Chapter Two LITERATURE REVIEW

Similar documents
Aspectual Classes of Verb Phrases

THE ACQUISITION OF PROGRESSIVE AND RESULTATIVE MEANINGS OF THE IMPERFECTIVE ASPECT MARKER BY L2 LEARNERS OF JAPANESE

THE PERCEPTIONS OF THE JAPANESE IMPERFECTIVE ASPECT MARKER TEIRU AMONG NATIVE SPEAKERS AND L2 LEARNERS OF JAPANESE

Dissertation Summaries. The Acquisition of Aspect and Motion Verbs in the Native Language (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 2014)

Describing Motion Events in Adult L2 Spanish Narratives

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Writing a composition

Lower and Upper Secondary

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Ch VI- SENTENCE PATTERNS.

Derivational and Inflectional Morphemes in Pak-Pak Language

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Chapter 3: Semi-lexical categories. nor truly functional. As Corver and van Riemsdijk rightly point out, There is more

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Construction Grammar. University of Jena.

Progressive Aspect in Nigerian English

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Words come in categories

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

arxiv: v1 [cs.cl] 2 Apr 2017

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

cambridge occasional papers in linguistics Volume 8, Article 3: 41 55, 2015 ISSN

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

Intension, Attitude, and Tense Annotation in a High-Fidelity Semantic Representation

AN EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH TO NEW AND OLD INFORMATION IN TURKISH LOCATIVES AND EXISTENTIALS

CONTENUTI DEL CORSO (presentazione di disciplina, argomenti, programma):

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Did they acquire? Or were they taught?

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 141 ( 2014 ) WCLTA Using Corpus Linguistics in the Development of Writing

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

GRAMMATICAL MORPHEME ACQUISITION: AN ANALYSIS OF AN EFL LEARNER S LANGUAGE SAMPLES *

A Comparative Study of Research Article Discussion Sections of Local and International Applied Linguistic Journals

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

The presence of interpretable but ungrammatical sentences corresponds to mismatches between interpretive and productive parsing.

Universal Grammar 2. Universal Grammar 1. Forms and functions 1. Universal Grammar 3. Conceptual and surface structure of complex clauses

Language Center. Course Catalog

Tutoring First-Year Writing Students at UNM

Written by: YULI AMRIA (RRA1B210085) ABSTRACT. Key words: ability, possessive pronouns, and possessive adjectives INTRODUCTION

Coast Academies Writing Framework Step 4. 1 of 7

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Phenomena of gender attraction in Polish *

Hindi Aspectual Verb Complexes

Review in ICAME Journal, Volume 38, 2014, DOI: /icame

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

National Literacy and Numeracy Framework for years 3/4

Sources of difficulties in cross-cultural communication and ELT: The case of the long-distance but in Chinese discourse

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

5 th Grade Language Arts Curriculum Map

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Linguistic Variation across Sports Category of Press Reportage from British Newspapers: a Diachronic Multidimensional Analysis

The BE + -ING form: Progressive aspect and metonymy

CAAP. Content Analysis Report. Sample College. Institution Code: 9011 Institution Type: 4-Year Subgroup: none Test Date: Spring 2011

Effect of Word Complexity on L2 Vocabulary Learning

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Iraqi EFL Students' Achievement In The Present Tense And Present Passive Constructions

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature

Module 12. Machine Learning. Version 2 CSE IIT, Kharagpur

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Proof Theory for Syntacticians

Language Acquisition Chart

Basic Syntax. Doug Arnold We review some basic grammatical ideas and terminology, and look at some common constructions in English.

Approaches to control phenomena handout Obligatory control and morphological case: Icelandic and Basque

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

The Structure of Multiple Complements to V

Beyond constructions:

Grade 5: Module 3A: Overview

Acquiring verb agreement in HKSL: Optional or obligatory?

a) analyse sentences, so you know what s going on and how to use that information to help you find the answer.

The Syntax of Inner Aspect

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER Teacher intervention in the process of L2 writing acquisition

Today we examine the distribution of infinitival clauses, which can be

Books Effective Literacy Y5-8 Learning Through Talk Y4-8 Switch onto Spelling Spelling Under Scrutiny

Course Outline for Honors Spanish II Mrs. Sharon Koller

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING

Frequency and pragmatically unmarked word order *

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

Discourse markers and grammaticalization

How to analyze visual narratives: A tutorial in Visual Narrative Grammar

The development of a new learner s dictionary for Modern Standard Arabic: the linguistic corpus approach

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Creating Travel Advice

THE ACQUISITION OF INFLECTIONAL MORPHEMES: THE PRIORITY OF PLURAL S

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

Transcription:

8 Chapter Two LITERATURE REVIEW The issue that how learners acquire the system of tense and aspect in their L2 has long been an interest in the research of second language acquisition. Many previous studies (such as Bardovi-Harlig 1999; Huang 1999; Robison 1990 & 1995; Salaberry 1999; Salaberry and Shirai 2002, etc.) have insightful investigations into this field. Bardovi-Harlig (2000) even has provided a very complete and comprehensive review of the previous studies on the learners interlanguage development of tense and aspect in second language acquisition. In the following sections of literature review, the focused aspect in this study the perfect will be reviewed first to get familiar with the characteristics in itself. After that, previous work on the acquisition of tense and aspect will be reviewed from the following three different dimensions: the form-meaning mapping in L2 acquisition, the relationship between grammatical and lexical aspect and the relationship between grammatical aspect and discourse organization.

9 2.1 THE PERFECT ASPECT IN ENGLISH The aspectual category of perfect is common among the world s languages. It is represented in English by distinct morphological marking and serves specific and unique functions. Reichenbach (1947) has already distinguished the two kinds of the perfect aspect as well as the simple present and the simple past by the concept of E, S and R in 1947. E refers to event time, S refers to speech time, and R refers to reference time. His representation of tense and aspect offered a clearer perspective on the internal makeups of simple present, simple past, present perfect and pluperfect and the meanings of the four tenses/aspects have the following semantics: (Reichenbach, 1947, p.290) (1) Simple present (2) Simple past S, R, E E, R S (3) Present perfect (4) Pluperfect E S, R E R S In the framework, the present perfect has R being simultaneous with S, while for the

10 pluperfect R precedes S. For the temporal reference to the past, simple past, present perfect and pluperfect are all relevant because their Es are located in the past. The difference between simple past and present perfect lies in the location of R: it is simultaneous with E in simple past while with S in present perfect. On the other hand, simple past and pluperfect differ from each other for R simultaneous with E in simple past but E precedes R in pluperfect. Due to the overlappings among the simple past, present perfect and pluperfect, learners may easily get confused and may have much more difficulties in learning the perfect aspect. They not only have to distinguish the three tenses/aspects, but also have to figure out how the perfect functions. As Giv?n (1993) has mentioned, Among the various aspects, the perfect, marked by the auxiliary have and the perfect suffixal form of the main verb, is functionally the most complex aspect in English. It involves a cluster of features, some more semantic, others more pragmatic (p.161). As the perfect aspect involves multiple meanings and functions, a number of previous studies concentrated on its usage and functions. Salkie (1989) describes that the perfect not only expresses the relevance to the present or the past but also signifies the relation and connection between the events involved, as exemplified in (5). (5) He died after he had been ill for a long time.

11 The pluperfect implies that the illness was relevant to the person s death. If simple past is used instead (He died after he was ill for a long time), the sentence becomes bizarre. The connection between the two events is cut off, as the speaker is not asserting any causal relation between them, which is quite strange. Klein (1992) also indicated and argued that there was an incompatibility of the present perfect and most past tense adverbials, such as *Chris has left New York yesterday. This shows that the perfect aspect indeed has its special constraint and uniqueness from other tenses/aspects referring to the past. 2.2 THE FORM-MEANING MAPPING IN L2 ACQUISITION To acquire a second language, an important task for learners is to match form and meaning. In order to observe how learners associate form and meaning, several studies investigate the relationships of them in the developing interlanguage. Learners of low proficiency usually mismatch form and meaning. However, even learners of higher proficiency level are observed to demonstrate inappropriate use of verb forms in their written compositions (Bardovi-Harlig & Bofman 1989). The different point lies in that the advanced learners make relatively few errors than the beginners in the formal accuracy. Later, in Bardovi-Harlig s (1992a) research, there was a more complete study

12 on this issue. She not only gave clear demonstrations of well-formedness and appropriate use, but also investigated the relationship of form and meaning by looking into the cloze passages and compositions of the learners from six different proficiency levels. She defined that the form of a response in the cloze passage was accurate when it was completely well-formed in its immediate syntactic frame (Bardovi-Harlig, 1992a, p.259). Besides, the meaning of a response was scored as being correct when it was an appropriate use in the cloze context. Whether it was appropriate or not was determined by the control group of 23 native speakers. She found that well-formedness preceded appropriate use in her 135 adult learners at six levels of proficiency from beginning to advanced. Learners from any one of the levels showed a much higher percentage of formal accuracy than appropriate use. That is, fully grammatical forms emerge and are used by learners before they carry targetlike meaning (Bardovi-Harlig, 1992a, p.261). Besides, learners seemed to have more difficulty with the aspect of the verbs than with the tense. Although Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) investigated general formal accuracy and appropriate use of tenses/aspects that appeared in the cloze passage and found learners seemed to have more difficulty with the aspect, she did not specifically go further with the learners use of the perfect aspect. In her later study (Bardovi-Harlig 1997), she described the order of emergence of the present perfect with respect to other tense

13 and aspect forms and the semantics of the emergent present perfect. The order of emergence of present perfect is in the much later stage of learners development of temporal references compared with other tense/aspect forms. Although the learners in her study showed mostly appropriate uses of present perfect in their production (86.9%), they differed from native speakers because they would overuse or underuse the present perfect. For example, due to the semantic overlap between past and present perfect, the learners were found to overuse present perfect in past contexts and past in present perfect contexts. Moreover, she also found that the pluperfect develops in the even later stage than the present perfect. However, she did not further investigate the learners use of the pluperfect. Housen (2002) also had some observations on the learners use of the perfect aspect. He collected oral interview data from 23 Dutch-speaking and 23 French-speaking pupils (n=46) from the English-L2 section of the European Schools in Brussels and Mol (Belgium). It is found that whenever a Perfect form occurs, it is nearly always used in a semantically appropriate way, i.e. in a past or perfect time context but not in a present or future tense context (Housen, 2002, p.160). This is in accordance to Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) that the learners showed a majority of appropriate uses of present perfect in their production. In addition, as Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) indicated, Housen (2002) also found that the learners would undergeneralize

14 and overgeneralize the perfect aspect in their production. He further found that his subjects tended to massively underuse the perfect but rarely overuse it. This similar phenomenon was observed by Robison (1995), too. 2.3 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN GRAMMATICAL AND LEXICAL ASPECT The relationship between grammatical aspect and lexical aspect in second language acquisition has been discussed in many previous studies (such as Bardovi-Harlig 1992a, Robison 1990 and Housen 2002). Grammatical aspect is conveyed morphologically to provide different ways of viewing situations whereas lexical aspect is part of the inherent semantics of the verb predicate. On the other hand, the lexical aspect of verbal predicates are classified into four situation types by Vendler (1967) 1, which has been widely applied by many researchers in their studies. The classification is based on the semantic constellation of verbs: dynamicity, telicity and durativity. The four types are STATES, ACTIVITIES, ACCOMPLISHMENTS and 1 This paper adopts Vendler s (1967) classification of situation types. Other classifications are not as appropriate as Vendler s. For example, Smith s (1997) classification includes another fifth type, Semelfactives, which have the features Dynamic, Atelic and Instantaneous. However, there is no need to further distinguish atelic events in detail because our focus is to examine if there is close relationship of the perfect aspect and telic events. Another classification is in Croft s (1998) study. He discussed amply that the adverbials may cause situation type shifts. Nevertheless, derived situation types (which include not only the verb but also the modifying adverbial) are not included and discussed in the present study. The second paragraph in this section (p. 12) describes the difference between basic and derived situation types.

15 ACHIEVEMENTS. Their individual constitutions by features are shown in Table 1. Table 1. Vendler s classification of lexical aspect Lexical Aspectual Categories Features STATES ACTIVITIES ACCOMPLISHMENTS ACHIEVEMENTS Dynamic Telic Durative The following are some examples of verbs typically associated with specific lexical aspectual categories: STATES (be, have, love), ACTIVITIES (walk, run, laugh), ACCOMPLISHMENTS (run a mile, paint a house, build a bridge) and ACHIEVEMENTS (reach the peak, break a stick, notice something). In this study, inherent lexical aspect is defined according to what Smith (1997) calls the basic situation types, which involve the internal temporal features of a verb constellation (a verb and its arguments). If a verb s situation type shifts to another because of the adverbials in the sentence or other information from context, the condition would be considered as Smith s (1997) derived situation types. Basic situation types and derived situation types belong to distinct levels. The following

16 examples illustrate the difference of the two levels. (5) Anny understands the rule. (6) Anny suddenly understands the rule. The sentences (5) and (6) have the same verb phrase understands the rule but (6) has an adverb suddenly to modify the verb. The situation types of the verb understand are totally different from each other. Sentence (5) involves the pure internal semantics of the verb and therefore the situation type is in basic level and is a STATE. However, sentence (6) involves a situation type shift triggered by the adverb. It changes from STATE to ACHIEVEMENT. Therefore, it is at the different level, the derived level. Since the situation type may be changed by adverbials or other information from context, we focus on the basic situation type. We want to examine the relationship between the pure inherent lexical aspect and the grammatical aspect, the perfect. Other inflected influential elements (such as temporal adverbials) should therefore be neglected for they may trigger different interpretation or situation type shifts. Many previous studies (such as Bardovi-Harlig 1992a, Robison 1990 & Housen 2002) in the area of language acquisition also discussed the relationship of lexical aspect and grammatical aspect on the basis of the basic situation types of

17 verbs. Lots of previous studies have pointed out that learners tend to make associations between grammatical aspect and lexical aspect. Because most of the previous studies concentrated on the system of the learners interlanguage of the past tense and progressive, a review of them shows that learners use of grammatical aspect (the past tense and progressive) will be influenced by lexical aspect of verbs. Moreover, these studies offer empirical evidence that there is a close relationship between them. The simple past tense tends to interact with more punctual verbs while the progressive tends to be with more durative verbs. Based on those empirical studies, there are two related theoretical hypotheses formulated in interlanguage research. The first one is the defective tense hypothesis (Andersen 1991) and the second is the primacy of aspect hypothesis (Robison 1990). The defective tense hypothesis states: In beginning stages of language acquisition only inherent aspectual [emphasis original] distinctions are encoded by verbal morphology, not tense or grammatical aspect (Andersen, 1991, p. 307). Andersen showed that children learning Spanish as a second language use the imperfect and preterite inflections to redundantly mark lexical aspect. Robison (1990) proposed the Primacy of aspect hypothesis, that he explained that aspect is primary in the sense not that morphemes that denote aspect in the target language are acquired first, but that target language verbal morphemes,

18 independent of their function in the target language are first used by the learner to mark aspect (Robison, 1990, p. 316). He showed that the learner use of ing tends to mark durative verbs and PAST/ en tends to mark punctual verbs. Prediction supported is that, although the exact pattern will vary depending on L1, L2, and individual differences between learners, verbal morphology correlates with lexical aspect at least during some stage in the development of interlangauge. Later in Bardovi-Harlig (1992a), she used the instrument of a cloze passage to test the learners use of tenses/aspects. By further scrutinizing the responses for the cloze, she found that learners tended to use simple past for more punctual verbs while progressive for more durative verbs. Although previous studies have pointed out the interactions between lexical aspect and the simple past tense and the progressive, little research discussed the influence of the lexical aspect on the use of the perfect aspect. Therefore, it remains unclear how learners utilize this aspect in their interlanguage or whether there is also a correlation between the perfect aspect and the inherent aspect of verbs. Nevertheless, some previous studies have implicated that there may be some interaction between the perfect aspect and the lexical aspect of verbs. The following four pieces of work are from the three different perspectives to indicate the relationship between the perfect and lexical aspect. The first one is from the investigation of the first language

19 acquisition. Youssef (1990) has done a study on the L1 acquisition of the perfect aspect. In this two-year longitudinal study, the use of perfect aspect by three Trinidadian children was investigated. It was found that the three subjects involved all applied the perfect aspect only to the Trinidad Creole verbs which characterize a result state, such as done, gone and finish. Next, the second one is from the findings of the second language acquisition of the perfect aspect. Kaplan s (1987) study investigated sixteen learners of French and indicated that the learners tended to use passé composé (which corresponds in English to the past or to the present perfect) with perfective events 2 (which correspond roughly to ACHIEVEMENTS and ACCOMPLISHMENTS). Learners seemed to also associate the characteristic of an end point with the perfect. Finally, from the perspective of the grammaticization of aspect markers in Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca (1994) and Li and Shirai (2000), they provided the development and change of the grammatical aspect. They claimed that completive and resultative aspect markers grammaticize into perfect markers. Completive markers denote an action performed completely and thoroughly, and resultative denotes a state that has been brought about by a prior action. Therefore, from this evolutional perspective of the markers of perfect aspect, it has an originally close relationship with the result state in the verb property. From the above four 2 Here the perfective events may refer to the events with end points, which are telic events.

20 pieces of literature (Youssef 1990, Kaplan 1987, Bybee, Perkins, and Pagliuca 1994 and Li and Shirai 2000), it can be inferred that there should be a close relationship between the perfect aspect and the situation types that indicate endpoints, telicity and boundary in learners interlanguage. 2.4 THE RELATIONSHIP OF GRAMMATICAL ASPECT AND DISCOURSE ORGANIZATION A number of studies on the acquisition of tense and aspect by learners of a second language point to the hypothesis that discourse organization influences the distribution of tense/aspect forms in interlanguage. In some of the previous work, they suggest that there is a relationship between the grounding of the discourse and the use of verbal morphology. The grounding includes the foreground and the background. The foreground relates events belonging to the skeletal structure of the discourse (Hopper 1979) and consists of clauses that move time forward (Dry 1981, 1983). And in contrast, the background elaborates or evaluates the events in the foreground (Hopper 1979). The main function of the background is to provide supportive material in the discourse. Earlier studies on the relationship between grammatical aspect and discourse structure were case studies. For example, Kumpf (1984) found that a Japanese learner of English used the base form of the verb to express completed

21 action in the foreground and used many morphologically marked verbs in the background. However, Flashner (1989) had different findings. It showed that three Russian learners of English marked foreground actions predominantly in simple past and left background portions in mainly base forms. Later, Bardovi-Harlig (1992b) investigated larger groups of learners. She found that 12 out of 16 intermediate learners showed a greater use of past in the foreground than in the background. It remains in wonder how advanced learners would be influenced by the grounding when they use tense/aspect. Other studies found that the perfect, especially the pluperfect, was mostly used in the background of the learners discourses. In Hopper (1979), he observed that the background may exhibit a variety of tenses such as pluperfect, future-perfect and future which do not occur in the foreground. Similar result was achieved in Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) which indicated that eight of her collected compositions showed the use of pluperfect as the function of scene-setting. As for the other perfect aspect the present perfect, it has not yet been investigated thoroughly. But from the results of the use of pluperfect in the background, we speculate that present perfect may also tend to appear in the background. And this inference needs further investigation and confirmation in the present study.

22 2.5 SUMMARY OF CHAPTER TWO In this chapter, we have reviewed the linguistic properties of the perfect aspect in English. Giv?n (1993) pointed out that the perfect is functionally the most complex aspect in English and it involves a cluster of semantic and pragmatic features. Salkie (1989) describes that the perfect expresses the relevance to the present or the past and also signifies the relation and connection between the events involved. In Section 2.2, learners associations of form and meaning in L2 are discussed. Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) found that fully grammatical forms emerge and are used by learners before they carry targetlike meaning (p.261). Furthermore, Bardovi-Harlig (1992a) and Housen (1002) both found that learners would overuse and underuse the perfect aspect in their production. Section 2.3 discusses the relationship between grammatical and lexical aspect. Previous research indicated that there seemed to be close connection between them. In learners use of tense/aspect, the simple past tense tends to interact with more punctual verbs while the progressive tends to be with more durative verbs. Other previous studies also imply that there seem to be some relationship between the perfect aspect and the verbs of a semantic endpoint. Finally, in Section 2.4, the relationship of grammatical aspect and discourse organization is discussed. Previous studies showed that learners tended to distinguish foreground and background by using either the base form of the verbs or the morphologically marked

23 verbs. Pluperfect seemed to appear in the background more. As for present perfect, it may be speculated that it tends to appear in the background, too. The speculation needs further examination in the present study.