Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading: How Much Can Non-Native Readers of English Be Expected to Develop their Reading Fluency?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading: How Much Can Non-Native Readers of English Be Expected to Develop their Reading Fluency?"

Transcription

1 103 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading: How Much Can Non-Native Readers of English Be Expected to Develop their Reading Fluency? Timothy Gutierrez Introduction The pedagogical technique of repeated-reading has a well-developed history of being used to develop reading fluency in native speakers of English. Concurrently developed by both Chomsky (1976) as well as Samuels (1979), having students repeatedly reading a text aloud has been shown to develop reading fluency in native speakers. Typically, a native English speaking developing reader who engages in repeated-reading will read a text which is at the reader s grade level repeatedly to a specified number of repetitions or until the reader can read the text to a criterion of a desired reading speed. Research into repeated-reading with native speaking children has shown that repeatedly reading a text aloud can improve native speaking children s reading fluency and reading comprehension (Schwanenflugel & Knapp, 2016, p ). Adapted to developing reading fluency in non-native readers of English, an English learner will read the same text, typically four times, but potentially up to seven times, in a single session. Nation (2009) describes that repeated-reading fits the requirements for a fluency development activity if (a) the students are reading for the meaning contained in the text, (b) the material the students are reading is within the range of control of the students, (c) the student is prompted to go faster while reading, and (d) there is a sufficiently large amount of reading. Repeated-reading fits into Nations (2007) four strands framework for designing a language curriculum for non-native speakers which recommends that about one-fourth of instructional time in a second language curriculum be devoted to fluency development activity. For example, in a reading class, students should be engaged in activities where reading fluency is developed about one-fourth of the time. In order for a teacher or an administrator to make the decision to include repeated-reading in an English as a foreign or second-language program, it is necessary to know what the likely outcomes of engaging in silent repeatedreading will be. The purpose of this paper is to inform such teachers and administrators as to the potential benefits of implementing silent repeated-reading with their students of English as a foreign language.

2 104 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading To fulfill the purposes of the paper, I review the literature investigating the effects of the pedagogical technique of silent repeated-reading on non-native readers of English. I discuss the findings of studies which investigated the effects of silent repeated-reading on non-native readers of English. The research has looked at several primary research questions. The first question asks how much faster can a native speaker read a text when reading the text repeatedly. The second question asks how much faster do readers read a new passage as they progress along a treatment period of repeated-reading. The third question asks can improvements in reading rate be transferred to texts which are different from the texts which are being used to read in the repeated-reading sessions. The fourth question asks if readers can improve their reading comprehension through engaging in a treatment of repeatedreading. The fifth question asks how repeated-reading improves students reading fluency. Methods I searched for studies to review in the following manner. I used the search term repeated reading on both Google Scholar and the library search engine from Temple University to find studies on using repeated-reading in the English language classroom. The library search engine amalgamated a search of 34 academic databases of research into education and psychology. From the studies I found, I searched the previously mentioned databases for studies from the authors who had authored papers investigating repeated-reading. Following these searches, I manually checked the references list of the papers I found for further studies. The purpose of this study was to review the effects of silent repeated-reading on reading fluency. As such, studies which investigated reading aloud were not included. In addition, this study focused on the results of English language learners. As such, studies which investigated improvements in languages other than English were not included. Seven studies were found which fit the criteria. While most studies of repeated-reading on non-native learners of English focus on multiple aspects of reading ability, for example the effect of engaging in repeated-reading on reading rate and reading comprehension, along with the learners perspectives on the repeated-reading activity. In this review, I analyze each aspect investigated under separate research questions. To facilitate comparison of the effects of repeated reading, each study is reviewed under multiple categories if it investigated multiple aspects of reading fluency. The purpose of this analysis strategy is to highlight just how much and by what qualities an aspect of reading fluency is developed through repeated reading.

3 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 105 Results RQ1: Can students read a text faster upon each repetition? The first research question investigated how much students reading changed during the procedure of repeated-reading itself. In this section, I review the four studies that explicitly investigated this phenomenon and reanalyze the data reported from one study to determine if students read faster during the repeated-reading treatment. In all cases, students read faster a text faster on their fifth repetition than they read on the first repetition. Where it was investigated, students also read the text faster on the eight repetition when compared to the first as well as the fifth repetition. The 16 Japanese university students investigated by Taguchi (1997) statistically significantly increased their silent reading rates on the first reading passage from the first repetition ( words per minute) to the fifth repetition ( words per minute), as well as from the fifth repetition to the seventh repetition ( words per minute). On the final session, the 28th session, the students also statistically significantly increased their silent reading rate from the first repetition ( words per minute) to the fifth repetition ( words per minute), and from the fifth repetition to the seventh repetition ( words per minute). While Taguchi and Gorsuch (2002) did not specifically analyze the average increases in the speed by which the 9 Japanese students in their study read the treatment passages as an average, one can compute from the data given that the students read the 28 passages an average of words per minute on the initial reading of a treatment text, read the same passage at words per minute on the fifth repetition, and at words per minute on the seventh repetition. 10 Japanese students with slow initial reading rates statistically significant improved their silent reading rate as an average of their reading rates across all the treatment sessions (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass & Gorsuch, 2004). Students read the treatment texts on average words per minute on the first reading and read the treatment texts on average on the fifth repetition of reading the text. The average increase of words per minute was significant statistically. A single Japanese advanced proficiency student of English increased her reading speed words per minute on the fifth repetition of the text with the initial average reading being at words per minute and the average fifth repetition being at words per minute (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass & Snipp, 2012). Likewise, her average sixth repetition was read at words per minute which was 9.22 words per minute faster than the fifth repetition and words per minute faster than the average initial reading.

4 106 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 24 Vietnamese learners of English statistically significantly improved their silent reading rate within the treatment sessions with the students reading on the texts initially at an average rate of words per minute and on the fifth repetition of the text were reading at words per minute (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008). This statistically significant gain of words per minute was larger than the gains made by the Japanese students investigated previously. In most cases, students who engage in silent repeated-reading increase their reading speed by approximately 40 words per minute on the fifth time they read a text as compared with the first time they read the same text. Vietnamese students (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) who initially read the texts faster than the Japanese students studies (Taguchi, 1997; Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002; Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass & Snipp, 2012; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass & Gorsuch, 2004), made much larger reading rate gains in reading a text on the fifth repetition than the Japanese students studied. RQ2: Can students read the treatment text faster after engaging in repeated-reading? A second finding from research into the effects of repeated-reading with non-native readers is that students while reading a text during a sustained repeated-reading program can read the treatment texts progressively faster as the repeated-reading program progresses. In this section, I review the six studies which investigated how much faster a non-native reader of English reads subsequent texts while engaging in a program of repeated-reading. As with the finding that students can read a text faster the more times they read the text, the finding that students read treatment texts faster at the end of a period of engaging in repeated-reading is replicated throughout the studies. A number of investigations which have shown that repeated-reading silently can produce benefits in improving silent reading rate. Taguchi (1997) found that 16 Japanese university students could read the passage used in the 28 session faster than students could read the passage used in the first session. When comparing the initial reading of the passage, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. Similarly, when comparing the fifth repetition of reading the passages, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of 29.8 words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. Likewise, when comparing the seventh repetition of reading the passages, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. Even though the gains made by the students in silent

5 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 107 reading rate were not statistically significant, this study showed that assisting student s reading by concurrently listening to an oral reading of the passage could increase student s silent reading rate across sessions. The results finding that Japanese students could read English texts faster after engaging in a treatment of repeated-reading was first replicated with 8 Japanese university students by Taguchi and Gorsuch (2002). The students made similar increases in reading rate to Taguchi s (1997) previous study. When comparing the initial reading of the passage, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. Similarly, when comparing the fifth repetition of reading the passages, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. Likewise, when comparing the seventh repetition of reading the passages, students read the final passage at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from their reading rate of words per minute when reading the passage used in the first treatment session. The authors (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004) replicated the improvements in silent reading rate with 10 lower proficiency Japanese university students who engaged in repeated-reading for 42 sessions. These lower proficiency students statistically significantly improved their silent reading rate when reading the treatment passages. The silent reading rate, word per minute, from the final four sessions averaged together (sessions 39, 40, 41, and 42) showed that the students could read words per minute faster at the end of the repeated reading treatment period than they could at the beginning as computed by averaging the students reading rate on the first four sessions (83.97 words per minute). Gorsuch and Taguchi (2008) replicated their previous findings of the effectiveness of engaging in repeated-reading in increasing rates of reading English in Japanese students with 24 3rd year Vietnamese university students. The Vietnamese participants statistically significantly increased their reading rate both within treatment sessions as well as when comparing their speed of reading the final text of the treatment period with the speed of reading the first text of the treatment period. The students read the 16th treatment passage initially at words per minute which was words per minute faster than the words per minute by which they read the first treatment passage. Likewise, for the fifth repetition, which the students read the 16th treatment passage at words per minute which was words per minute faster than the words per minute by which they read the first treatment passage.

6 108 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading A Japanese advanced proficiency non-native reader of English also increased her reading rate through 14 weeks (i.e., 69 sessions) of self-administered repeated reading (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012). When comparing her initial reading of the passage, she read the final passages (i.e. the average of the final five sessions) at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute from her average reading rate of words per minute when reading the passages used in the first five treatment sessions. 26 Taiwanese college students increased their reading speed through 26 sessions (over 13 weeks) of repeated-reading (Chang & Millett, 2013). When comparing the initial reading of the passage, students read the final two passage at an average of 150 words per minute which was a gain of 47 words per minute from their average reading rate of 103 words per minute when reading the passages used in the second and third treatment sessions. Improvements in reading rate were made by students in all studies on the initial reading of the final treatment text. Two of the studies (Chang & Millett, 2013; Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) showed that students who engaging in repeated-reading improved their reading rate on the treatment texts to a statistically significant degree as compared to a comparison group, although students in one study (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002) improved their reading rates without statistically significantly outperforming the comparison group. The other studies either did not show statistically significant gains (Taguchi, 1997; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass & Gorsuch, 2004) or the reading rate could not be compared to others as it was a diary study (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass & Snipp, 2012). RQ3: Can students read a new text faster after the repeated reading treatment? A third finding from research into the effects of repeated-reading with non-native readers is that students while reading a text during a sustained repeated-reading program have seldom been able to read a text different from the treatment text at an increased reading rate which the students have been able to read the treatment text. In this section, I review the five studies investigating how much faster a non-native reader of English is likely to be able to read a text which is different from the texts which were used while engaging in a program of repeatedreading. In contrast to the previous two findings, the finding that students can read a text different from the texts used to implement the repeated-reading treatments at a faster rate than a comparison group is only demonstrated in one study. An initial investigation into reading rate changes on different texts was conducted by Taguchi and Gorsuch (2002). Before and after a 28-session treatment period of repeated reading, the 9 first-year Japanese university students in the study were timed on the initial reading of a standardized reading passage. These students who engaged in repeated-reading during the

7 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 109 treatment sessions statistically significantly increased their reading rate to words per minute on the posttest which was an increase of words per minute when compared to words per minute, which was the speed by which they initially read an equivalent passage as a pretest. However, these gains were not significantly different from reading rate gains (10.49 words per minute) made by a control group of 9 equivalent proficiency first-year Japanese university students doing intensive reading for the 28 sessions (pretest = words per minute; posttest = words per minute). Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, and Gorsuch (2004) again investigated reading rate changes on a new text. Before and after a 42-session treatment period of repeated reading, the 10 first-year Japanese university students in the study were timed on the initial reading of a standardized reading passages designed for native speaking fourth grade students. These students who engaged in repeated-reading during the treatment sessions read the posttest at the slightly slower rate of words per minute than the rate, words per minute by which they read the pretest passage. These rates showed no statistically significant difference from each other, nor was there any statistically significant difference from the rates by which 10 students of equivalent proficiency who engaged in extensive reading during the treatment period read the testing passages (pretest = words per minute; posttest = words per minute). Although, the slower rate by which all the students read the posttest was most likely a related to an inequivalancy between the test form for non-native readers of English. The silent reading speed of 24 3rd year Vietnamese university students of Gorsuch and Taguchi s study (2008) was measured in two passages developed to test reading comprehension changes through the 16 sessions (11 weeks) of repeated-reading. The authors investigated the students silent reading rate changes on two sets of texts which were designed to test students changes in reading comprehension. These texts also served as tests of reading rate changes on different texts due to the 16 sessions of repeated-reading. The first type of non-treatment text for which students were tested on was derived from an essay written for young native speakers of English. On this text, the students who engaged in repeated-reading during the treatment sessions decreased their initial reading rate to words per minute on the posttest designed as a short-answer test which was a decrease of 17.2 words per minute when compared to words per minute, which was the speed by which they initially read an equivalent passage as a pretest. Although the rate by which the experimental group read the pretest passage initially was statistically significantly faster than the rate by which the control group read the passage initially (pretest = words per minute), there was no statistical difference between the rate by which the control group read the posttest passage initially (i.e words per minute).

8 110 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading The second type of non-treatment text for which students were tested on was adapted from a graded reader and was used to test the ability of the students to recall the contents of the story. These students who engaged in repeated-reading during the treatment sessions increased their initial reading rate to words per minute on the posttest designed as a recall test which was an increase of words per minute when compared to words per minute, which was the speed by which they initially read an equivalent passage as a pretest. These results were also not statistically different from the rates by which a control group read the same passages (posttest = words per minute; pretest = words per minute). The advanced proficiency learner in Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, and Snipp s (2012) study increased her posttest reading rate after 14 weeks (i.e., 69 sessions) of self-administered repeated reading on a short story divided into two sections used as a pretest and a posttest which was primarily used to test reading comprehension. She read the posttest passage initially at words per minute which was a gain of words per minute when compared to the words per minute rate at which she read the pretest passage initially. Her reading performance on the test passages were close, but less than her reading performance on the first and last five passages averaged together (posttest = words per minute; pretest = words per minute; gain = words per minute). Despite the inconclusive results of the previous studies, one study showed positive results of students being able to read a passage different from the treatment passage at an increased rate comparable to the rate increases on the treatment passages used in repeated-reading. The 26 Taiwanese college students investigated by Chang and Millett (2013) improved their reading rate on narrative passages after 26 sessions (over 13-weeks) of silent repeated-reading of expository texts. They read the posttest passage at 145 words per minute which was a gain of 45 words per minute when compared to the 100 words per minute rate at which they read the pretest passage. These gains were comparable to the gain of 47 words per minute by which the students were able to improve their reading of the expository treatment passages (pretest = 150 words per minute; pretest = 103 words per minute). Clear improvements in reading rate on new texts above a comparison group were only made by students in one study (Chang & Millett, 2013). Students in two studies (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002 and the recall tests of Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) improved their reading rates without statistically significantly outperforming the comparison group. The other studies showed either non-significant changes (the short answer tests of Gorsuch Taguchi, 2008) or decreases in reading rate on new texts (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass & Gorsuch, 2004). It should be noted that students read the new texts used as a posttest at a slower rate than the treatment texts used as posttests in all cases.

9 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 111 RQ4: How does reading comprehension improve due to silent repeated-reading? A fourth finding from research into the effects of repeated-reading with non-native readers is that reading comprehension can be improved through engaging in silent repeated-reading for a treatment period. In this section, I review the results from five studies which investigated changes in reading comprehension made by students of English. As with the previous three findings, the findings that students can improve their comprehension through engaging in repeated reading is replicated through the studies. However, gains in reading comprehension made through engaging in repeated-reading are comparable to gains in reading comprehension from students who engage in another form of reading instruction. As measured in correct or partially correct responses to close-ended questions designed to test if a reader had understood the passage, the 28 students who engaged in 28-sessions of silent repeated reading scored progressively higher on a new text designed to investigate students silent reading fluency (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002). These students scored 9.33 out of 20 after reading the posttest passage once, after reading the passage three times, and after reading the passage seven times. The students score on the comprehension test after reading the posttest passage once was statistically significantly higher than the students comprehension score after reading the pretest passage once. Also, the students scores on the comprehension tests after reading the pretest and posttest passages seven times were statistically significantly higher than the students score after reading the passage only once. However, these scores where not statistically significantly different from the scores of the comparison group which engaged in intensive reading for the treatment period. Similar reading comprehension results on 8 open-ended comprehension questions for a total of 16 points were obtained by 10 students low proficiency Japanese students of English who engaged in 42 sessions of silent repeated-reading (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004). These students scored 3.90 after reading the posttest passage once, 8.40 after reading the passage three times, and 8.80 after reading the passage five times. As with Taguchi and Gorsuch (2002), both groups statistically significantly increased their reading comprehension from the first reading to the fifth reading in each test. However, there were no statistically significant gains in the posttest comprehension test made by the students who engaged in repeated-reading when their test results were compared to the results of the pretest comprehension test. In addition, the group who engaged in repeated reading did not differ in reading comprehension from the 10 comparable Japanese students who engaged in extensive reading for the same amount of sessions. More promising results were obtained in the following studies. 24 3rd year Vietnamese university students who engaged in 16 sessions (11 weeks) of silent repeated-reading

10 112 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading demonstrated higher levels of reading comprehension on a test measuring short answer recall and a test measuring proposition recall (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008). To measure the students ability to answer short questions about a reading passage, the authors asked the students to answer eight short answer questions after reading a passage that was similar to the repeated reading treatment passages. The students who engaged in repeated reading answered 41.2% of the propositions correctly after reading the posttest passage once and answered 58.2% of the propositions correctly after reading the posttest passage five times. These results where statistically significantly higher than the students results on the pretest. The students also answered statistically significantly more propositions on the short answer test than the students who participated in the control group. The authors also investigated recall of story propositions as reading comprehension. The students who engaged in repeated-reading recalled 26.1% of the story s propositions after reading the posttest passage once and recalled 61.5% of the story s propositions after reading the posttest passage five times. As with the short answer test, the students who engaged in repeated-reading recalled statistically significantly more of the posttest passage after the repeated reading treatment than they recalled on the pretest passage. Likewise, they recalled statistically significantly more propositions than the control group recalled on the posttest passage. The advanced English proficiency Japanese adult woman who engaged in 14 weeks (i.e., 69 sessions) of self-administered repeated reading also successfully increased her reading comprehension (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012). Similar to the reading rate results reported earlier, the participant s reading comprehension increased across testing sessions (i.e. from pretest to posttest). By answering 5 open-ended questions written to test the participants understanding of the events of the story, the participant increased her reading comprehension score from 4 after the first reading of the pretest passage to 8 after the first reading on the posttest passage. Contrasting to expectations, she received a full comprehension score of 15 after reading the passage twice on the pretest, but she needed to read the posttest passage three times before receiving an equivalent score on the posttest. 26 Taiwanese college students after a 13-week (i.e. 26 sessions) period of engaging in silent repeated reading treatment period clearly maintained their reading comprehension as measured by the two types of texts used to test the students silent reading speed. (Chang & Millett, 2013). The authors investigated changes in comprehension of two types of reading passages. The first type of reading passages were expository passages which were comparable to the treatment passages. After the treatment, students who had engaged in repeated reading answered 70% of the multiple-choice questions about two posttest passages which was

11 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 113 statistically significantly higher than their scores on comparable pretest passages. The second type of reading passages used in the study were narrative passages developed from a graded reader. The students also answered 66% of the multiple-choice questions about a story with similar vocabulary composition to the treatment passage, which was statistically significantly higher than their score on the questions after reading the same story before the treatment sessions. The comprehension scores of the treatment group on both posttests were statistically significantly higher than the scores of the group which engaged in a treatment consisting of timed, single readings of the treatment passages. The results of these studies support the notion that students who engage in silent repeatedreading increase their ability to comprehend similar texts after the period of engaging in repeated-reading. The early studies (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004) showed that students comprehended only a small portion of the text on the initial reading of the text. The studies with carefully controlled tests of comprehension (Chang & Millett, 2013; Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) showed students can improve their comprehension more than a comparison group. RQ5: How does Repeated-Reading develop reading fluency? A fifth finding from research into the effects of repeated-reading with non-native readers is that non-native readers of English who have engaged in silent repeated-reading have perceived that repeated-reading has improved their reading fluency. In this section, I review the results from three studies which investigated how the learners who engaged in repeated-reading perceived the repeated-reading treatment. Consist through these studies are student perceptions that repetitions assist with fluent reading of the treatment texts, audio renditions of the texts assist students in integrate verbal language with text, and the combined processes of repetition along with the audio assisted vocabulary recognition and boost reading comprehension. Analysis of a post treatment questionnaire answered by Japanese students who engaged in 42-sessions of repeated-reading (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004) found that students perceived that the repetition in repeated reading had a positive role in developing their reading fluency and comprehension. Students also perceived that the concurrent audio version of the text assisted the development of their listening skills. In addition, repeated reading helped the students develop a tolerance for reading longer texts and cope more efficiently with unknown words. Likewise, the perceptions of an advanced English proficiency Japanese adult woman who engaged in 69 sessions of self-administered repeated-reading were analyzed through diary

12 114 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading entries following each repeated-reading session (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012) supported the assertion that engaging in repeated-reading improves a non-native speaker s English reading fluency. The participant reported that repeated reading assisted her comprehension of the text at both the discourse level and the sentence level as well as assisted her comprehension of both grammar and vocabulary used in the passages. However, the participant reported the repetition of reading could be overdone and lead to decreased motivation and could not overcome the hindering effect of a critical mass of unknown words in the text. The participant reported that the audio model helped her develop fluency by making her keep up the pace of reading the passage and assisted her comprehension of the text by helping her comprehend dialogs and recognize the pronunciation of words. However, the audio did not help her overcome unknown features of the text. A descriptive and exploratory analysis of the end of session reports written by 30 Vietnamese college students who engaged in 16-sessions of repeated-reading (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2010) found five reading behaviors which engaging in repeated-reading helped to develop. Firstly, the students reported a wide use of reading strategies while engaging in repeated reading with topdown and meta-cognitive reading strategies being reported widely in the beginning sessions and top-down strategies being mentioned more widely during the later sessions. Secondly, the repeated reading method motivated the students to read more intently and widely as well as apply the repeated reading method outside of class. Thirdly, students mentioned consistently, but not widely, perceived improvements in reading speed and comprehension, with some mention of the repeated reading treatment as causing the improvement. Fourthly, the participants perceived that the repeated-reading treatment assisted the participants with a variety of language skills other than reading, especially the audio model to which the students listened to while reading. Finally, the participants mentioned to a limited degree that word recognition became easier and the students could focus on higher level features of the texts as the treatment period progressed. Three studies found the perceptions of students who engaged in repeated-reading to support the idea that silent repeated-reading with an audio model increased the students ability to read faster and comprehend more easily. Learner perceptions of engaging in repeated-reading supports the assertion that repeated-reading improves a learners reading fluency, reading comprehension, and ability to implement reading strategies. There is also support that learners move away from lexical and grammatical concerns to a focus on sentence and discourse level meaning, as predicted by Automaticity Theory (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974).

13 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 115 Discussion The primary goal of this study was to specify how silent repeated-reading could improve nonnative students English reading fluency by reviewing the literature which investigated reading fluency gains from English language learners who engaged in repeated reading. The studies investigated five aspects of reading improvements through repeated reading. Researchers are interested (a) in how much students read faster while repeatedly reading a text; (b) in how much faster students can read treatment texts at the end of a period of repeated-reading; (c) in how much faster students can read new texts at the end of a period of repeated-reading; (d) in how much readers can improve their reading comprehension at the end of a period of repeatedreading; and (e) the processes by which repeated-reading improves readers reading fluency. RQ1: Can students read a text faster upon each repetition? The first research question asked if non-native speaking students of English read a single text faster as they repeatedly read a text designed for repeated-reading. A clear finding from research into the effects of repeated-reading with non-native speakers is that students can read a text faster, the more times they read the text. This finding is clearly replicated in a series of studies by Etsuo Taguchi and his colleagues. For the most part, Japanese students improve their reading by approximately 40 words per minute on a fifth repetition of a text studies (Taguchi, 1997; Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002; Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass & Snipp, 2012; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass & Gorsuch, 2004), but Vietnamese students (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) were able to improve their reading by words per minute. The results discussed above should take into consideration some benchmarks for a good silent reading speed. A good careful silent reading speed is around 250 words per minute (Nation, 2009, p ). As a comparison, native speaking readers read for enjoyment (i.e. Rauding) at 300 words per minute, read for learning at 200 words for minute, and read for memorizing at 138 words per minute (Carver, 1997). As a further comparison, third-year Chinese university students of English read for enjoyment at 185 words per minute, read for learning at 140 words per minute, and read for memorization at 97 words per minute (Fraser, 2007). The obvious standout for increasing their reading rate in the process of repeated-reading are the Vietnamese learners of Gorsuch and Taguchi (2008). By the fifth repetition, they were reading near the rate of native speakers reading for enjoyment. While the Japanese students investigated in the other studies made more modest increases in reading rate on repeated reading of the texts, they were still able to increase their reading rates to a higher rate on

14 116 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading repeated readings of a text. This finding was also replicated in reading Japanese with beginning learners of Japanese. These results suggest that by engaging in silent repeated reading, nonnative readers can experience reading for enjoyment on subsequent readings of a text, even though they might read at a rate for learning or memorization on an initial reading of a text. As observed by Taguchi (1997), the apparent ability for non-native readers to increase their rate of reading a text (even up to increases in the eight repetition) supports the assertion that repeated-reading can assist non-native readers in reading English more fluently. RQ2: Can students read the treatment text faster after engaging in repeated-reading? The second research question asking if students can read a treatment text faster at the end of a period of engaging in repeated-reading can be answered that students do read the treatment text faster at the end of the treatment period when compared to the speed by which the students read treatment texts at the beginning of the treatment period. When considering the speed by which students read a passage for the first time in programs where students made substantial improvements in reading speed, students made improvements of 55 words per minute (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008), 47 words per minute (Chang & Millett, 2013), and 38 words per minute (Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012). These positive results can generally be attributed to advanced learners and texts carefully controlled for vocabulary and grammar. When considering gains made in programs where students made less impressive gains on the initial reading of treatment passages, students made improvements of 23 words per minute (Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004), 21 words per minute (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002), and 20 words per minute (Taguchi, 1997). For initial reading of a treatment passage, more advanced students can be expected to increase their reading rate by about 35 to 50 words per minute through a program of repeated reading. Less proficient students can be can be expected to increase their reading rate by around 20 words per minute through a program of repeated-reading. As a benchmark, the difference between Fraser s (1998) students speed when reading to memorize (i.e. 97 words per minute) and their speed when reading to learn (i.e. 140 words per minute) was 43 words per minute. Also, the difference between these students speed when reading to learn and their speed when reading for enjoyment (i.e. 185 words per minute) was 45 words per minute. Improving students reading rate by 40 words per minute, which is obtainable in a school semester, would give students a push toward being able to read with enjoyment through higher fluency. However, this is not nearly enough to push low or intermediate readers to the 250 words per minute reading rate suggested by Nation (2009). If reading rates consistently increased through a continued treatment of repeated-reading, then two years of undergoing

15 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 117 repeated-reading could possibly support students to achieving a good reading speed in English. RQ3: Can students read a new text faster after the repeated reading treatment? The third research question asking if students can read a new text faster at the end of a period of engaging in repeated-reading can be answered that students can read a text different from the treatment text passages faster at the end of the treatment period when compared to the speed by which the students read a comparable text at the beginning of the treatment period. However, the support for this assentation is quite weak. The gain of 45 words per minute on narrative texts by Taiwanese learners (Chang & Millett, 2013) and gain of 40 words per minute by Japanese learners (Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002) were notable gains which compared favorably to the gains these learners made when reading the treatment texts. However, only gains made by Chang and Millett learners (2013) were statistically significantly different from a comparison group. The single adult proficiency learner investigated by Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, and Snipp (2012) was also able to read 24 words per minute faster after the treatment period. The other studies showed either a decrease in reading speed (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004) or an increase which was not statistically different from a control group (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008). There were 4 unsuccessful attempts to show that students could read a text different from those used to practice repeated-reading at a rate comparable to the rates which the students could read the treatment passages before a single study showed success. Chang and Millett (2013) posit that the biggest factor was the careful control of vocabulary and grammar in the passages used in the study. The testing passages contained a comparable level of vocabulary words and were less grammatically complex than the treatment passages. It is possible that the testing passages that were used in the studies by Taguchi and his coauthors has been more carefully controlled for vocabulary and grammar, the students in those studies would have shown reading rate gains. More research is needed, but it can be tentatively stated that engaging in repeated-reading assists a retained, greater level of reading rate. RQ4: How does reading comprehension improve due to silent repeated-reading? The fourth research question asking if students can increase their reading comprehension after a period of engaging in repeated-reading can be answered that students can comprehend a text different from the treatment text passages more comprehensively at the end of the treatment period when compared to the amount by which the students comprehended a comparable text at the beginning of the treatment period. According to Nation (2009, p. 141), non-native readers should be able to score 7 or 8 out of

16 118 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 10 on a comprehension test when reading with higher scores representing that a reader is going too slow and lower scores indicating that a reader is going too fast. Studies where the proficiency of the students under investigation were clearly higher than the reading materials used for testing reading comprehension (Chang & Millett, 2013; Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008; Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012) demonstrated that students could read the text at an appropriate rate and improve their reading comprehension. However, only the students in Chang and Millett (2013) scored near the benchmark of 70% to 80% of questions answered correctly proposed by Nation (2009). The other studies which measured reading comprehension as recall of propositions required the students to read the passage five times to approach 50% recall of the text. Mostly, comprehension was operationalized as recall of propositions in the text (Chang & Millett, 2013; Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008; Taguchi, 1997; Taguchi & Gorsuch, 2002; Taguchi, Gorsuch, Takayasu-Maass, & Snipp, 2012; Taguchi, Takayasu-Maass, & Gorsuch, 2004). Recall is a more difficult test of comprehension than identification (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001, p.66-70). Even after the repeated-reading treatment, the percentage of propositions recalled is quite low. These studies show that keeping to Nation s (2009) 70% criterion on a first reading is quite difficult, if reading comprehension is measured through what students can recall. Students reading comprehension does improve through repeated-reading, however, the results obtained does not show repeated-reading improves reading comprehension more than another type of reading instruction, such as extensive reading or intensive reading. As with improvements in silent reading rate, the groups where the tests of reading comprehension where the vocabulary contained in the texts and the grammatical composition of the text were clearly within the students knowledge showed the clearest improvements in reading comprehension. This follows from Nation s (2009) criterion for a fluency development activity where students should be in control of the reading material that they are reading. Improvements in reading comprehension through repeated-reading are clearly shown in studies with contrasts between the students who engaged in repeated-reading and students who do not engage in a reading treatment (Gorsuch & Taguchi, 2008) and students who used a comparison group which spent less time reading (i.e. read the same text only once) than the treatment group (Chang & Millett, 2013). RQ5: How does Repeated-Reading develop reading fluency? The fifth research question asking how students can develop their reading fluency through silent repeated reading can be answered that students improve their lexical and grammatical processing so that they can devote more cognitive resources to semantic processing which is a

17 Implementing Silent Repeated-Reading 119 primary tenant of Automaticity Theory (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974). Automaticity Theory posits that readers naturally focus on higher levels of meaning (i.e. episodes contained in the text) and only focus on the textual features if there is less than automatic recognition of the words or phrases being read. Automaticity Theory also posits that language processing is naturally an auditory process for which the visual recognition of text is only an intermediary step in phonologic processing. As such, non-native readers need to automatize the conversion of textual features into auditory language to become more fluent readers. The learners perceptions of engaging in repeated-reading provide some support for this process of developing their reading fluency. Taguchi, Gorsuch, Lems, and Rosszell (2016) synthesized their research into how engaging in repeated-reading develops reading fluency in non-native speakers. They concluded that the research into developing reading fluency has shown that repeated-reading does positively affect the reading ability of non-native readers of English in three primary ways. Firstly, the authors argued that repetition and auditory models assist English language learners by scaffolding the reading process. In this context, this means that audio helps the learners to internally convert the text on the page to sound in their head. Thus, the audio model of the text assists readers in utilizing inner speech which occurs in reading (Pollatsek, 2015) while silent reading. The authors explained their assertion through the implicit prosody hypothesis (Fodor, 2002) which posits that prosody allows readers to hold language that they are reading in working memory which assists in the process of parsing and semantic analysis. The authors supported their view by reviewing the qualitative data from studies into the effects of repeated reading which showed that learners who engaged in repeated-reading perceived that the audio version of the text that they listened to while reading both (a) pushed the speed by which English language learners read the text and (b) assisted English language learners in comprehending story dialog. The studies consistently found that repeated-reading helped learners develop vocabulary recognition skills which allowed readers to focus on higher level features of reading comprehension (i.e. parsing the meaning of sentences and synthesizing the text into a coherent discourse) as opposed to lower level features such as vocabulary identification and grammatical parsing. One of the studies also identified that it is imperative that texts which are used in interventions designed for fluency development are controlled to contain only vocabulary which are within the control of the reader as neither an audio model nor repetition could provide the meaning for words which are completely unknown to, or unable to be guessed through context by, the reader. Secondly, Taguchi, Gorsuch, Lems, and Rosszell (2016) argued that engaging in repeated-

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency s CEFR CEFR OVERALL ORAL PRODUCTION Has a good command of idiomatic expressions and colloquialisms with awareness of connotative levels of meaning. Can convey

More information

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY?

DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY? DOES RETELLING TECHNIQUE IMPROVE SPEAKING FLUENCY? Noor Rachmawaty (itaw75123@yahoo.com) Istanti Hermagustiana (dulcemaria_81@yahoo.com) Universitas Mulawarman, Indonesia Abstract: This paper is based

More information

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE

5. UPPER INTERMEDIATE Triolearn General Programmes adapt the standards and the Qualifications of Common European Framework of Reference (CEFR) and Cambridge ESOL. It is designed to be compatible to the local and the regional

More information

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards) Grade 4 Common Core Adoption Process (Unpacked Standards) Grade 4 Reading: Literature RL.4.1 Refer to details and examples in a text when explaining what the text says explicitly and when drawing inferences

More information

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University Kifah Rakan Alqadi Al Al-Bayt University Faculty of Arts Department of English Language

More information

and secondary sources, attending to such features as the date and origin of the information.

and secondary sources, attending to such features as the date and origin of the information. RH.9-10.1. Cite specific textual evidence to support analysis of primary and secondary sources, attending to such features as the date and origin of the information. RH.9-10.1. Cite specific textual evidence

More information

21st Century Community Learning Center

21st Century Community Learning Center 21st Century Community Learning Center Grant Overview This Request for Proposal (RFP) is designed to distribute funds to qualified applicants pursuant to Title IV, Part B, of the Elementary and Secondary

More information

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London

To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING. Kazuya Saito. Birkbeck, University of London To appear in The TESOL encyclopedia of ELT (Wiley-Blackwell) 1 RECASTING Kazuya Saito Birkbeck, University of London Abstract Among the many corrective feedback techniques at ESL/EFL teachers' disposal,

More information

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3 The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3 The State Board adopted the Oregon K-12 Literacy Framework (December 2009) as guidance for the State, districts, and schools

More information

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful?

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful? University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Action Research Projects Math in the Middle Institute Partnership 7-2008 Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom:

More information

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level.

Candidates must achieve a grade of at least C2 level in each examination in order to achieve the overall qualification at C2 Level. The Test of Interactive English, C2 Level Qualification Structure The Test of Interactive English consists of two units: Unit Name English English Each Unit is assessed via a separate examination, set,

More information

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England THE EFFECTS OF SUPER SPEED 100 ON READING FLUENCY 1 The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency Jennifer Thorne University of New England THE EFFECTS OF SUPER SPEED 100 ON READING FLUENCY 2 Abstract

More information

Language Acquisition Chart

Language Acquisition Chart Language Acquisition Chart This chart was designed to help teachers better understand the process of second language acquisition. Please use this chart as a resource for learning more about the way people

More information

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started

Organizing Comprehensive Literacy Assessment: How to Get Started Organizing Comprehensive Assessment: How to Get Started September 9 & 16, 2009 Questions to Consider How do you design individualized, comprehensive instruction? How can you determine where to begin instruction?

More information

South Carolina English Language Arts

South Carolina English Language Arts South Carolina English Language Arts A S O F J U N E 2 0, 2 0 1 0, T H I S S TAT E H A D A D O P T E D T H E CO M M O N CO R E S TAT E S TA N DA R D S. DOCUMENTS REVIEWED South Carolina Academic Content

More information

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s))

PAGE(S) WHERE TAUGHT If sub mission ins not a book, cite appropriate location(s)) Ohio Academic Content Standards Grade Level Indicators (Grade 11) A. ACQUISITION OF VOCABULARY Students acquire vocabulary through exposure to language-rich situations, such as reading books and other

More information

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction

CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1. High Priority Items Phonemic Awareness Instruction CLASSIFICATION OF PROGRAM Critical Elements Analysis 1 Program Name: Macmillan/McGraw Hill Reading 2003 Date of Publication: 2003 Publisher: Macmillan/McGraw Hill Reviewer Code: 1. X The program meets

More information

Number of Items and Test Administration Times IDEA English Language Proficiency Tests/ North Carolina Testing Program.

Number of Items and Test Administration Times IDEA English Language Proficiency Tests/ North Carolina Testing Program. IDEA English Language Proficiency Tests/ North Carolina Testing Program IPT Kindergarten Subtest Tasks Number of Items Testing Time Answer Questions about Yourself & Follow Directions Give Directions Understand

More information

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1

Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1 Florida Reading Endorsement Alignment Matrix Competency 1 Reading Endorsement Guiding Principle: Teachers will understand and teach reading as an ongoing strategic process resulting in students comprehending

More information

Curriculum and Assessment Guide (CAG) Elementary California Treasures First Grade

Curriculum and Assessment Guide (CAG) Elementary California Treasures First Grade Curriculum and Assessment Guide (CAG) Elementary 2012-2013 California Treasures First Grade 1 2 English Language Arts CORE INSTRUCTIONAL MATERIALS 2012-2013 Grade 1 Macmillan/McGraw-Hill California Treasures

More information

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening

A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors in L2 Listening ISSN 1798-4769 Journal of Language Teaching and Research, Vol. 4, No. 3, pp. 504-510, May 2013 Manufactured in Finland. doi:10.4304/jltr.4.3.504-510 A Study of Metacognitive Awareness of Non-English Majors

More information

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading

Program Matrix - Reading English 6-12 (DOE Code 398) University of Florida. Reading Program Requirements Competency 1: Foundations of Instruction 60 In-service Hours Teachers will develop substantive understanding of six components of reading as a process: comprehension, oral language,

More information

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities.

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities. The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities By Erica Blouin Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany

SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1. Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany Journal of Reading Behavior 1980, Vol. II, No. 1 SCHEMA ACTIVATION IN MEMORY FOR PROSE 1 Michael A. R. Townsend State University of New York at Albany Abstract. Forty-eight college students listened to

More information

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Tyler Perrachione LING 451-0 Proseminar in Sound Structure Prof. A. Bradlow 17 March 2006 Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Abstract Although the acoustic and

More information

EQuIP Review Feedback

EQuIP Review Feedback EQuIP Review Feedback Lesson/Unit Name: On the Rainy River and The Red Convertible (Module 4, Unit 1) Content Area: English language arts Grade Level: 11 Dimension I Alignment to the Depth of the CCSS

More information

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017

Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School. January 2017 Understanding and Supporting Dyslexia Godstone Village School January 2017 By then end of the session I will: Have a greater understanding of Dyslexia and the ways in which children can be affected by

More information

Implementing the English Language Arts Common Core State Standards

Implementing the English Language Arts Common Core State Standards 1st Grade Implementing the English Language Arts Common Core State Standards A Teacher s Guide to the Common Core Standards: An Illinois Content Model Framework English Language Arts/Literacy Adapted from

More information

Lower and Upper Secondary

Lower and Upper Secondary Lower and Upper Secondary Type of Course Age Group Content Duration Target General English Lower secondary Grammar work, reading and comprehension skills, speech and drama. Using Multi-Media CD - Rom 7

More information

prehending general textbooks, but are unable to compensate these problems on the micro level in comprehending mathematical texts.

prehending general textbooks, but are unable to compensate these problems on the micro level in comprehending mathematical texts. Summary Chapter 1 of this thesis shows that language plays an important role in education. Students are expected to learn from textbooks on their own, to listen actively to the instruction of the teacher,

More information

THE IMPLEMENTATION OF SPEED READING TECHNIQUE TO IMPROVE COMPREHENSION ACHIEVEMENT

THE IMPLEMENTATION OF SPEED READING TECHNIQUE TO IMPROVE COMPREHENSION ACHIEVEMENT THE IMPLEMENTATION OF SPEED READING TECHNIQUE TO IMPROVE COMPREHENSION ACHIEVEMENT Fusthaathul Rizkoh 1, Jos E. Ohoiwutun 2, Nur Sehang Thamrin 3 Abstract This study investigated that the implementation

More information

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL)  Feb 2015 Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) www.angielskiwmedycynie.org.pl Feb 2015 Developing speaking abilities is a prerequisite for HELP in order to promote effective communication

More information

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach

SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach SLINGERLAND: A Multisensory Structured Language Instructional Approach nancycushenwhite@gmail.com Lexicon Reading Center Dubai Teaching Reading IS Rocket Science 5% will learn to read on their own. 20-30%

More information

MYP Language A Course Outline Year 3

MYP Language A Course Outline Year 3 Course Description: The fundamental piece to learning, thinking, communicating, and reflecting is language. Language A seeks to further develop six key skill areas: listening, speaking, reading, writing,

More information

Student Name: OSIS#: DOB: / / School: Grade:

Student Name: OSIS#: DOB: / / School: Grade: Grade 6 ELA CCLS: Reading Standards for Literature Column : In preparation for the IEP meeting, check the standards the student has already met. Column : In preparation for the IEP meeting, check the standards

More information

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82

The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82 The Common European Framework of Reference for Languages p. 58 to p. 82 -- Chapter 4 Language use and language user/learner in 4.1 «Communicative language activities and strategies» -- Oral Production

More information

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None

Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM. Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None Grade 11 Language Arts (2 Semester Course) CURRICULUM Course Description ENGLISH 11 (2 Semester Course) Duration: 2 Semesters Prerequisite: None Through the integrated study of literature, composition,

More information

Metadiscourse in Knowledge Building: A question about written or verbal metadiscourse

Metadiscourse in Knowledge Building: A question about written or verbal metadiscourse Metadiscourse in Knowledge Building: A question about written or verbal metadiscourse Rolf K. Baltzersen Paper submitted to the Knowledge Building Summer Institute 2013 in Puebla, Mexico Author: Rolf K.

More information

The role of the first language in foreign language learning. Paul Nation. The role of the first language in foreign language learning

The role of the first language in foreign language learning. Paul Nation. The role of the first language in foreign language learning 1 Article Title The role of the first language in foreign language learning Author Paul Nation Bio: Paul Nation teaches in the School of Linguistics and Applied Language Studies at Victoria University

More information

Publisher Citations. Program Description. Primary Supporting Y N Universal Access: Teacher s Editions Adjust on the Fly all grades:

Publisher Citations. Program Description. Primary Supporting Y N Universal Access: Teacher s Editions Adjust on the Fly all grades: KEY: Editions (TE), Extra Support (EX), Amazing Words (AW), Think, Talk, and Write (TTW) SECTION 1: PROGRAM DESCRIPTION All instructional material submissions must meet the requirements of this program

More information

Running head: METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING 1. The Relationship between Metacognitive Strategies Awareness

Running head: METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING 1. The Relationship between Metacognitive Strategies Awareness Running head: METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING 1 The Relationship between Metacognitive Strategies Awareness and Listening Comprehension Performance Valeriia Bogorevich Northern Arizona

More information

Language Center. Course Catalog

Language Center. Course Catalog Language Center Course Catalog 2016-2017 Mastery of languages facilitates access to new and diverse opportunities, and IE University (IEU) considers knowledge of multiple languages a key element of its

More information

Application of Multimedia Technology in Vocabulary Learning for Engineering Students

Application of Multimedia Technology in Vocabulary Learning for Engineering Students Application of Multimedia Technology in Vocabulary Learning for Engineering Students https://doi.org/10.3991/ijet.v12i01.6153 Xue Shi Luoyang Institute of Science and Technology, Luoyang, China xuewonder@aliyun.com

More information

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 -

Think A F R I C A when assessing speaking. C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria. Think A F R I C A - 1 - C.E.F.R. Oral Assessment Criteria Think A F R I C A - 1 - 1. The extracts in the left hand column are taken from the official descriptors of the CEFR levels. How would you grade them on a scale of low,

More information

Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers

Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers Section II Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers Chapter 5 Components of Effective Instruction After conducting assessments, Ms. Lopez should be aware of her students needs in the following areas:

More information

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity.

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity. Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1 Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity Jessica Hanna Eastern Illinois University DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICITY

More information

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report

Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report Linking the Common European Framework of Reference and the Michigan English Language Assessment Battery Technical Report Contact Information All correspondence and mailings should be addressed to: CaMLA

More information

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS

Arizona s English Language Arts Standards th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS Arizona s English Language Arts Standards 11-12th Grade ARIZONA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION HIGH ACADEMIC STANDARDS FOR STUDENTS 11 th -12 th Grade Overview Arizona s English Language Arts Standards work together

More information

National Standards for Foreign Language Education

National Standards for Foreign Language Education A Correlation of Prentice Hall Ecce Romani I To the ACTFL American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Language National Standards for Foreign Language Education A Correlation of Statement of Philosophy

More information

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH

REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH Language Learning & Technology http://llt.msu.edu/vol8num1/review2/ January 2004, Volume 8, Number 1 pp. 24-28 REVIEW OF CONNECTED SPEECH Title Connected Speech (North American English), 2000 Platform

More information

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations

Criterion Met? Primary Supporting Y N Reading Street Comprehensive. Publisher Citations Program 2: / Arts English Development Basic Program, K-8 Grade Level(s): K 3 SECTIO 1: PROGRAM DESCRIPTIO All instructional material submissions must meet the requirements of this program description section,

More information

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW

UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW UDL AND LANGUAGE ARTS LESSON OVERVIEW Title: Reading Comprehension Author: Carol Sue Englert Subject: Language Arts Grade Level 3 rd grade Duration 60 minutes Unit Description Focusing on the students

More information

Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: GRADE 1

Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: GRADE 1 The Common Core State Standards and the Social Studies: Preparing Young Students for College, Career, and Citizenship Common Core Exemplar for English Language Arts and Social Studies: Why We Need Rules

More information

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight

Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development. Ben Knight Assessing speaking skills:. a workshop for teacher development Ben Knight Speaking skills are often considered the most important part of an EFL course, and yet the difficulties in testing oral skills

More information

Let's Learn English Lesson Plan

Let's Learn English Lesson Plan Let's Learn English Lesson Plan Introduction: Let's Learn English lesson plans are based on the CALLA approach. See the end of each lesson for more information and resources on teaching with the CALLA

More information

Language Arts: ( ) Instructional Syllabus. Teachers: T. Beard address

Language Arts: ( ) Instructional Syllabus. Teachers: T. Beard  address Renaissance Middle School 7155 Hall Road Fairburn, Georgia 30213 Phone: 770-306-4330 Fax: 770-306-4338 Dr. Sandra DeShazier, Principal Benzie Brinson, 7 th grade Administrator Language Arts: (2013-2014)

More information

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning Age Effects on Syntactic Control in Second Language Learning Miriam Tullgren Loyola University Chicago Abstract 1 This paper explores the effects of age on second language acquisition in adolescents, ages

More information

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis

Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis FYE Program at Marquette University Rubric for Scoring English 1 Unit 1, Rhetorical Analysis Writing Conventions INTEGRATING SOURCE MATERIAL 3 Proficient Outcome Effectively expresses purpose in the introduction

More information

ELS LanguagE CEntrES CurriCuLum OvErviEw & PEDagOgiCaL PhiLOSOPhy

ELS LanguagE CEntrES CurriCuLum OvErviEw & PEDagOgiCaL PhiLOSOPhy ELS Language Centres Curriculum Overview & Pedagogical Philosophy .. TABLE OF CONTENTS ELS Background. 1 Acceptance of ELS Levels. 1 Features of ELS Language Centres Academic Program 2 English for Academic

More information

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t.

Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why. develop dyslexia and others don t. The Dyslexia Handbook 2013 69 Aryan van der Leij, Elsje van Bergen and Peter de Jong Longitudinal family-risk studies of dyslexia: why some children develop dyslexia and others don t. Longitudinal family-risk

More information

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT

The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic Familiarity on Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners Written Performance in TBLT ISSN 1799-2591 Theory and Practice in Language Studies, Vol. 2, No. 11, pp. 2308-2315, November 2012 Manufactured in Finland. doi:10.4304/tpls.2.11.2308-2315 The Effects of Strategic Planning and Topic

More information

TAIWANESE STUDENT ATTITUDES TOWARDS AND BEHAVIORS DURING ONLINE GRAMMAR TESTING WITH MOODLE

TAIWANESE STUDENT ATTITUDES TOWARDS AND BEHAVIORS DURING ONLINE GRAMMAR TESTING WITH MOODLE TAIWANESE STUDENT ATTITUDES TOWARDS AND BEHAVIORS DURING ONLINE GRAMMAR TESTING WITH MOODLE Ryan Berg TransWorld University Yi-chen Lu TransWorld University Main Points 2 When taking online tests, students

More information

George Mason University Graduate School of Education Program: Special Education

George Mason University Graduate School of Education Program: Special Education George Mason University Graduate School of Education Program: Special Education 1 EDSE 590: Research Methods in Special Education Instructor: Margo A. Mastropieri, Ph.D. Assistant: Judy Ericksen Section

More information

The Effect of Syntactic Simplicity and Complexity on the Readability of the Text

The Effect of Syntactic Simplicity and Complexity on the Readability of the Text ISSN 798-769 Journal of Language Teaching and Research, Vol., No., pp. 8-9, September 2 2 ACADEMY PUBLISHER Manufactured in Finland. doi:.3/jltr...8-9 The Effect of Syntactic Simplicity and Complexity

More information

E-3: Check for academic understanding

E-3: Check for academic understanding Respond instructively After you check student understanding, it is time to respond - through feedback and follow-up questions. Doing this allows you to gauge how much students actually comprehend and push

More information

TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING

TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING TRAITS OF GOOD WRITING Each paper was scored on a scale of - on the following traits of good writing: Ideas and Content: Organization: Voice: Word Choice: Sentence Fluency: Conventions: The ideas are clear,

More information

One Stop Shop For Educators

One Stop Shop For Educators Modern Languages Level II Course Description One Stop Shop For Educators The Level II language course focuses on the continued development of communicative competence in the target language and understanding

More information

November 2012 MUET (800)

November 2012 MUET (800) November 2012 MUET (800) OVERALL PERFORMANCE A total of 75 589 candidates took the November 2012 MUET. The performance of candidates for each paper, 800/1 Listening, 800/2 Speaking, 800/3 Reading and 800/4

More information

Reference to Tenure track faculty in this document includes tenured faculty, unless otherwise noted.

Reference to Tenure track faculty in this document includes tenured faculty, unless otherwise noted. PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT FACULTY DEVELOPMENT and EVALUATION MANUAL Approved by Philosophy Department April 14, 2011 Approved by the Office of the Provost June 30, 2011 The Department of Philosophy Faculty

More information

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE

LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE LEXICAL COHESION ANALYSIS OF THE ARTICLE WHAT IS A GOOD RESEARCH PROJECT? BY BRIAN PALTRIDGE A JOURNAL ARTICLE Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Sarjana Sastra (S.S.)

More information

TEKS Correlations Proclamation 2017

TEKS Correlations Proclamation 2017 and Skills (TEKS): Material Correlations to the Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills (TEKS): Material Subject Course Publisher Program Title Program ISBN TEKS Coverage (%) Chapter 114. Texas Essential

More information

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition

Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition Achievement Level Descriptors for American Literature and Composition Georgia Department of Education September 2015 All Rights Reserved Achievement Levels and Achievement Level Descriptors With the implementation

More information

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Volume 3, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 110-120 Available online at www.jallr.com ISSN: 2376-760X The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of

More information

Textbook Evalyation:

Textbook Evalyation: STUDIES IN LITERATURE AND LANGUAGE Vol. 1, No. 8, 2010, pp. 54-60 www.cscanada.net ISSN 1923-1555 [Print] ISSN 1923-1563 [Online] www.cscanada.org Textbook Evalyation: EFL Teachers Perspectives on New

More information

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012

Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012 Linguistics Program Outcomes Assessment 2012 BA in Linguistics / MA in Applied Linguistics Compiled by Siri Tuttle, Program Head The mission of the UAF Linguistics Program is to promote a broader understanding

More information

Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing. Mika MIYASONE

Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing. Mika MIYASONE Improving Advanced Learners' Communication Skills Through Paragraph Reading and Writing Mika MIYASONE Tohoku Institute of Technology 6, Futatsusawa, Taihaku Sendau, Miyagi, 982-8588 Japan Tel: +81-22-304-5532

More information

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF)

Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF) Introduction to the Common European Framework (CEF) The Common European Framework is a common reference for describing language learning, teaching, and assessment. In order to facilitate both teaching

More information

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading ELA/ELD Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading The English Language Arts (ELA) required for the one hour of English-Language Development (ELD) Materials are listed in Appendix 9-A, Matrix

More information

IMPROVING READING COMPREHENSION THROUGH PQRST OF EIGHTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMP NEGERI 2 BANJARANGKAN IN ACADEMIC YEAR 2012/2013

IMPROVING READING COMPREHENSION THROUGH PQRST OF EIGHTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMP NEGERI 2 BANJARANGKAN IN ACADEMIC YEAR 2012/2013 IMPROVING READING COMPREHENSION THROUGH PQRST OF EIGHTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMP NEGERI 2 BANJARANGKAN IN ACADEMIC YEAR 2012/2013 BY KADEK ANGGA SEPTIARI NPM: 11.8.03.51.31.2.5.4522 ENGLISH DEPARTMENT THE

More information

Fall 2016 ARA 4400/ 7152

Fall 2016 ARA 4400/ 7152 Instructor information: Instructor: Sarra Tlili Office hours: Thursday 10-12 Office: Pugh Hall, 354 Email address: satlili@ufl.edu Phone: (352) 392-8678 meeting times and places Days Per Bldg Room T 08

More information

COMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING

COMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING COMMUNICATIVE LANGUAGE TEACHING There are many ways to teach language. One is called Communicative Language Teaching (CLT). This method is learner-centered and emphasizes communication and real-life situations.

More information

Teachers Guide Chair Study

Teachers Guide Chair Study Certificate of Initial Mastery Task Booklet 2006-2007 School Year Teachers Guide Chair Study Dance Modified On-Demand Task Revised 4-19-07 Central Falls Johnston Middletown West Warwick Coventry Lincoln

More information

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW Method Rosetta Stone teaches languages using a fully-interactive immersion process that requires the student to indicate comprehension of the new language and provides immediate

More information

Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson

Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson English Highlighting and Annotation Tips Foundation Lesson About this Lesson Annotating a text can be a permanent record of the reader s intellectual conversation with a text. Annotation can help a reader

More information

New Ways of Connecting Reading and Writing

New Ways of Connecting Reading and Writing Sanchez, P., & Salazar, M. (2012). Transnational computer use in urban Latino immigrant communities: Implications for schooling. Urban Education, 47(1), 90 116. doi:10.1177/0042085911427740 Smith, N. (1993).

More information

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017 GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017 Instructor: Dr. Claudia Schwabe Class hours: TR 9:00-10:15 p.m. claudia.schwabe@usu.edu Class room: Old Main 301 Office: Old Main 002D Office hours:

More information

Texas First Fluency Folder For First Grade

Texas First Fluency Folder For First Grade Texas First Fluency Folder For First Grade Free PDF ebook Download: Texas First Fluency Folder For First Grade Download or Read Online ebook texas first fluency folder for first grade in PDF Format From

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF ENGLISH SONG TOWARD STUDENTS VOCABULARY MASTERY AND STUDENTS MOTIVATION

THE INFLUENCE OF ENGLISH SONG TOWARD STUDENTS VOCABULARY MASTERY AND STUDENTS MOTIVATION 77 THE INFLUENCE OF ENGLISH SONG TOWARD STUDENTS VOCABULARY MASTERY AND STUDENTS MOTIVATION By Eva Faliyanti Muhammadiyah University of Metro evafaliyanti1980@gmail.com Abstract Learning vocabulary is

More information

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS Pirjo Moen Department of Computer Science P.O. Box 68 FI-00014 University of Helsinki pirjo.moen@cs.helsinki.fi http://www.cs.helsinki.fi/pirjo.moen

More information

PROGRESS MONITORING FOR STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES Participant Materials

PROGRESS MONITORING FOR STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES Participant Materials Instructional Accommodations and Curricular Modifications Bringing Learning Within the Reach of Every Student PROGRESS MONITORING FOR STUDENTS WITH DISABILITIES Participant Materials 2007, Stetson Online

More information

University of Pittsburgh Department of Slavic Languages and Literatures. Russian 0015: Russian for Heritage Learners 2 MoWe 3:00PM - 4:15PM G13 CL

University of Pittsburgh Department of Slavic Languages and Literatures. Russian 0015: Russian for Heritage Learners 2 MoWe 3:00PM - 4:15PM G13 CL 1 University of Pittsburgh Department of Slavic Languages and Literatures Russian 0015: Russian for Heritage Learners 2 MoWe 3:00PM - 4:15PM G13 CL Spring 2011 Instructor: Yuliya Basina e-mail basina@pitt.edu

More information

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure

Characteristics of the Text Genre Informational Text Text Structure LESSON 4 TEACHER S GUIDE by Taiyo Kobayashi Fountas-Pinnell Level C Informational Text Selection Summary The narrator presents key locations in his town and why each is important to the community: a store,

More information

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy 1 Desired Results Developmental Profile (2015) [DRDP (2015)] Correspondence to California Foundations: Language and Development (LLD) and the Foundations (PLF) The Language and Development (LLD) domain

More information

PIRLS. International Achievement in the Processes of Reading Comprehension Results from PIRLS 2001 in 35 Countries

PIRLS. International Achievement in the Processes of Reading Comprehension Results from PIRLS 2001 in 35 Countries Ina V.S. Mullis Michael O. Martin Eugenio J. Gonzalez PIRLS International Achievement in the Processes of Reading Comprehension Results from PIRLS 2001 in 35 Countries International Study Center International

More information

MERGA 20 - Aotearoa

MERGA 20 - Aotearoa Assessing Number Sense: Collaborative Initiatives in Australia, United States, Sweden and Taiwan AIistair McIntosh, Jack Bana & Brian FarreII Edith Cowan University Group tests of Number Sense were devised

More information

FREE COLLEGE Can Happen to You!

FREE COLLEGE Can Happen to You! FREE COLLEGE Can Happen to You! LECTURE NOTES by Jean Burk Thank you so much for coming to my seminars/ booth or purchasing this lecture on tape. Here is the handout from my speech, complete with notes.

More information

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts

Common Core State Standards for English Language Arts Reading Standards for Literature 6-12 Grade 9-10 Students: 1. Cite strong and thorough textual evidence to support analysis of what the text says explicitly as well as inferences drawn from the text. 2.

More information

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature

Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature Correlation of Literature and the Language Arts Experiencing Literature Grade 9 2 nd edition to the Nebraska Reading/Writing Standards EMC/Paradigm Publishing 875 Montreal Way St. Paul, Minnesota 55102

More information

1/25/2012. Common Core Georgia Performance Standards Grade 4 English Language Arts. Andria Bunner Sallie Mills ELA Program Specialists

1/25/2012. Common Core Georgia Performance Standards Grade 4 English Language Arts. Andria Bunner Sallie Mills ELA Program Specialists Common Core Georgia Performance Standards Grade 4 English Language Arts Andria Bunner Sallie Mills ELA Program Specialists 1 Welcome Today s Agenda 4 th Grade ELA CCGPS Overview Organizational Comparisons

More information