down either the first or last morpheme of the compound word. The experiment consisted of a ten-trial practice session and an experimental session consisting of 124 trials. The experimental session lasted 15 min and was conducted in a single block of trials. Because each trial involved the presentation of one compound and two monomorphemic words, participants were exposed to 248 monomorphemic words and 124 compounds in total. Sixty-two of these were the critical ambiguous novel compounds and each was randomly assigned to either the left or right arrow conditions. Participants were not in-

AMBIGUOUS NOVEL COMPOUNDS 381 TABLE 1 Parsing Preferences for Ambiguous Novel Compounds Focus First parse Last parse (clam vs clamp) 56% 44% (prod vs rod) 48% 52% Mean 52% 48% formed of the ambiguity of the critical stimuli until the debriefing session that followed the experiment. Results Thirty-one of the 62 ambiguous novel compounds were accompanied by a left arrow and 31 were accompanied by a right arrow. Parsing preferences were calculated by counting the number of first parses vs. the number of last parses for each compound. This yielded the values in Table 1. Although overall parsing choices showed a tendency to favor first parses, this difference was not significant and analyses by items revealed substantial variation among individual ambiguous novel compounds. We explored the source of this variation in a subanalysis in which the number of first parses for each item was correlated to the semantic plausibility rating for the first parse of that item. The semantic plausibility rating was obtained from a separate group of 32 undergraduate students registered in a different section of the same introductory course in linguistics. In that task, participants were shown both readings of all ambiguous novel compounds and were asked to rate each reading on a five-point scale of plausibility. The resulting correlation between parsing preference and semantic plausibility was.74. We conducted a similar analysis to investigate the relation between parsing choices and constituent frequency. This yielded a correlation of.42 for the number of first parses and the frequency of first parse constituents as rated by 28 undergraduate linguistics students. The correlation of parsing preference with the Kucera & Francis (1967) frequency count yielded r.45. Discussion The results suggest that prelexical morphological parsing does not proceed in a simple left-to-right manner that is terminated by the creation of a legal and interpretable parse. As was discussed above, this predicts that participants would show consistent first possible parses, which is not what we observed. Moreover, our second finding in this experiment that parsing choices are highly correlated with semantic plausibility suggests that both parses were generated by the prelexical decomposition procedure. Only in this case would it be possible for the participant to know which parse

382 LIBBEN, DERWING, AND DE ALMEIDA was the more plausible. We are led, therefore, to the view that the recognition of novel compounds such as clamprod results in the activation of the constituents of all parses (in this case, clam, clamp, prod, and rod). EXPERIMENT 2: SEMANTIC PRIMING IN AN ACCURACY PARADIGM The results of Experiment 1 lead to a straightforward prediction regarding semantic priming in the morpheme recall task. If indeed both clam and clamp are activated during the processing of the ambiguous novel compound clamprod, then this compound should prime semantic associates of each of these constituents (i.e., sea for clam and hold for clamp). This prediction was tested by modifying the morpheme recall task in the following manner: whereas in Experiment 1, the monomorphemic words that appeared above and below the critical compounds were fillers that made the recall task sufficiently difficult to yield an error rate, in Experiment 2 they were used instead as pre- and postprimes (because they either preceded or followed the critical compounds by 750 ms.). This overall design allowed us to investigate two separate questions in the same experiment: (1) whether ambiguous novel compounds facilitate recall accuracy to monomorphemic associates of both parsing choices and (2) whether the presence of semantically associated words in the presentation affects participants choice of initial morpheme for ambiguous novel compounds (i.e., whether it influences participants morphological parsing of those compounds). Participants One hundred fourteen native speakers of English registered in an introductory course in linguistics participated as volunteers in the experiment. None had participated in Experiment 1. Procedure Stimuli were presented to the group using the same apparatus as in Experiment 1. In this experiment, however, the participants were randomly assigned to four groups labeled by the card suits clubs, spades, hearts, and diamonds. The alert and stimulus presentation components of the experiment were identical to those in Experiment 1. However, the focus component differed in the following manner: Instead of seeing a single arrow pointing in one of four directions, participants saw a screen such as that presented in Fig. 2. In this example, participants in the hearts group would be required to write down the monomorphemic word that appeared at the bottom of the screen and participants in the spades group would be required to write down the first part of the compound word. The position of the card suits was rerandomized for each trial so that subjects in each of the card suit groups supplied data for all morpheme positions (above, left, right, and below). As in Experiment 1, participants completed a ten-trial practice session and an experimental session consisting of 124 trials of which 62 involved the critical ambiguous novel compounds.

AMBIGUOUS NOVEL COMPOUNDS 383 FIG. 2. The focus screen for Experiment 2. Results The recall of constituent associates. For each participant, accuracy scores were computed for the monomorphemic words that occurred above and below the ambiguous novel compounds. The monomorphemic words were constructed to be either semantically unrelated to any of the compound constituents, related to the first parse initial constituent (e.g., sea for clam in clamprod), or related to the second parse initial constituent (e.g., hold for clamp in clamprod). Our analysis centered on these conditions as well as the two additional conditions in which the monomorphemic words were related to the same parse (e.g., sea-clamprod-shell) or conflicting parses (e.g., sea-clamprod-hold). Table 2 summarizes these accuracy scores. As can be seen in Table 2, accuracy scores were essentially parallel for monomorphemic words occurring above (i.e., before) and below (i.e., after) the compounds. For both monomorphemic positions, a main effect of condition was found TABLE 2 Accuracy Scores (Proportion Correct) for Monomorphemic Words Shown Above and Below Ambiguous Novel Compounds Monomorphemic word Above Below Unrelated to either parse.75 (.35).74 (.21) Related to first parse (other is unrelated).87 (.18).82 (.32) Related to second parse (other is unrelated).85 (.19).85 (.27) Related to first parse (other is related to second).87 (.27).86 (.27) Related to first parse (other is supporting).90 (.30).83 (.32) Note. Standard deviations are provided in brackets.

384 LIBBEN, DERWING, AND DE ALMEIDA TABLE 3 Parsing Choices (Proportion of the First Possible Parses) for Ambiguous Novel Compounds in Relation to Priming Conditions Presentation condition Proportion of first parses Unrelated to either parse.70 (.10) Related to first parse (other is unrelated).52 (.27) Related to second parse (other is unrelated).81 (.15) Related (other is contradictory).34 (.64) Related to first parse (other is supporting).83 (.11) Related to second parse (other is supporting).79 (.19) Note. Standard deviations are provided in brackets. (F above (4,452) 3.8, p.004; F below (4,200) 2.7, p.03). 1 No significant differences were found among any of the related conditions. However, for both positions, single-df comparisons showed accuracy scores in the unrelated condition to be significantly lower than all others (p.002 in both analyses). In summary, then, ambiguous compounds primed semantic associates of both their parses in both conditions, suggesting that both parses were actually conducted. This effect was not changed by the presence of contradictory information in the stimulus presentation. The effect of constituent associates on compound parsing. The observation that contradictory information did not inhibit the priming effects leads to the expectation that the parsing process itself is not affected by the presence of the monomorphemic primes. This is exactly what our analysis of parsing choices revealed (this analysis is essentially the same as that conducted for Experiment 1). Our analysis focused on 23 ambiguous novel compounds that showed the greatest likelihood of being affected by monomorphemic primes because they did not contain a graphemic or phonological structure that would constrain their parses. Thus ambiguous novel compounds containing digraphs (e.g., seathorn) and those in which different parses possessed different syllable structures (e.g., planetrail) were excluded from the analysis. As can be seen in Table 3, the data do not fall into a consistent pattern in relation to the presence, absence, or combinations of monomorphemic primes. Because in this paradigm relatively few compounds were involved and because participants responded to different compounds under different conditions, we conclude that the response variance is attributable to semantic properties of compounds and the frequencies of their constituents (because phonological and graphemic factors were controlled). In any case, it seems 1 Data points were included in the within-subjects ANOVA only if participants responded to all five conditions.

AMBIGUOUS NOVEL COMPOUNDS 385 clear that the idiosyncratic properties of individual compounds cannot be overridden by priming effects in this paradigm. GENERAL DISCUSSION In this study we have introduced a new experimental technique that targets morphological processing in a nonchronometric, mass presentation paradigm. The results of the two experiments that we have reported using this paradigm can be summarized in the following manner: Parsing choices for ambiguous novel compounds do not seem to be determined by the operation of the prelexical parser. Thus, readers are not uniformly led by the parser to either a first possible parse or last possible parse of an ambiguous compound. Rather, the primary function of the prelexical parser seems to be to supply all possible parses of a string. This conclusion is supported by the finding in this study that ambiguous compounds prime semantic associates of all constituents. This conclusion also follows from the results of Libben, Derwing, and de Almeida (1999) who found that naming latencies to ambiguous novel compounds such as clamprod were longer than those to reversals of these compounds (e.g., prodclamp) which contain the identical morphemes, but are rendered unambiguous as a result of the constituent reversal. Libben, Derwing, and de Almeida reasoned that this increased response latency reflects additional activity at the level of prelexical parsing as well as additional activity required at a later stage of processing in which one parse is chosen over the other. We are left, then, with converging evidence for a view of morphological parsing in which relatively autonomous procedures interact to yield final interpretations for novel compounds. This parsing modularity was particularly evident in Experiment 2 of this study, in which we found extended semantic priming effects result as an outcome of prelexical parsing, but did not find that semantic priming could affect the prelexical parsing procedure itself. This latter finding forces us, in our view, toward a re-analysis of the role of morphological parsing in the lexical processing system as a whole. From the outset, a key assumption in the literature on morphological parsing has been that a mental lexicon that is organized by morphemes would have the advantage of storage efficiency (Aitchison, 1994). It has also been assumed that the morphological parser functions as the front-end of this scheme to optimize storage efficiency. Yet in this study, we do not find evidence for a prelexical parser that is terribly concerned with efficiency. Rather, we find evidence that the parser passes on to the lexical level all possible morphemes that can be uncovered by left-to-right parsing. As was shown in Experiment 2, this parser seems not to be constructed to make use of hints (i.e., primes) that in most natural settings would also lead toward processing efficiency.

386 LIBBEN, DERWING, AND DE ALMEIDA In short, our study suggests that if prelexical parsing is indeed guided by any design principle, efficiency is not it. REFERENCES Aitchison, J. 1994. Words in the mind. Oxford: Blackwell. Berman, M. W., Hudson, P. T. W., & Eiling, P. 1988. How simple complex words can be: morphological processing and word representations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 41 72. Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. 1967. Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press. Libben, G. 1994. How is morphological decomposition achieved? Language and Cognitive Processes, 9(3), 369 391. Libben, G. 1998. Semantic transparency in the processing of compounds: Consequences for representation, processing, and impairment. Brain and Language 61, 30 44. Libben, G., Derwing, B. L., & de Almeida, R. G. (1999). Ambiguous novel compounds and prelexical parsing. Paper submitted for publication. Taft, M. (1981). Prefix stripping revisited. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 289 297. Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. 1976. Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 607 620.