Early vocabulary and gestures in Estonian children*

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Early vocabulary and gestures in Estonian children*"

Transcription

1 J. Child Lang. 39 (2012), f Cambridge University Press 2011 doi: /s Early vocabulary and gestures in Estonian children* ASTRA SCHULTS, TIIA TULVISTE University of Tartu, Centre of Behavioral and Health Sciences, Estonia AND KENN KONSTABEL University of Tartu, Centre of Behavioral and Health Sciences, Estonia, and National Institute for Health Development, Estonia (Received 7 April 2010 Revised 25 November 2010 Accepted 24 May 2011 First published online 31 August 2011) ABSTRACT Parents of 592 children between the age of 0;8 and 1;4 completed the Estonian adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (ECDI Infant Form). The relationships between comprehension and production of different categories of words and gestures were examined. According to the results of regression modelling the production of object gestures and gestural routines was positively correlated with the use of all the word categories. Comprehension of common nouns was positively correlated to the production of common nouns and predicates, whereas the comprehension of predicates was negatively correlated to the production of common nouns and social terms. The older the children were the more they produced words from each category. Girls were reported to produce more social terms. First-born children had an advantage over later-born children in the production of common nouns. Maternal educational level was associated with the production of common nouns and predicates. INTRODUCTION Early development of vocabulary and gestures has been in the focus of research for decades. Extensive research has been carried out to describe [*] Research for this article was supported by the Estonian Ministry of Science and Education (grant SF ). The authors would like to thank the parents participating in the study. We also would like to thank Larry Fenson and the CDI Advisory Board for permission to adapt the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories to Estonian. Address for correspondence: Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tiigi , Tartu, ESTONIA. tel: (372 7) ; fax: (372 7) ; astra.schults@ut.ee 664

2 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES infants vocabulary composition or gestural repertoire by means of parental reports using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures (CDI) that has been adapted into more than thirty languages (Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal & Pethick, 1994). The present study concerns Estonian-speaking children. The first purpose of the study was to describe the proportional acquisition of different word classes in early vocabulary in Estonian children between the ages of 0;8 and 1;4. The second purpose was to investigate the concurrent associations between Estonian-speaking children s early gestural repertoire and comprehension vocabulary with their production vocabulary. The third purpose of our study was to study the effects of the child s age, gender, birth order and maternal education on infants production vocabulary. With these aims, the Estonian adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures (ECDI Infant Form) was used (Schults & Tulviste, 2008). Word comprehension and production Comprehension of words used to label objects or to designate actions appears in a child s development between the age of 0;8 and 0;10, before they have started to produce any words (Fenson et al., 1994; Stolt, Haataja, Lapinleimu & Lehtonen, 2008). Children comprehend more words than they are able to produce up to the age of three (Fenson et al., 1994). Before the age of 1; 0 the production of words is minimal (Bates et al., 1994; Fenson et al., 1994; Stolt et al., 2008). The onset of speech is followed by a period of quite steady growth in the size of the vocabulary. Still, there are individual differences which lead to the variability in the size of production vocabulary after the age of 1; 1 (Fenson et al., 1994). Accelerated growth (the so-called spurt ) in early production vocabulary, especially after the age of 1; 3, has been demonstrated, among many other languages, in Finnish a language that is closely related to Estonian (Stolt et al., 2008). Both Estonian and Finnish belong to the Finno-Ugric languages. Both are agglutinating languages in which mainly suffixes are used to express grammatical relations. Estonian has a large number of cases fourteen to inflect nominals, and verbs are inflected as well for subject verb agreement, tense, etc. Estonian as well as Finnish lacks grammatical future and grammatical gender. We expected to find the accelerated growth pattern after the age of 1;3 to apply also to Estonian-speaking children and we explored the ratio between comprehension and production of words in Estonian-speaking children in the age range of 0;8 to 1;4. When the size of the vocabulary is less than fifty words, social terms are used proportionally more by the children than words belonging to other categories (e.g. common nouns, predicates) (Caselli, Casadio & Bates, 1999; 665

3 SCHULTS ET AL. Stolt et al., 2008). At the early stages of vocabulary production the acquisition of predicates is at a low level but shows a slow and steady growth pattern; at the same time the acquisition of function words stays at a constant low level (Caselli et al., 1999; Bates et al., 1994). As the vocabulary grows to between 50 and 200 words nouns form the majority of production vocabulary items (Caselli et al., 1999; Bates et al., 1994). Since the studies of Nelson (1973) it has been known that for English-speaking children nouns, compared to non-nouns, form the larger proportion of the early vocabulary that contains fifty words. Research on early vocabulary has found this pattern of nouns forming the largest proportion of the early production vocabulary in several languages, including Finnish (Bornstein et al., 2004a; Caselli et al., 1995; Fenson et al., 1994; Stolt et al., 2008; Wehberg, Vach, Bleses, Thomsen, Madsen & Basbøll, 2007). Still, noun dominance is not universal. There are reported findings about Korean and Mandarin that have shown that more verbs as compared to nouns can be produced by children in the phase of early vocabulary development (e.g. Tardif, 1996). One possible explanation for the noun bias has been differences in child-directed speech (Hart, 2004). For instance, it has been found that North American mothers speech contains a high proportion of nouns whereas Asian mothers speech contains proportionally more verbs (see Hoff, 2006). It is also possible that parents from different cultural background may use different strategies directed towards leading their child either to produce more nouns or to produce more words belonging to the predicates category such as verbs (Fernald & Morikawa, 1993; Tardif, 1996). Comparative studies of mother child interaction have found that the speech directed towards Estonian children is more directive than the speech of American English, Finnish and Swedish mothers (Junefelt & Tulviste, 1997; Tulviste, Mizera & De Geer, 2004). Taking into account that a positive relationship has been found between maternal use of directives and the proportion of verbs in a child s early vocabulary (Tomasello & Kruger, 1992), and that the noun bias has been found using parental report in Finnish (Stolt et al., 2008), we were interested in exploring the proportions of nouns and predicates in Estonian children s early comprehension and production vocabularies. Because of that we focused on different categories of words (common nouns, predicates, social terms and function words). In addition to that, we looked into the possible connections between the comprehension and production of words belonging to different categories. Gesture production Gestures can be categorized into three types: deictic gestures, object gestures and gestural routines. 666

4 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES Deictic gestures are among the first signs of intentional communication and are used to point out an object or to draw someone s attention to an object (Bates et al., 1975; McNeill, 1992). Deictic gestures are prelinguistic, as they accompany word comprehension and emerge before the onset of word production between the ages of 0;9 and 1;1 (Bates, Camioni & Volterra, 1975; Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni & Volterra, 1979). Harris, Barlow-Brown and Chasin (1995) found that pointing gesture production and object name comprehension both emerge around the age of 0;10 or 0;11. The more primitive types of these gestures (e.g. requesting via reaching) start to decline in usage frequency as early as around the age of 0;11 (Blake & Dology, 1993), whereas pointing persists in the communication repertoire throughout life. Object gestures convey the idea of object s function (e.g. holding a phone against one s ear), and they are executed with appropriate objects in the hand (Bates, Thal, Whitesell, Fenson & Oakes, 1989). Children start to produce object gestures around the same time as they start to name objects, approximately around their first birthday. Children with more object gestures have larger vocabularies and they reach the 10-word milestone earlier than children with fewer object gestures (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988). The positive correlation between object gestures and naming will persist for about six months, approximately until the child reaches the age of 1;6 (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Bates & Dick, 2002; Bates et al., 1989; Bretherton, Bates, McNew, Williamson & Beeghly-Smith, 1981; Goodwyn & Acredolo, 1993; Iverson, Capirci & Caselli, 1994; Nelson, 1973). Gestural routines. This third type of gestures addressed in the study are culturally appropriate routine behavioural patterns associated with certain events or interaction routines, and rather than referring to common objects (e.g. waving goodbye or playing pat-a-cake) (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Bates et al., 1989; Iverson et al., 1994). Gestural routines are sometimes accompanied by words or phrases, e.g. saying bye-bye while waving goodbye. Inter-relatedness of vocabulary and gestures There has been a long theoretical debate about what kind of relationship exists between early language and gesture development: is language development parallel to gesture development, independent from it, or do the relationships vary across language components (see Bates et al., 1989; Goodwyn & Acredolo, 1993). It is known that children start to gesture before they produce their first words, that early verbal vocabulary is correlated with gestures, and that gestures predict later gestural and language development (Bates et al., 1975; Bates & Dick, 2002; Volterra, Caselli, Capirci & Pizzuto, 2005). Recent studies carried out by Rowe and Goldin-Meadow (2009a; 667

5 SCHULTS ET AL. 2009b) found that children who produced more gestures with different meanings at 1;2 had larger vocabularies at 4;6. It is not known whether infants comprehension of different types of words is related to concurrent use of various types of gestures and to what extent gesture production and word comprehension are connected to the concurrent word production. The study aims to explore relationships between infants gestural repertoire (deictic gestures, object gestures and gestural routines) and his/her concurrent comprehension and production vocabularies. Child s gender, birth order, socioeconomic status and early communication development In this study we were also interested in the effect of factors such as the child s gender, birth order and maternal education on the production of words from different categories (common nouns, predicates, social terms and function words) in the child s early vocabulary. Existing gender differences have been found to be modest. Girls tend to produce slightly more words than boys from the ages of two to five years (see Bornstein, Hahn & Haynes, 2004b). There is less information about younger children. Fenson and colleagues (1994) reported a slight advantage in girls for gesture production, word comprehension and word production in American children aged 0;8 2;6. A study with European children from ten non-english language communities showed that girls were slightly ahead of boys in early communicative gestures, in productive vocabulary and in combining words from age of 0;8 to 2;6 (Eriksson et al., in press). Some studies of birth order and language development have found that first-born children have larger vocabularies and that they acquire language in a different manner than later-borns (see Hoff, 2006). The vocabularies of first-born children are more dominated by object labels than the vocabularies of later-borns (Goldfield & Reznick, 1990). At the same time, later-born children have been found to have an advantage in some other aspects of language, for instance in the development of conversational skills (Fenson et al., 1994; Bornstein, Leach & Haynes, 2004c). It is known that parental socioeconomic status (SES), frequently associated with the level of maternal education, has an impact on a child s language development. Higher SES mothers have been found to talk more, to use a larger vocabulary and to expect more verbalization from their children than lower SES mothers. SES-related differences have been found both in measures of children s comprehension and in production vocabularies (Hoff, 2006). In addition, Rowe and Goldin-Meadow (2009a) found high SES parents to be likely to use more gestures while communicating with their children and that that was in turn positively related to more gestures used by children. However, other research has not supported the effect of 668

6 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES TABLE 1. The number of children by age and gender Gender Child s age 0;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 Row totals Boys Girls All groups parental SES on children s vocabulary and gestures at the earliest stages of language development. For instance, Fenson and colleagues (1994) found no significant SES-related differences in infants very early word and gesture production. We assumed that word comprehension had a positive effect on word production (Fenson et al., 1994; Stolt et al., 2008) and studied the inter-relations of word comprehension and production within each of the word categories. We expected children with more gestures to have more words in their production vocabulary (Bates et al., 1975; Bates & Dick, 2002; Volterra et al., 2005) and especially children with more objects gestures to produce more common nouns (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Bates & Dick, 2002; Bates et al., 1989; Bretherton et al., 1981; Goodwyn & Acredolo, 1993; Iverson et al., 1994; Nelson, 1973). We expected higher average vocabulary scores for girls than boys, for first-borns than later-borns, and for children of mothers with a higher level of education, in keeping with findings from previous studies. METHOD Participants and materials The data for this study were gathered using the Estonian adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Language and Gestures (ECDI Infant Form) for 592 children between the age of 0;8 and 1;4 (M=1;0, SD=0;0. 2) (see Table 1). Children and their parents were recruited through paediatricians, play-group teachers, Internet forums and the parents who had already completed the inventory by themselves recruited other parents, too. Parents who agreed to participate were handed out or sent copies of the ECDI Infant Form. A subject information sheet that contained questions about the child s developmental history, language(s) spoken in the home and parents education was also included. All children chosen for the present study were acquiring Estonian as their first language. In addition, the inclusion criteria stated that the children had to be healthy (not born prematurely, without significant birth trauma and without serious medical problems according to 669

7 SCHULTS ET AL. parental report). Also, the parental report instruments had to be completed by the parent about the child s language and communicative development for the first time. Most of the participants (96. 3%) were from higher or middle SES homes (mothers having at least a high school education, that is at least twelve years of schooling). The ECDI Infant Form consists of two main parts. The first part contains a list of 347 words, organized into semantic categories (e.g. words for toys, words for people, etc.). Parents were asked to note the words that their children comprehended or comprehended and produced. We followed the approach of Bates et al. (1994); Caselli et al. (1995; 1999) and Stolt et al. (2008) in defining the categories of words. The category of COMMON NOUNS included the following parts of the ECDI Infant Form: animals names, vehicles, toys, food and drink, clothing, body parts, furniture and rooms, small household items. There were 175 common nouns, thus representing 50. 4% of the 347 words. The category of PREDICATES included verbs ( action words ) and adjective ( descriptive words ), 88 words altogether, representing 25. 4% of the words. The SOCIAL TERMS category combined sound effects and animal sounds, people, and games and routines. The social terms category had 50 words, representing 14. 4% of the words in ECDI Infant Form. The category of FUNCTION WORDS included pronouns, question words, prepositions and locations, and quantifiers, totalling 34 words, representing 9. 8% of the words in the checklist. The second part of the ECDI Infant Form consists of a list of 60 actions and gestures. Parents were asked to note the gestures and actions they had seen their child produce. From the list of 60 actions and gestures 25 items were used in this study and categorized as deictic gestures, object gestures or gestural routines. The DEICTIC GESTURES category included four gestures from the list of first communicative gestures, namely requesting, showing, giving and pointing. The OBJECT GESTURES category included 13 items, including the items from the list of actions with objects. The category of GESTURAL ROUTINES included eight items all of the first communicative gestures but the above-mentioned deictic gestures and the item Lay head on hands and squeeze eyes shut as if sleeping was included from the list of actions with objects (see Table 2). Scoring The following subscale scores were used from ECDI Infant Form: 1. Word comprehension the number of total words comprehended. We separated common nouns, predicates, social terms and function words. Numbers of words comprehended in each category were calculated as well as proportion scores. Words which were noted by parents as produced were added to the total for words comprehended. 670

8 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES TABLE 2. The items belonging to the gesture groups Deictic gestures Object gestures Gestural routines Extends arm to show you something he/she is holding Reaches out and gives you a toy or some object that he/she is holding Points (with arm and index finger extended) at some interesting object or event Requests something by extending arm and opening and closing hand Eat with a spoon or fork Drink from a cup containing liquid Comb or brush own hair Brush teeth Wipe face or hands with towel or cloth Put on hat Put on a shoe or sock Put on a necklace, bracelet or watch Blow to indicate that something is hot Put telephone to ear Sniff flowers Push a toy car or truck Throw a ball Waves bye-bye on his/her own when someone leaves Extends his/her arm upwards to signal a wish to be picked up Shakes head no Nods head yes Gestures hush by placing finger to lips Smacks lips in a yum yum gesture to indicate that something taste good Shrugs to indicate all gone or where d it go? Lay head on hands and squeeze eyes shut as if sleeping 2. Word production the numbers and proportion scores of total words, common nouns, predicates, social terms and function words produced. 3. Gestural production the number of total gestures, deictic gestures, object gestures and gestural routines produced. Analyses Age and gender differences are described. Analyses of variance and Scheffé post hoc analyses were used to this end. Pearson correlations between vocabulary comprehension, vocabulary production and gesture production are shown. In addition, models of factors predicting the production of words belonging to different categories are presented. RESULTS Table 3 presents the number of items in the ECDI and summarizes the descriptive statistics for word comprehension, word production and gesture production. According to parental reports the number of words 671

9 SCHULTS ET AL. TABLE 3. Descriptive statistics for parental report of words and gestures Measure Number of items M SD Range Comprehension Total Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Production Total Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Gesture Total Deictic gestures Object gestures Gestural routines comprehended exceeds the number of words produced by 6. 5 times. The number of words comprehended exceeded the number of words produced in all four categories: by 6. 8 times in common nouns, by times in predicates, by 3. 6 in social terms, and by times in function words. Proportions of words from different categories As to vocabulary composition, the proportions of common nouns, predicates, social terms and function words according to the size of vocabulary are shown in Figure 1 for comprehension and in Figure 2 for production. The percentages were calculated from the total number of words in the child s comprehension and production vocabulary, respectively. Comprehension. Children who were reported to comprehend no words (N=4) were omitted from this analysis. As Figure 1 shows, the proportion of social terms decreases significantly as the child starts to comprehend more words (F(1,4)= , p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 73). It decreases continuously throughout the range of vocabulary covered by the EDCI (Scheffé tests p<0. 05). At the same time, the proportions of common nouns (F(1,4)= , p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 44) and predicates (F(1,4)= , p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 44) increase. The proportion of common nouns increases until the child understands more than 200 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 001). The proportion of predicates rises until the child has started to use up to 100 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 01). Function words 672

10 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES Mean percentage (N=78) (N=118) (N=139) (N=170) Comprehension vocabulary size >200 (N=83) Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Fig. 1. The compostion of the comprehension vocabulary as the proportional function of the comprehension vocabulary size. are at a low proportional level but still rise significantly as more words are comprehended (F(1,4)=42. 64, p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 23). The proportion of function words increases until the child has started to use up to 200 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 05). Production. Children who were reported to produce no words (N=63) were omitted from this analysis. As Figure 2 shows, the children s production vocabulary increases mainly by the addition of common nouns. The proportions of common nouns (F(1,3)=81. 65, p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 32) and predicates (F(1,3)=15. 67, p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 08) increase as the child s production vocabulary grows bigger. The proportion of common nouns starts to rise after the child has started to use more than 20 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 001). Also, the proportion of predicates starts to rise after the child has started to use more than 20 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 05). The proportion of social terms decreases constantly (F(1,3)= , p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 37) until the child has started to use more than 100 words (Scheffé tests p<0. 05). Function words remain constantly at a very low proportional level at this early age (F(1,3)=1. 43, n.s.). 673

11 SCHULTS ET AL Mean percentage (N=406) (N=82) (N=29) Production vocabulary size >100 (N=12) Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Fig. 2. The compostion of the production vocabulary as the proportional function of the production vocabulary size. Inter-relatedness of vocabulary and gestures Table 4 summarizes the correlations among all of the scores of categories of vocabulary comprehension and production, and gesture production. The correlations with age partialled out are also shown. All of the significant correlations were positive. The only non-significant results were found between deictic gesture production and the production of common nouns, predicates and function words. This finding can be attributed to the ceiling effect in deictic gesture production (M=3. 4, SD=0. 9, ranging from 0 to 4). To describe the concurrent effects of the different factors (child s age, gender, birth order and maternal education, as well as the number of words from multiple categories comprehended and gestures used) on the production of words belonging to the different categories (common nouns, predicates, social terms), three models were created using Poisson regression. Poisson regression (allowing for overdispersion) was used because the dependent variables were absolute numbers that were, in essence, count data, and the distribution was very far from normality. The modelling was carried out using R version (R Development Core Team, 2009). The models for three categories of vocabulary production are shown in Table 5. The 674

12 TABLE 4. Pearson s correlations and correlations with age partialled out for vocabulary comprehension, vocabulary production and gesture production Comprehension Production Gestures 675 Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Deictic gestures Object gestures Gestural routines Comprehension Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Production Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words Gestures n.s. n.s n.s Deictic gestures Object gestures Gestural routines VOCABULARY AND GESTURES NOTE : Partial correlations are represented below the diagonal formed by the empty cells, and correlations above the diagonal. Presented correlations are significant at p<0. 05.

13 TABLE 5. Models of the factors influencing the production of vocabulary Production of common nouns Production of predicates Production of social terms b SE(b) t b SE(b) t b SE(b) t 676 Child s age *** * **** Gender x x ** Comprehension Common nouns **** **** Predicates x x4. 5**** x x0. 8 x x3. 0** Social terms **** Gestures Deictic gestures * * Object gestures *** ** ** Gestural routines * ** **** Maternal low educational level x x3. 1** x x2. 1* x x1. 7 First-born child * SCHULTS ET AL. NOTES :*p<0. 05; ** p<0. 01; *** p<0. 001; **** p< Mothers low educational level is coded as 1, higher educational levels are coded as 0.

14 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES production of function words was not modelled due to the very low numbers produced (M=0. 4, SD=1. 7, ranging from 0 to 22). As multiple factors are contributing simultaneously to the size of the child s production vocabulary, the discussion is based on the results from the regression models, not on the single correlations. If we compare the correlations with the results of regression models it is noteworthy that while all of the significant single correlations were positive, the same did not hold true for the regressions. Interpretation of the individual effects of different factors on the production of words is based on the premise that during this interpretation the effects of all of the other factors are kept at a constant level. The child s age, object gesture production and gestural routine production contribute positively to the scores of production of common nouns, predicates and social terms (see Table 4). The child s gender predicts only social term production, due to the fact that girls produce more social terms than boys. The number of common nouns comprehended by the child is positively correlated to the number of common nouns and predicates produced. The number of predicates comprehended by the child is negatively correlated with the numbers of common nouns and social terms produced. Social terms comprehension is positively correlated to social word production. As to gesture production, all of the types of gestures were positively correlated with the production of words from all of the categories with the single exception of deictic gesture production, which had no effect on predicate production. Mothers educational level had an effect on common noun and predicate production. Children from the families where mothers had at least high school education produced more common nouns and predicates than the children of less educated mothers. Birth order had an effect on common noun production as the first-born children produced more common nouns than the later-born children. The effects of the child s age and gender Detailed descriptions for the differences between age groups for numbers of words comprehended, words produced and gestures produced by categories according to the analysis of variance are presented in Figures 3 to 5. Table 6 presents the age differences in the numbers of words comprehended, words produced and gestures produced by categories. Table 7 presents the quantile scores for vocabulary comprehension and production for girls and boys separately because the girls have more words in their production vocabulary (F(1,1)=8. 11, p<0. 005, g p2 =0. 01). It can be noted that both comprehension and production vocabularies grow as the children s age grows (for comprehension vocabulary: F(1,8)=55. 04, p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 43; for production vocabulary: F(1,8)=22. 71, p<0. 001, g p2 =0. 24). 677

15 SCHULTS ET AL Comprehension vocabulary ;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 Child's age Fig. 3. The subscale scores of the comprehension vocabulary by age group. Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words DISCUSSION The main goal of the present study was to explore early communicative development in Estonian-speaking infants with an Estonian version of the CDI. We were interested in the composition of early comprehension and production vocabularies and gestural repertoire as well as the relationships between different word categories in Estonian infants comprehension vocabulary and the use of different types of gestures with the production vocabulary. Proportions of words from different categories The first purpose of the study was to describe the proportional use of different word classes in early vocabulary in Estonian children between the ages of 0; 8 and 1; 4. According to the parental reports, social terms dominate in Estonian-speaking children s vocabulary when it is smaller than 20 words and common nouns start to dominate in their vocabulary after it has grown larger than 50 words. These findings are in accord with previous findings showing that as the size of the vocabulary is less than 50 words children use proportionally more social terms (Caselli et al., 1999; Stolt et al., 2008). 678

16 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES Production vocabulary ;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 Child's age Fig. 4. The subscale scores of the production vocabulary by age group. Common nouns Predicates Social terms Function words As these words are used to refer to daily games and routines, people who are participating in the child s daily activities and sound effects, these are likely to be used often and during the times when the child pays attention to the object or activity. Also, the sound patterns of social terms tend to be phonologically repetitive, which may make it easier for the child to say these words. Our findings also support the noun bias in early vocabulary, in both comprehension and production, found in several other languages (Bornstein et al., 2004a; Caselli et al., 1995; Fenson et al., 1994; Gentner, 1982; Nelson, 1973; Stolt et al., 2008; Wehberg et al., 2007). When the child s productive vocabulary size is between 21 and 50 words, social terms and nominals are used in equal proportions. As the vocabulary increases there will be proportionally less social terms and more nominals. It might be easier for a child to learn to name a particular object than an event or to describe an object. In addition, using the parental report as a means of data gathering might be partly responsible for this finding of proportionally more common nouns in children s vocabulary as it might be easier for the parents to notice and remember the words their children use to name objects than those used to label actions or to describe objects. 679

17 SCHULTS ET AL Gestures production ;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 Child's age Fig. 5. The subscale scores of the gesture production by age group. Deictic gestures Object gestures Gestural routines The inter-relatedness of vocabulary comprehension, gestures production and vocabulary production Our second purpose was to investigate the concurrent associations between Estonian-speaking children s early gestural repertoire and comprehension vocabulary with their production vocabulary. According to the results from regression modelling, one of the findings was that the more common nouns infants comprehended the more common nouns and predicates they produced. The infant s comprehension of more social terms was positively related only to the concurrent use of more words from the same category. At the same time, the infants who comprehended more predicates were reported to produce less common nouns and social terms than the others. Maternal high use of directives has been shown to be typical of Estonian mothers (Junefelt & Tulviste, 1997; Tulviste et al., 2004). Tomasello and Kruger (1992) found that the frequent use of directives by the mother is positively related to the relatively higher proportion of verbs in the child s vocabulary. High use of directives is also correlated with talking less and expecting less verbalization from children (Tulviste et al., 2004). Mothers who have been found to talk more, to use a larger vocabulary and to expect more verbalization from their children have been shown to have children 680

18 TABLE 6. Age differences in word comprehension and production and gesture production Age 0;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 M (SD) 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 Partial g Comprehension Common * nouns (14. 2) a (17. 2) a (21. 1) b (31. 9) c (32. 9) c, a (33. 2) d, a, b (38. 7) a, b, c (38. 2) a, b, c, d (41. 7) a, b, c, d Predicates * (8. 5) a (7. 9) a (13. 6) b (17. 2) b (17. 4) b, a (15. 5) b, c, a (20. 5) d, a, b (20. 2) a, b, c (20. 9) a, b, c, d Social terms (9. 2) a (8. 7) a (8. 3) b (9. 6) c, a (9. 7) d, a, b (8. 6) e, a, b (9. 1) a, b, c (8. 3) a, b, c, d (8. 4) a, b, c, d, e 0. 39* Function words Production Common nouns * (2. 6) a (2. 9) a (4. 4) b (6. 1) b (6. 9) c, a (6. 3) c, a (7. 8) d, a, b (8. 5) a, b, c (8. 8) a, b, c, d 0. 3 (0. 7) a 0. 3 (0. 7) a 1. 1 (3. 9) a 2. 2 (3. 5) b 3. 6 (6. 2) b 4. 4 (6. 9) b (19. 4) b, a (18. 7) b, a (31. 1) a, b 0. 19* Predicates 0. 1 (0. 3) a 0 1 (0. 3) a 0 3 (0. 9) a 0 4 (0. 9) a 0 7 (1. 5) a 0 7 (1. 5) a 2 8 (6. 8) a 2 9 (5. 3) a 6 4 (10. 2) a 0 16* Social terms 1. 4 (1. 9) a 2 1 (2. 4) a 3 5 (2. 9) b 4 7 (3. 9) b 6 5 (4. 8) c, a 6 9 (5. 0) c, a 9 0 (7. 4) d, a, b 10 9 (6. 8) a, b, c 15 0 (9. 5) a, b, c, d 0 35* Function * words (0. 1) a (0. 1) a (0. 2) a (0. 2) a (1. 1) a (0. 4) a (2. 6) (1. 4) (3. 6) a Gesture Deictic * gestures (1. 2) a (1. 3) a (0. 8) b, a (0. 6) a (0. 6) a (0. 6) a (0. 5) a, b (0. 4) a, b (0. 2) a, b Object gestures 2. 2 (1. 7) a 2. 5 (1. 8) a 4. 5 (2. 3) b, a 5. 3 (2. 4) c, a 6. 8 (2. 5) d, a, b 8. 2 (2. 2) e, a, b, c 8. 8 (2. 5) e, a, b, c, d (2. 0) a, b, c, d, e (1. 8) a, b, c, d, e 0. 65* Gestural routines * (1. 2) a (1. 5) b (1. 3) b (1. 7) c, a (1. 7) c, a, b (1. 6) d, a, b (1. 6) a, b, c (1. 6) a, b, c, d (1. 6) a, b, c, d VOCABULARY AND GESTURES NOTE : Significant (p<0. 05) differences between age groups according to ANOVA post hoc comparisons (Scheffé test) are indicated with the superscript letters and the same letters with apostrophe. * p<

19 TABLE 7. Percentile scores for children s vocabulary comprehension and production Boy s age Girl s age 682 0;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 0;8 0;9 0;10 0;11 1;0 1;1 1;2 1;3 1;4 (28) (31) (33) (25) (44) (40) (34) (43) (36) (34) (24) (28) (24) (30) (35) (39) (34) (30) Comprehension 75% % % Production 75% % % SCHULTS ET AL.

20 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES with larger comprehension and production vocabularies (Hoff, 2006). Thus it might be plausible that while a child is being exposed to more words from the predicates category (i.e. verbs and adjectives), the exposure to the words from other categories is diminished, which might be one possible explanation for the negative correlation between comprehension of predicates and production of social terms and nominals found in this study. It is noteworthy that comprehension of words belonging to different categories can be correlated positively as well as negatively with the production of different types of words. These findings indicate the importance of considering specific types of words rather than whole vocabulary while searching for predictors of production vocabulary development. Another important finding was that gesture production was positively linked to the production vocabulary as expected. The infants with more gestures had larger production vocabularies. Somewhat unexpected was the finding that a positive link exists regardless of the categories of gestures and words (except for the lack of association between the production of deictic gestures and predicates). The finding that object gestures are positively related to predicate and social term production as well as to common noun production was unexpected because in previous research it has been shown that object gestures are positively related to noun production (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Bates & Dick, 2002; Bates et al., 1989; Bretherton et al., 1981; Iverson et al., 1994; Nelson, 1973). The results showed that the type of words and gestures has to be taken into account when studying the associations between early gestures and comprehension with concurrent productive vocabulary. The effects of background factors on vocabulary The third goal of the study was to explore the role of the child s age, gender and birth order and maternal education in the size of an infant s production vocabulary. As expected, the older the children were the more words they comprehended and produced, and this finding held for all of the word categories. Up to the age of 0;10 children comprehend and produce very few words. From that point onwards the number of words comprehended and produced starts to increase in all of the word categories except the function words. According to the data from parental reports the number of common nouns comprehended increases more rapidly than the number of words in other categories. At the same time the reported number of common nouns produced exceeds the number of social terms produced only at the age of 1; 4. Older children produced more of each type of gestures compared to the younger children, in this particular age range. Production of deictic gestures reaches ceiling at the age of 0;11, whereas the production of object gestures increases rapidly throughout this age range 683

21 SCHULTS ET AL. and the production of gestural routines continues to grow at a slower rate as well. Our conclusions here are based on the regression models that showed the effects of the background variables on the production of words from different categories. Several researchers have suggested that girls have slightly larger vocabularies than boys (Bornstein et al., 2004b; Eriksson et al., in press; Fenson et al., 1994). The current study shows that this also holds true in Estonian-speaking children. Girls did have more words in their productive vocabulary (but not in their comprehension vocabulary) as compared to boys. As to the specific word groups, according to our data, at this early age girls had an advantage over boys only in the production of social terms. It is likely that an advantage for girls did not emerge in common nouns or predicates because our participants were very young (between the ages of 0;8 and 1;4.) and only 31% of them had a production vocabulary larger than 20 words. Future studies should establish whether there are more common nouns and/or more predicates in the girls productive vocabulary as compared to the boys after the 50-word milestone has been reached. As to birth order, our study partly replicated previous findings. We found as expected an advantage for first-born children as compared to the later-borns in productive vocabulary but only in the use of common nouns (Goldfield & Reznick, 1990). It has been pointed out in previous studies that later-born children have better conversational skills as compared to first-borns (Fenson et al., 1994; Bornstein et al., 2004c). We did not find an advantage for later-born children in the production of any word category. The lack of any advantage for later-borns in this study might be due to the fact that there was no specific word category that might be linked to the development of overall conversational skills. Before children s second birthday, SES-related differences do not emerge consistently (Fenson et al., 1994). After reaching the age of 2; 0 toddlers of more educated mothers produce slightly more words compared to the toddlers of less educated mothers (Hoff, 2006; Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, 2009a). Our study found that infants aged 0;8 to 1;4 of more educated mothers produced more common nouns and predicates as compared to the infants of less educated mothers. Finally, the study has some limitations. The ECDI is a parent report instrument and not a direct measure of child language. It is possible that some mothers overestimate their children s overall gesture repertoire or the size of their vocabulary, particularly when reporting words as being understood by young children. To improve our understanding of the developmental links between comprehension and production of different types of words and production of different types of gestures longitudinal data and data gathered with different methods are needed. 684

22 VOCABULARY AND GESTURES REFERENCES Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59, Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from 9 13 months: Correlational findings. In E. Bates (ed.), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy, New York: Academic Press. Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, Bates, E. & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology 40, Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J. (1994). Development and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21, Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L. & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology 25, Blake, J. & Dology, S. J. (1993). Gestural development and its relation to cognition during the transisiton to language. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 17, Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L., Pêcheux, M.-G., Ruel, J., Venuti, P. & Vyt, A. (2004a). Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development 75, Bornstein, M. C., Hahn, C.-S. & Haynes, O. M. (2004b). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language 24, Bornstein, M. C., Leach, D. B. & Haynes, O. M. (2004c). Vocabulary competence in first- and secondborn siblings of the same chronological age. Journal of Child Language 31, Bretherton, I., Bates, E., McNew, S., Shore, C., Williamson, C. & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Comprehension and production of symbols in infancy: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology 17, Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L. & Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development 10, Caselli, C., Casadio, P. & Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from first words to grammar in English and Italian. Journal of Child Language 26, Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., Marjanovič Umek, L., Gayraud, F., Kovacevic, M. & Gallego, C. (in press). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59, Fernald, A. & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variation in Japanese and American mothers speech to infants. Child Development 64, Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuczaj (ed.), Language development : Vol.2 Language, thought and culture, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Goldfield, B. A. & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, Goodwyn, S. & Acredolo, L. (1993). Symbolic gesture versus word: Is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Child Development 64, Harris, M., Barlow-Brown, F. & Chasin, J. (1995). The emergence of referential understanding : Pointing and the comprehension of object names. First Language 15,

23 SCHULTS ET AL. Hart, B. (2004). What toddlers talk about. First Language 24, Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9, Junefelt, K. & Tulviste, T. (1997). Regulation and praise in America, Estonian, and Swedish mother child interaction. Mind, Culture, and Activity 4, McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind what gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38. R Development Core Team (2009). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009a). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323, Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009b). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science 12, Schults, A. & Tulviste, T. (2008). The Estonian adaptation of MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures. Unpublished ms, University of Tartu, Estonia. Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H. & Lehtonen, L. (2008). Early lexical development of Finnish children: A longitudinal study. First Language 28, Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers early vocabulary. Developmental Psychology 32, Tomasello, M. & Kruger, C. A. (1992). Joint attention on actions: Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language 19, Tulviste, T., Mizera, L. & De Geer, B. (2004). Expressing communicative intents in Estonian, Finnish, and Swedish mother adolescent interactions. Journal of Child Language 4, Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O. & Pizzuto, E. (2005). Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In M. Tomasello & D. I. Slobin (eds), Beyond nature nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wehberg, S., Vach, W., Bleses, D., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O. & Basbøll, H. (2007). Danish children s first words: Analyzing longitudinal data based on monthly CDI parental reports. First Language 27,

Downloaded on T18:40:04Z. Title. Using parent report to assess early lexical production in children exposed to more than one language

Downloaded on T18:40:04Z. Title. Using parent report to assess early lexical production in children exposed to more than one language Title Author(s) Editor(s) Using parent report to assess early lexical production in children exposed to more than one language Gatt, Daniela; O'Toole, Ciara; Haman, Ewa Armon-Lotem, Sharon de Jong, Jan

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 35 (2012) 773 778 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Twelve-month-olds vocal production during pointing in naturalistic interactions:

More information

Miguel Pérez-Pereira. University of Santiago de Compostela (Spain) Sections: 1. Introduction. 1.1 Previous studies. 1.2 Aims of the study

Miguel Pérez-Pereira. University of Santiago de Compostela (Spain) Sections: 1. Introduction. 1.1 Previous studies. 1.2 Aims of the study Early Galician/Spanish bilingualism: contrasts with monolingualism. Miguel Pérez-Pereira University of Santiago de Compostela (Spain) Sections: 1. Introduction 1.1 Previous studies 1.2 Aims of the study

More information

Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam

Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam Accessing Higher Education in Developing Countries: panel data analysis from India, Peru and Vietnam Alan Sanchez (GRADE) y Abhijeet Singh (UCL) 12 de Agosto, 2017 Introduction Higher education in developing

More information

2 months: Social and Emotional Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent

2 months: Social and Emotional Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent 2 months: Begins to smile at people Can briefly calm self (may bring hands to mouth and suck on hand) Tries to look at parent Coos, makes gurgling sounds Turns head toward sounds Pays attention to faces

More information

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed.

Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed. Teachers: Use this checklist periodically to keep track of the progress indicators that your learners have displayed. Speaking Standard Language Aspect: Purpose and Context Benchmark S1.1 To exit this

More information

Joint Book Reading in the Second Year and Vocabulary Outcomes

Joint Book Reading in the Second Year and Vocabulary Outcomes Journal of Research in Childhood Education 2007, Vol. 21, No. 3 Copyright 2007 by the Association for Childhood Education International 0256-8543/07 Joint Book Reading in the Second Year and Vocabulary

More information

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg

Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg Tracy Dudek & Jenifer Russell Trinity Services, Inc. *Copyright 2008, Mark L. Sundberg Verbal Behavior-Milestones Assessment & Placement Program Criterion-referenced assessment tool Guides goals and objectives/benchmark

More information

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW

ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW ROSETTA STONE PRODUCT OVERVIEW Method Rosetta Stone teaches languages using a fully-interactive immersion process that requires the student to indicate comprehension of the new language and provides immediate

More information

Developing Grammar in Context

Developing Grammar in Context Developing Grammar in Context intermediate with answers Mark Nettle and Diana Hopkins PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United

More information

Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON. Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon. Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe. Indiana University

Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON. Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon. Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe. Indiana University Fast Mapping 1 Running head: FAST MAPPING SKILLS IN THE DEVELOPING LEXICON Fast Mapping Skills in the Developing Lexicon Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe Indiana University Erin R. Hahn Furman University Fast Mapping

More information

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus

Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter Lexical Categories. Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus Language Acquisition Fall 2010/Winter 2011 Lexical Categories Afra Alishahi, Heiner Drenhaus Computational Linguistics and Phonetics Saarland University Children s Sensitivity to Lexical Categories Look,

More information

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith

NAME: East Carolina University PSYC Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith Module 10 1 NAME: East Carolina University PSYC 3206 -- Developmental Psychology Dr. Eppler & Dr. Ironsmith Study Questions for Chapter 10: Language and Education Sigelman & Rider (2009). Life-span human

More information

JSLHR. Research Article. Lexical Characteristics of Expressive Vocabulary in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder

JSLHR. Research Article. Lexical Characteristics of Expressive Vocabulary in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder JSLHR Research Article Lexical Characteristics of Expressive Vocabulary in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder Sara T. Kover a and Susan Ellis Weismer a Purpose: Vocabulary is a domain of particular

More information

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF) in Language and Literacy 1 Desired Results Developmental Profile (2015) [DRDP (2015)] Correspondence to California Foundations: Language and Development (LLD) and the Foundations (PLF) The Language and Development (LLD) domain

More information

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds

Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds Linking object names and object categories: Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization in 6- and 12-month-olds Anne L. Fulkerson 1, Sandra R. Waxman 2, and Jennifer M. Seymour 1 1 University

More information

Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention

Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention Shared Book Reading between Mother and Infant Facilitates The Frequency of Joint Attention Ayumi Sato (ayusatotenjin@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters Doshisha University,

More information

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Tyler Perrachione LING 451-0 Proseminar in Sound Structure Prof. A. Bradlow 17 March 2006 Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Abstract Although the acoustic and

More information

Miami-Dade County Public Schools

Miami-Dade County Public Schools ENGLISH LANGUAGE LEARNERS AND THEIR ACADEMIC PROGRESS: 2010-2011 Author: Aleksandr Shneyderman, Ed.D. January 2012 Research Services Office of Assessment, Research, and Data Analysis 1450 NE Second Avenue,

More information

A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion

A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion Words Known A Stochastic Model for the Vocabulary Explosion Colleen C. Mitchell (colleen-mitchell@uiowa.edu) Department of Mathematics, 225E MLH Iowa City, IA 52242 USA Bob McMurray (bob-mcmurray@uiowa.edu)

More information

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning

Age Effects on Syntactic Control in. Second Language Learning Age Effects on Syntactic Control in Second Language Learning Miriam Tullgren Loyola University Chicago Abstract 1 This paper explores the effects of age on second language acquisition in adolescents, ages

More information

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin

Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin Stromswold & Rifkin, Language Acquisition by MZ & DZ SLI Twins (SRCLD, 1996) 1 Language Acquisition by Identical vs. Fraternal SLI Twins * Karin Stromswold & Jay I. Rifkin Dept. of Psychology & Ctr. for

More information

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade

GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade Assessment Alignment of GOLD Objectives for Development & Learning: Birth Through Third Grade WITH , Birth Through Third Grade aligned to Arizona Early Learning Standards Grade: Ages 3-5 - Adopted: 2013

More information

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales

CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency Scales CEFR Overall Illustrative English Proficiency s CEFR CEFR OVERALL ORAL PRODUCTION Has a good command of idiomatic expressions and colloquialisms with awareness of connotative levels of meaning. Can convey

More information

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning

Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning Degeneracy results in canalisation of language structure: A computational model of word learning Padraic Monaghan (p.monaghan@lancaster.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Lancaster University Lancaster LA1

More information

Elizabeth R. Crais, Ph.D., CCC-SLP

Elizabeth R. Crais, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Elizabeth R. Crais, Ph.D., CCC-SLP Division of Speech & Hearing Sciences Medical School The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Indiana Speech-Language-Hearing Association April 5, 2013 Linda Watson,

More information

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services

Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services Normal Language Development Community Paediatric Audiology Cambridgeshire Community Services NHS Trust: delivering excellence in children and young people s health services Language develops unconsciously

More information

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8

California Department of Education English Language Development Standards for Grade 8 Section 1: Goal, Critical Principles, and Overview Goal: English learners read, analyze, interpret, and create a variety of literary and informational text types. They develop an understanding of how language

More information

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017

GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017 GERM 3040 GERMAN GRAMMAR AND COMPOSITION SPRING 2017 Instructor: Dr. Claudia Schwabe Class hours: TR 9:00-10:15 p.m. claudia.schwabe@usu.edu Class room: Old Main 301 Office: Old Main 002D Office hours:

More information

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK

THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK THE HEAD START CHILD OUTCOMES FRAMEWORK Released in 2000, the Head Start Child Outcomes Framework is intended to guide Head Start programs in their curriculum planning and ongoing assessment of the progress

More information

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES

AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES AN ANALYSIS OF GRAMMTICAL ERRORS MADE BY THE SECOND YEAR STUDENTS OF SMAN 5 PADANG IN WRITING PAST EXPERIENCES Yelna Oktavia 1, Lely Refnita 1,Ernati 1 1 English Department, the Faculty of Teacher Training

More information

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University

The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical. Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University The Effect of Extensive Reading on Developing the Grammatical Accuracy of the EFL Freshmen at Al Al-Bayt University Kifah Rakan Alqadi Al Al-Bayt University Faculty of Arts Department of English Language

More information

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics Delphine Sasanguie 1. Introduction Mapping hypothesis Innate approximate representation of number (ANS) Symbols

More information

Gestures in Communication through Line Graphs

Gestures in Communication through Line Graphs Gestures in Communication through Line Graphs Cengiz Acartürk (ACARTURK@Metu.Edu.Tr) Özge Alaçam (OZGE@Metu.Edu.Tr) Cognitive Science, Informatics Institute Middle East Technical University, 06800, Ankara,

More information

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts. Recommendation 1 Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts. Students come to kindergarten with a rudimentary understanding of basic fraction

More information

One Stop Shop For Educators

One Stop Shop For Educators Modern Languages Level II Course Description One Stop Shop For Educators The Level II language course focuses on the continued development of communicative competence in the target language and understanding

More information

IMPLEMENTING THE EARLY YEARS LEARNING FRAMEWORK

IMPLEMENTING THE EARLY YEARS LEARNING FRAMEWORK IMPLEMENTING THE EARLY YEARS LEARNING FRAMEWORK A focus on Dr Jean Ashton Faculty of Education and Social Work Conclusive national and international research evidence shows that the first five years of

More information

ROA Technical Report. Jaap Dronkers ROA-TR-2014/1. Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market ROA

ROA Technical Report. Jaap Dronkers ROA-TR-2014/1. Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market ROA Research Centre for Education and the Labour Market ROA Parental background, early scholastic ability, the allocation into secondary tracks and language skills at the age of 15 years in a highly differentiated

More information

Advanced Grammar in Use

Advanced Grammar in Use Advanced Grammar in Use A self-study reference and practice book for advanced learners of English Third Edition with answers and CD-ROM cambridge university press cambridge, new york, melbourne, madrid,

More information

Language Acquisition Chart

Language Acquisition Chart Language Acquisition Chart This chart was designed to help teachers better understand the process of second language acquisition. Please use this chart as a resource for learning more about the way people

More information

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature

1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature 1 st Grade Curriculum Map Common Core Standards Language Arts 2013 2014 1 st Quarter (September, October, November) August/September Strand Topic Standard Notes Reading for Literature Key Ideas and Details

More information

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams

Language-Specific Patterns in Danish and Zapotec Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams Language-Specific Patterns in and Children s Comprehension of Spatial Grams Kristine Jensen de López University of Aalborg, Denmark Kristine@hum.auc.dk 1 Introduction Existing cross-linguistic studies

More information

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) Feb 2015

Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL)  Feb 2015 Author: Justyna Kowalczys Stowarzyszenie Angielski w Medycynie (PL) www.angielskiwmedycynie.org.pl Feb 2015 Developing speaking abilities is a prerequisite for HELP in order to promote effective communication

More information

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek

The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in Early Greek Vol. 4 (2012) 15-25 University of Reading ISSN 2040-3461 LANGUAGE STUDIES WORKING PAPERS Editors: C. Ciarlo and D.S. Giannoni The Acquisition of Person and Number Morphology Within the Verbal Domain in

More information

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity

An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity An Empirical and Computational Test of Linguistic Relativity Kathleen M. Eberhard* (eberhard.1@nd.edu) Matthias Scheutz** (mscheutz@cse.nd.edu) Michael Heilman** (mheilman@nd.edu) *Department of Psychology,

More information

Eliciting Language in the Classroom. Presented by: Dionne Ramey, SBCUSD SLP Amanda Drake, SBCUSD Special Ed. Program Specialist

Eliciting Language in the Classroom. Presented by: Dionne Ramey, SBCUSD SLP Amanda Drake, SBCUSD Special Ed. Program Specialist Eliciting Language in the Classroom Presented by: Dionne Ramey, SBCUSD SLP Amanda Drake, SBCUSD Special Ed. Program Specialist Classroom Language: What we anticipate Students are expected to arrive with

More information

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities.

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities. The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities By Erica Blouin Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications

Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications Formulaic Language and Fluency: ESL Teaching Applications Formulaic Language Terminology Formulaic sequence One such item Formulaic language Non-count noun referring to these items Phraseology The study

More information

Cognitive Development Facilitator s Guide

Cognitive Development Facilitator s Guide Cognitive Development Facilitator s Guide Competency-Based Learning Objectives Description of Target Audience Training Methodologies/ Strategies Utilized Sequence of Training By the end of this module,

More information

Effects of connecting reading and writing and a checklist to guide the reading process on EFL learners learning about English writing

Effects of connecting reading and writing and a checklist to guide the reading process on EFL learners learning about English writing Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 1 (2009) 1871 1883 World Conference on Educational Sciences 2009 Effects of connecting reading and writing and a checklist

More information

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark

Subject: Opening the American West. What are you teaching? Explorations of Lewis and Clark Theme 2: My World & Others (Geography) Grade 5: Lewis and Clark: Opening the American West by Ellen Rodger (U.S. Geography) This 4MAT lesson incorporates activities in the Daily Lesson Guide (DLG) that

More information

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many

A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching. In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one. There are many Schmidt 1 Eric Schmidt Prof. Suzanne Flynn Linguistic Study of Bilingualism December 13, 2013 A Minimalist Approach to Code-Switching In the field of linguistics, the topic of bilingualism is a broad one.

More information

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on Developmental Science DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00627.x REPORT Blackwell Publishing Ltd Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on processing speed visual processing Christopher W. Robinson

More information

Writing a composition

Writing a composition A good composition has three elements: Writing a composition an introduction: A topic sentence which contains the main idea of the paragraph. a body : Supporting sentences that develop the main idea. a

More information

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397,

Dyslexia/dyslexic, 3, 9, 24, 97, 187, 189, 206, 217, , , 367, , , 397, Adoption studies, 274 275 Alliteration skill, 113, 115, 117 118, 122 123, 128, 136, 138 Alphabetic writing system, 5, 40, 127, 136, 410, 415 Alphabets (types of ) artificial transparent alphabet, 5 German

More information

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National

Philosophy of Literacy Education. Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National Philosophy of Literacy Education Becoming literate is a complex step by step process that begins at birth. The National Association for Young Children explains, Even in the first few months of life, children

More information

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6

Language Development: The Components of Language. How Children Develop. Chapter 6 How Children Develop Language Acquisition: Part I Chapter 6 What is language? Creative or generative Structured Referential Species-Specific Units of Language Language Development: The Components of Language

More information

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level

Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level LSHSS Clinical Exchange Clinical Application of the Mean Babbling Level and Syllable Structure Level Sherrill R. Morris Northern Illinois University, DeKalb T here is a documented synergy between development

More information

Linking the Ohio State Assessments to NWEA MAP Growth Tests *

Linking the Ohio State Assessments to NWEA MAP Growth Tests * Linking the Ohio State Assessments to NWEA MAP Growth Tests * *As of June 2017 Measures of Academic Progress (MAP ) is known as MAP Growth. August 2016 Introduction Northwest Evaluation Association (NWEA

More information

24 months. Maryhan Baker (1, 2) * Graham Schafer (1) Katie J Alcock (3) Shelley Bartlett (1, 4)

24 months. Maryhan Baker (1, 2) * Graham Schafer (1) Katie J Alcock (3) Shelley Bartlett (1, 4) 1 A parentally-administered cognitive development assessment for children from 10 to 24 months Maryhan Baker (1, 2) * Graham Schafer (1) Katie J Alcock (3) Shelley Bartlett (1, 4) (1) University of Reading,

More information

Science Clubs as a Vehicle to Enhance Science Teaching and Learning in Schools

Science Clubs as a Vehicle to Enhance Science Teaching and Learning in Schools 2014 4th International Conference on Education, Research and Innovation IPEDR vol.81 (2014) (2014) IACSIT Press, Singapore DOI: 10.7763/IPEDR.2014.V81.26 Science Clubs as a Vehicle to Enhance Science Teaching

More information

More ESL Teaching Ideas

More ESL Teaching Ideas More ESL Teaching Ideas Grades 1-8 Written by Anne Moore and Dana Pilling Illustrated by Tom Riddolls, Alicia Macdonald About the authors: Anne Moore is a certified teacher with a specialist certification

More information

ANALYSIS: LABOUR MARKET SUCCESS OF VOCATIONAL AND HIGHER EDUCATION GRADUATES

ANALYSIS: LABOUR MARKET SUCCESS OF VOCATIONAL AND HIGHER EDUCATION GRADUATES ANALYSIS: LABOUR MARKET SUCCESS OF VOCATIONAL AND HIGHER EDUCATION GRADUATES Authors: Ingrid Jaggo, Mart Reinhold & Aune Valk, Analysis Department of the Ministry of Education and Research I KEY CONCLUSIONS

More information

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools

Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools Listening and Speaking Skills of English Language of Adolescents of Government and Private Schools Dr. Amardeep Kaur Professor, Babe Ke College of Education, Mudki, Ferozepur, Punjab Abstract The present

More information

Typing versus thinking aloud when reading: Implications for computer-based assessment and training tools

Typing versus thinking aloud when reading: Implications for computer-based assessment and training tools Behavior Research Methods 2006, 38 (2), 211-217 Typing versus thinking aloud when reading: Implications for computer-based assessment and training tools BRENTON MUÑOZ, JOSEPH P. MAGLIANO, and ROBIN SHERIDAN

More information

2012 ACT RESULTS BACKGROUND

2012 ACT RESULTS BACKGROUND Report from the Office of Student Assessment 31 November 29, 2012 2012 ACT RESULTS AUTHOR: Douglas G. Wren, Ed.D., Assessment Specialist Department of Educational Leadership and Assessment OTHER CONTACT

More information

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading

ELA/ELD Standards Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading ELA/ELD Correlation Matrix for ELD Materials Grade 1 Reading The English Language Arts (ELA) required for the one hour of English-Language Development (ELD) Materials are listed in Appendix 9-A, Matrix

More information

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1 Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1 In Press at Memory & Cognition Effects of Delay of Prospective Memory Cues in an Ongoing Task on Prospective Memory Task Performance Dawn M. McBride, Jaclyn

More information

CS 598 Natural Language Processing

CS 598 Natural Language Processing CS 598 Natural Language Processing Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere Natural language is everywhere!"#$%&'&()*+,-./012 34*5665756638/9:;< =>?@ABCDEFGHIJ5KL@

More information

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5-

Reading Grammar Section and Lesson Writing Chapter and Lesson Identify a purpose for reading W1-LO; W2- LO; W3- LO; W4- LO; W5- New York Grade 7 Core Performance Indicators Grades 7 8: common to all four ELA standards Throughout grades 7 and 8, students demonstrate the following core performance indicators in the key ideas of reading,

More information

Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11)

Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11) Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3-11 Project (EPPE 3-11) A longitudinal study funded by the DfES (2003 2008) Exploring pupils views of primary school in Year 5 Address for correspondence: EPPSE

More information

5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay

5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay 5 Star Writing Persuasive Essay Grades 5-6 Intro paragraph states position and plan Multiparagraphs Organized At least 3 reasons Explanations, Examples, Elaborations to support reasons Arguments/Counter

More information

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs

Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs Analyzing Linguistically Appropriate IEP Goals in Dual Language Programs 2016 Dual Language Conference: Making Connections Between Policy and Practice March 19, 2016 Framingham, MA Session Description

More information

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards

First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards First Grade Curriculum Highlights: In alignment with the Common Core Standards ENGLISH LANGUAGE ARTS Foundational Skills Print Concepts Demonstrate understanding of the organization and basic features

More information

Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer

Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice Volume 7 Issue 2 Article 6 July 213 Sector Differences in Student Learning: Differences in Achievement Gains Across School Years and During the Summer

More information

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2

BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 INTRODUCTION TO THE BULATS A2 WORDLIST 2 The BULATS A2 WORDLIST 21 is a list of approximately 750 words to help candidates aiming at an A2 pass in the Cambridge BULATS exam. It is

More information

Sample Goals and Benchmarks

Sample Goals and Benchmarks Sample Goals and Benchmarks for Students with Hearing Loss In this document, you will find examples of potential goals and benchmarks for each area. Please note that these are just examples. You should

More information

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I RP7-1 Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I Pages 46 48 Standards: 7.RP.A. Goals: Students will write equivalent statements for proportions by keeping track of the part and the whole, and by

More information

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume? Difficulty of Interruptions 1 Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume? David M. Cades Deborah A. Boehm Davis J. Gregory Trafton Naval Research Laboratory Christopher A. Monk

More information

BSID-II-NL project. Heidelberg March Selma Ruiter, University of Groningen

BSID-II-NL project. Heidelberg March Selma Ruiter, University of Groningen BSID-II-NL project Heidelberg March 2006 Selma Ruiter, University of Groningen BSID-II-NL project Dutch standardization and validation project Important alterations Two results of psychometric studies

More information

The St. Marylebone Church of England Bridge School

The St. Marylebone Church of England Bridge School The St. Marylebone Church of England Bridge School Home/School Agreement 2016 / 17 HOME / SCHOOL AGREEMENT The St. Marylebone CE Bridge School 17 23 Third Avenue, London W10 4RS MISSION STATEMENT The St.

More information

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN

YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN YMCA SCHOOL AGE CHILD CARE PROGRAM PLAN (normal view is landscape, not portrait) SCHOOL AGE DOMAIN SKILLS ARE SOCIAL: COMMUNICATION, LANGUAGE AND LITERACY: EMOTIONAL: COGNITIVE: PHYSICAL: DEVELOPMENTAL

More information

ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS

ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS RESEARCH ARTICLE ROLE OF SELF-ESTEEM IN ENGLISH SPEAKING SKILLS IN ADOLESCENT LEARNERS NAVITA Lecturer in English Govt. Sr. Sec. School, Raichand Wala, Jind, Haryana ABSTRACT The aim of this study was

More information

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex

CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1. Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex CHILDREN S POSSESSIVE STRUCTURES: A CASE STUDY 1 Andrew Radford and Joseph Galasso, University of Essex 1998 Two-and three-year-old children generally go through a stage during which they sporadically

More information

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing

The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of English Article Usage in L2 Writing Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Volume 3, Issue 1, 2016, pp. 110-120 Available online at www.jallr.com ISSN: 2376-760X The Effect of Written Corrective Feedback on the Accuracy of

More information

Guided Reading with A SPECIAL DAY written and illustrated by Anne Sibley O Brien

Guided Reading with A SPECIAL DAY written and illustrated by Anne Sibley O Brien Bebop Books Page 1 Guided Reading with A SPECIAL DAY written and illustrated by Anne Sibley O Brien Realistic Fiction Guided Reading : D DRA: 4 Reading Recovery : 6 12 pages, 75 words Focus: using picture

More information

SALAMAH COLLEGE A PARENTS GUIDE TO SCHOOL UNIFORM

SALAMAH COLLEGE A PARENTS GUIDE TO SCHOOL UNIFORM SALAMAH COLLEGE A PARENTS GUIDE TO SCHOOL UNIFORM Introduction: At Salamah College our uniform is a sign of our community. It reflects the School Vision Statement and goals. All families sign as part of

More information

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4

English for Life. B e g i n n e r. Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started. Student s Book 3 Date. Workbook. MultiROM. Test 1 4 Lessons 1 4 Checklist Getting Started Lesson 1 Lesson 2 Lesson 3 Lesson 4 Introducing yourself Numbers 0 10 Names Indefinite articles: a / an this / that Useful expressions Classroom language Imperatives

More information

Paraprofessional Evaluation: School Year:

Paraprofessional Evaluation: School Year: Paraprofessional Evaluation: School Year: 2014-2015 Name Evaluator Contributing Evaluator Program Grade Site Observat ion Date: Observation Date Post-Conference Date Additional Observation Date-As Needed

More information

W O R L D L A N G U A G E S

W O R L D L A N G U A G E S W O R L D L A N G U A G E S Life in a global community has heightened awareness as to the value of and the need for effective communication in two or more languages. The World Languages Department believes

More information

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today!

Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Reading Rods offer many outstanding features! Read on to discover how to put Reading Rods to work today! Dear Teacher: Welcome to Reading Rods! Your Sentence Building Reading Rod Set contains 156 interlocking plastic Rods printed with words representing different parts of speech and punctuation marks. Students

More information

CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA

CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA CHAPTER 5: COMPARABILITY OF WRITTEN QUESTIONNAIRE DATA AND INTERVIEW DATA Virginia C. Mueller Gathercole As a supplement to the interviews, we also sent out written questionnaires, to gauge the generality

More information

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel

L1 and L2 acquisition. Holger Diessel L1 and L2 acquisition Holger Diessel Schedule Comparing L1 and L2 acquisition The role of the native language in L2 acquisition The critical period hypothesis [student presentation] Non-linguistic factors

More information

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs

Greek Teachers Attitudes toward the Inclusion of Students with Special Educational Needs American Journal of Educational Research, 2014, Vol. 2, No. 4, 208-218 Available online at http://pubs.sciepub.com/education/2/4/6 Science and Education Publishing DOI:10.12691/education-2-4-6 Greek Teachers

More information

GDP Falls as MBA Rises?

GDP Falls as MBA Rises? Applied Mathematics, 2013, 4, 1455-1459 http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/am.2013.410196 Published Online October 2013 (http://www.scirp.org/journal/am) GDP Falls as MBA Rises? T. N. Cummins EconomicGPS, Aurora,

More information

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar:

MERRY CHRISTMAS Level: 5th year of Primary Education Grammar: Level: 5 th year of Primary Education Grammar: Present Simple Tense. Sentence word order (Present Simple). Imperative forms. Functions: Expressing habits and routines. Describing customs and traditions.

More information

DOI /cog Cognitive Linguistics 2013; 24(2):

DOI /cog Cognitive Linguistics 2013; 24(2): DOI 10.1515/cog-2013-0010 Cognitive Linguistics 2013; 24(2): 309 343 Irit Meir, Carol Padden, Mark Aronoff and Wendy Sandler Competing iconicities in the structure of languages Abstract: The paper examines

More information

Turkish- and German-speaking bilingual 4-to-6-year-olds living in Sweden: Effects of age, SES and home language input on vocabulary production

Turkish- and German-speaking bilingual 4-to-6-year-olds living in Sweden: Effects of age, SES and home language input on vocabulary production Journal of Home Language Research (JHLR) Volume 1, 2016, pages 17-41 http://hdl.handle.net/10092/12907 Turkish- and German-speaking bilingual 4-to-6-year-olds living in Sweden: Effects of age, SES and

More information

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative

Opportunities for Writing Title Key Stage 1 Key Stage 2 Narrative English Teaching Cycle The English curriculum at Wardley CE Primary is based upon the National Curriculum. Our English is taught through a text based curriculum as we believe this is the best way to develop

More information

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996.

Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint Conference, Singapore, November, 1996. THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-CONCEPT IN YOUNG CHILDREN: PRESCHOOLERS' VIEWS OF THEIR COMPETENCE AND ACCEPTANCE Christine Johnston, Faculty of Nursing, University of Sydney Paper presented at the ERA-AARE Joint

More information