Does Critical Period Play a Role in Second language acquisition? Indrani Chakravarty,Y0146

Similar documents
Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Language Acquisition Chart

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

THE ROBUSTNESS OF APTITUDE EFFECTS IN NEAR-NATIVE SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 146 ( 2014 )

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Spanish III Class Description

The Acquisition of English Grammatical Morphemes: A Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

English Language and Applied Linguistics. Module Descriptions 2017/18

University of Pittsburgh Department of Slavic Languages and Literatures. Russian 0015: Russian for Heritage Learners 2 MoWe 3:00PM - 4:15PM G13 CL

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

Stages of Literacy Ros Lugg

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

Exams: Accommodations Guidelines. English Language Learners

STAFF DEVELOPMENT in SPECIAL EDUCATION

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage?

Lower and Upper Secondary

Linguistics. Undergraduate. Departmental Honors. Graduate. Faculty. Linguistics 1

An Assessment of the Dual Language Acquisition Model. On Improving Student WASL Scores at. McClure Elementary School at Yakima, Washington.

Reviewed by Florina Erbeli

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

ACCREDITATION STANDARDS

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Guided Reading with A SPECIAL DAY written and illustrated by Anne Sibley O Brien

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

5/26/12. Adult L3 learners who are re- learning their L1: heritage speakers A growing trend in American colleges

Foreign Languages. Foreign Languages, General

Science Fair Project Handbook

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 12: 9 September 2012 ISSN

Copyright Corwin 2015

University of New Orleans

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Psychology and Language

A non-profit educational institution dedicated to making the world a better place to live

GCSE English Language 2012 An investigation into the outcomes for candidates in Wales

ANGLAIS LANGUE SECONDE

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Proficiency Illusion

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Handbook for Graduate Students in TESL and Applied Linguistics Programs

MYCIN. The MYCIN Task

NTU Student Dashboard

COMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Recommended Guidelines for the Diagnosis of Children with Learning Disabilities

Case study Norway case 1

ELP in whole-school use. Case study Norway. Anita Nyberg

NATIONAL CENTER FOR EDUCATION STATISTICS RESPONSE TO RECOMMENDATIONS OF THE NATIONAL ASSESSMENT GOVERNING BOARD AD HOC COMMITTEE ON.

ENGBG1 ENGBL1 Campus Linguistics. Meeting 2. Chapter 7 (Morphology) and chapter 9 (Syntax) Pia Sundqvist

A Guide to Adequate Yearly Progress Analyses in Nevada 2007 Nevada Department of Education

Word Stress and Intonation: Introduction

Acquisition vs. Learning of a Second Language: English Negation

EDUCATING TEACHERS FOR CULTURAL AND LINGUISTIC DIVERSITY: A MODEL FOR ALL TEACHERS

Generative Second Language Acquisition & Foreign Language Teaching Winter 2009

What the National Curriculum requires in reading at Y5 and Y6

The History of Language Teaching

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

CLINTON-MACOMB PUBLIC LIBRARY STRATEGIC PLAN

THE ACQUISITION OF INFLECTIONAL MORPHEMES: THE PRIORITY OF PLURAL S

Making Sales Calls. Watertown High School, Watertown, Massachusetts. 1 hour, 4 5 days per week

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

OUTLINE OF ACTIVITIES

Strategy Study on Primary School English Game Teaching

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion

FOREWORD.. 5 THE PROPER RUSSIAN PRONUNCIATION. 8. УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) УРОК (Unit) 4 80.

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

Unit: Human Impact Differentiated (Tiered) Task How Does Human Activity Impact Soil Erosion?

BASIC EDUCATION IN GHANA IN THE POST-REFORM PERIOD

Developing an Assessment Plan to Learn About Student Learning

Lab 1 - The Scientific Method

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

FREE COLLEGE Can Happen to You!

The College Board Redesigned SAT Grade 12

Cross-linguistic aspects in child L2 acquisition

Underlying and Surface Grammatical Relations in Greek consider

Unequal Opportunity in Environmental Education: Environmental Education Programs and Funding at Contra Costa Secondary Schools.

International Conference on Education and Educational Psychology (ICEEPSY 2012)

New Jersey Department of Education World Languages Model Program Application Guidance Document

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities.

Implementing cross-disciplinary learning environment benefits and challenges in engineering education

The phonological grammar is probabilistic: New evidence pitting abstract representation against analogy

Multimedia, Subtitles, and Native Language Vocabulary Acquisition

OPTIMIZATINON OF TRAINING SETS FOR HEBBIAN-LEARNING- BASED CLASSIFIERS

Modern Languages. Introduction. Degrees Offered

Transcription:

Does Critical Period Play a Role in Second language acquisition? Indrani Chakravarty,Y0146

Critical Period Hypothesis Notion of critical period in learning Maturational time period during which some crucial experience will have its peak effect on development or learning, resulting in normal behavior attuned to the particular environment to which the organism has been exposed. [5] Examples [1,5] The identification of a species member as an attachment object ( imprinting ) in ducks and birds. Professor Konrad being followed by a flock of geese.

Critical Period Hypothesis Contd. White crowned sparrows when brought up in isolation from a few days after birth would sing crude songs having some basic structures to the original whitecrowned sparrows songs. However, when trained using a tape recorder within a specific time period(10-50 days after birth), the sparrows even if brought up in isolation learnt to sing the original songs properly. But if they were exposed to the tape recorded songs after this specific time period, there was little or no effect at all in the quality of the songs sung. There is a concept of Critical Period among other species.

Critical Period in Language Acquisition Concept of critical period among human beings developed by analogy with the learning mechanisms in other species; proposed by Penfield and Roberts (1959). Eric Lenneberg [1,3,4,5] supported and hypothesized in his book Biological Foundations of Language(1967)- Language acquisition is a Biologically constrained learning Normally acquired during a critical period( early in life and puberty) Outside this period, language acquired through a different learning process or with difficulty.

Critical Period in Language Acquisition Contd. Evidence from[1,3,4,5]: Language processing of people with brain damage is dependent on the age of the person at the time of the accident or disease onset. The loss of language functions was irreversible past the age of puberty (clinical analysis by Lenneberg). Children with Down's Syndrome, whose language development is slower. Isolated children who have not been spoken to during the crucial period of childhood; referred to as "wild children".

Critical Period in Language Acquisition Contd. Lenneberg s citation[3,5]: An isolated 13 yrs old child Genie( isolated since she was a few months old). She was tethered to her bed and not spoken to at all. After rescue received intensive speech therapy. But, she was not able to construct grammatical sentences. Critical period had passed or the brain damage that resulted from the physical and mental abuse endured by Genie (??). Pinker s citation[3,5]: A woman known in the literature as "Chelsea" whose deafness was only discovered at the age of 31. She had not been abused unlike Genie. Language acquisition after hearing aids were fitted was quite similar to Genie's pattern of significant vocabulary acquisition but little grasp of syntax. Critical period during which acquisition of grammatical structure can occur had passed. But, why wasn t her deafness revealed before?

Evidence to the hypothesis Contd: Lenneberg s hypothesis was also supported by Language development of children with Down's Syndrome halted at puberty. Abused children isolated from exposure to first language showed deficits in phonology, morphology, and syntax. [1,5] General physical and cognitive status may be a concern for isolated and deficit children. For normal children and adults, it is not:[1,5] Adults during the first few months of learning have an advantage over children in case of higher acquisition of vocabulary and speed of learning, but people who start early produce lesser grammatical mistakes than those who start late. Flawless control over the accent and rhythm of language. Full productive control over syntax and morphology.

Evidence to the hypothesis Contd: With increasing ages of exposure, beginning from 4-6 yrs, there is a gradual decline in language proficiency until it plateaus for adult learners (Johnson and Newport,1989;Newport 1990)[5] Learners exposed to the language in adulthood show lower performance than those who are exposed in early childhood. [Johnson and Newport,1989] [5] Effects seen in degree of accent, morphology and syntax. Critical periods affect phonology, morphology and syntax not the vocabulary and semantic processing (occurs relatively normally in late learners).

Johnson and Newport s Study Sample consisted of native speakers of Korean and Chinese who had immigrated to the US at different ages. [1,3,4,5] subjects asked to make grammaticality judgments about 276 English sentences. Half the sentences were rendered ungrammatical by violating rules about articles, gender agreement and verb structures. The seven subjects who had arrived between ages 3 and 7 performed indistinguishably from native speakers of English. Strongly negative correlation between age of arrival (esp before 15) and ability to judge grammaticality, For the adult learners there was no significant correlation between age of arrival and grammaticality judgment ability.

Johnson and Newport s Study Contd. Johnson and Newport concluded that [1,3,4,5] A critical period does exist for the acquisition of grammar. Effects seen for both first and 2nd languages. They reported that prior to age 15, there was a very strong negative correlation with age but after age 15, there was no correlation with age Pinker supported this conclusion heavily and summed it up as [3] acquisition of a normal language is guaranteed for children up to the age of six, is steadily compromised from then until shortly after puberty, and is rare thereafter

Controversies Questions-- [5] Does the L1 acquisition affect the L2 acquisition? Does it reduce the effects of later stage learning? Late first language acquisition results in lower performance than does the 2nd language acquisition, regardless of signed or spoken languages. [Newport] Two challenging evidence The identification of older learners who achieve native- like competence in the second language (Birdsong, 1992) Behavioral evidence that fails to reveal a qualitative change in learning outcomes at the close of a critical period (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1999).

Two Hypothesis on L2 acquisition Does L2 acquisition recapitulate the L1 acquisition? [3] Is L2=L1 Hypothesis correct? If yes, critical period of L1 acquisition relevant to L2 acquisition. Is L2 acquisition a cumulative process that builds on the competence already developed in L1?[3] If yes, then critical period of L1 acquisition irrelevant to L2 acquisition.

Which one is correct? A native speaker of Spanish will acquire English more rapidly than would a native speaker of Chinese, all other things being equal, because of the linguistic similarity between Spanish and English. This evidence would imply that the cumulative model is correct. There is a remarkable similarity across speakers of different languages learning a given L2, indicating that there is much more than simple transfer from L1 to L2 going on (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994). and that indeed there is some sort of reenactment of the L1 acquisition process at work.

Lenneberg, the originator of the critical period hypothesis, said [3] we may assume that the cerebral organization for language learning as such has taken place during childhood, and since natural languages tend to resemble one another in many fundamental aspects, the matrix of language skills is present (p. 176). He favored the cumulative model. This implies critical period of L1 acquisition irrelevant to L2 acquisition according to the cumulative model hypothesis.

Characteristics of a Critical Period A critical period must have two characteristics (Bornstein, 1989; Columbo, 1982). [5] high level of preparedness for learning within a specified developmental period to assure the domain is mastered by the species, and lack of preparedness outside of this period. Condition 1 [3]: There should be clearly specified beginning and end points for the period. Lenneberg suggested puberty, Johnson and Newport suggested age 15, Pinker suggested between age 6 and end of puberty. True if specific end points are there.

Key elements of Critical Period Condition 2 [3]: There should be a well-defined decline in L2 acquisition at the end of the period, not a monotonic decline with age. True if a rapid decline could be found around the end of the critical period, rather than a general monotonic decline with age that continues throughout the life span. Condition 3 [3]: There should be evidence of qualitative differences in learning between acquisition within and outside the critical period. in the patterns of acquisition between child and adult second language learners. True if certain grammatical errors could be found among adult learners, that are never found in child learners, or if child learners were able to learn specific aspects of the language that no adults could learn.

Key elements Contd. Condition 4[3]: There should be a robustness to environmental variation inside the critical period. True if within the critical period, even with considerable environmental variation, the outcomes are uniform. Beyond that period, the environment might play a larger role, and therefore the outcomes would become more variable. Johnson's and Newport s conclusion was A critical period does exist for the acquisition of grammar. Effects seen for both first and 2nd languages. Age 15 was the end point of the critical period.

Theoretical Predictions[3]

Re-analysis of Johnson s and Newport s Study by Bialystok and Hakuta in 1994[3,4] Bialystok and Hakuta claimed the breakpoint to be at age 20 and not at 15 or puberty. the data showed a discontinuity not at puberty but rather at 20. and that there was statistically significant evidence for a continued decline in L2 acquisition well into adulthood.

Re-analysis of Johnson s and Newport s Study Contd.[3,4] From the 1990 Census Analysis of the same by Bialystok and Hakuta in 1999, the following two graphs were obtained which contradicted their previous claim.

How Conditions 1 and 2 fail[3] Conditions 1 and 2: End Point for the Critical Period and Discontinuity at that Point The study looked at Chinese and Spanish speaking immigrants, who had immigrated to the U. S. at ages ranging from just after birth up to 70 years old. The Census Bureau asked for a self-report of their English ability, which was converted to a 4-point scale. continuous decline with age, and no evidence of a discontinuity or sharp break at puberty, as would be expected by Conditions 1 and 2. Essentially a straight line, and no evidence of conditions 1 and 2: There should be clearly specified beginning and end points for the period. There should be a well-defined decline in L2 acquisition at the end of the period, not a monotonic decline with age.

How Condition 3 fails[3] Condition 3:There should be evidence of qualitative differences in learning between acquisition within and outside the critical period. in the patterns of acquisition between child and adult second language learners. True if certain grammatical errors could be found among adult learners, that are never found in child learners, or if child learners were able to learn specific aspects of the language that no adults could learn. Supported by Language Transfer view : Extent of Influence by the native language on the second language acquisition. The points of contrast between the native language and the target language can determine the course of learning. positive transfer happened where the two languages are similar, and negative transfer where they are different.

Condition 3 Contd. Examples Japanese people having difficulty with the English determiner system (e.g., a, the, some) Spanish language has a similar determiner system. adult learners show more evidence of transfer errors than do children because, according to the critical period hypothesis, children directly access the target language whereas adults must go through their native language. Not correct.

Condition 3 Contd.[2,3] White and Genesee s test (1996): They provided adults who had learnt english at various stages of life with some grammatically correct and incorrect sentences along with some abstract concepts. Sentences Who do you want to see?(1) Who do you want to feed the dog?(2) Who do you wanna see?(3) *Who do you wanna feed the dog? (4) (incorrect sentence) The underlying structure for the sentences can be hypothesized as: You want to see who?(5) You want who to feed the dog?(6)

Condition 3 Contd. Abstract concepts provided According to the theoretical model of Universal Grammar, these underlying forms of who are moved to the front of the sentence, leaving behind a trace, t in the original location: Who i do you want to see t i?(7) Who i do you want t i to feed the dog?(8) The rule that reduces want to to wanna for (8) is blocked by the trace between want and to. Makes the sentence Who do you wanna feed the dog? (4) to be incorrect.

White and Genesee s test results (Condition 3) Although more adult learners had difficulty in distinguishing between with these sentences than did child learners, about one-third of the adults had acquired these rules showed equivalently high performance to child learners and native speakers of English. Adults are capable of learning even these highly abstract rules that theory would say are accessible only with specialized language acquisition mechanisms. no demonstrated differences between the process of second language acquisition in child and adults.

How Condition 4 fails:[3] Condition 4: There should be a robustness to environmental variation inside the critical period. True if within the critical period, even with considerable environmental variation, the outcomes are uniform. Beyond that period, the environment might play a larger role, and therefore the outcomes would become more variable. An important variant in the environment is socioeconomic status of the learner. Experiment on oral proficiency data for immigrant students from a school district in Northern California, varying by the socioeconomic environment of the school. data show students who are socio-economically poorer schools (>50% free lunch) to be attaining English proficiency at a rate of about a full year slower than those in less poor schools.

The Effects of Environmental Variation [3]

Condition 4 Contd. Strong socioeconomic effects can be found in the Census data as well. Figure shows the same data as the previous one, but disaggregated by years of education attained as a proxy for socioeconomic status. Enormous effects for years of education. No indication for whether it works differently in child and adult learners.

Census Data:[3]

Conclusion: No definable end point (condition 1 fails). If there had been a critical period, points outside this period should have Either a drop in English proficiency, Or a change in slope between age of immigration and English proficiency, Or both a mean drop and a slope change. No well defined decline found. All studies show a monotonic decline in second language acquisition.( Condition 2 fails) No demonstrated qualitative differences between the process of second language acquisition in child and adults. ( Condition 3 fails). No robustness to environmental variation inside the critical period. ( Condition 4 fails).

No evidence of Critical Period in second language acquisition. This implies, the view of a biologically constrained and specialized language acquisition device that is turned off at puberty is not correct for second language acquisition. The gradual decline over age can be attributed to multiple factors at work physiological, cognitive, and social. Reduction in cognitive resources (working- memory limitations, cognitive slowing, or attentional deficits), mean that older learners will find 2 nd language acquisition cognitively difficult than the young learners. It can be inferred that Critical Period may exist for the first language acquisition But, there is no evidence of it playing a role in second language acquisition.

References: 1. http://pages.slc.edu/~ebj/im_97/lecture16/l16.html 2. White, L. & Genesee, F. (1996). How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12, 233-265. 3. DRAFT 9/7/99, A Critical Period for Second Language Acquisition? A Status Review, By Kenji Hakuta, Stanford University 4. CRITICAL PERIOD IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION, Critical Evidence: A Test of the Critical Period Hypothesis for Second Language Acquisition Kenji Hakuta, Stanford University, Ellen Bialystok, York University, and Edward Wiley, Stanford University 5. Language Development, Critical Periods in, EL Newport, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, USA.