Metacognition and the spacing effect: the role of repetition, feedback, and instruction on judgments of learning for massed and spaced rehearsal

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Metacognition and the spacing effect: the role of repetition, feedback, and instruction on judgments of learning for massed and spaced rehearsal"

Transcription

1 Metacognition Learning (2012) 7: DOI /s Metacognition and the spacing effect: the role of repetition, feedback, and instruction on judgments of learning for massed and spaced rehearsal Jessica M. Logan & Alan D. Castel & Sara Haber & Emily J. Viehman Received: 10 April 2012 / Accepted: 17 September 2012 / Published online: 26 September 2012 # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012 Abstract Although memory performance benefits from the spacing of information at encoding, judgments of learning (JOLs) are often not sensitive to the benefits of spacing. The present research examines how practice, feedback, and instruction influence JOLs for spaced and massed items. In Experiment 1, in which JOLs were made after the presentation of each item and participants were given multiple study-test cycles, JOLs were strongly influenced by the repetition of the items, but there was little difference in JOLs for massed versus spaced items. A similar effect was shown in Experiments 2 and 3, inwhich participants scored their own recall performance and were given feedback, although participants did learn to assign higher JOLs to spaced items with task experience. In Experiment 4, after participants were given direct instruction about the benefits of spacing, they showed a greater difference for JOLs of spaced vs massed items, but their JOLs still underestimated their recall for spaced items. Although spacing effects are very robust and have important implications for memory and education, people often underestimate the benefits of spaced repetition when learning, possibly due to the reliance on processing fluency during study and attending to repetition, and not taking into account the beneficial aspects of study schedule. Keywords Metamemory. Memory. Spacing effects. Judgments of learning Author Note We would like to thank David Balota, Aaron Benjamin, Bob Bjork, Elizabeth Bjork, Nate Kornell, David McCabe and Matthew Rhodes for valuable comments at various points during this project. Pooja Agarwal was extremely helpful in collecting and analyzing data and providing insight. J. M. Logan (*) : E. J. Viehman Department of Psychology, Rice University, P.O. Box 1892, MS-25, Houston, TX , USA Jessica.Logan@rice.edu A. D. Castel (*) Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, 1285 Franz Hall Box , Los Angeles, CA , USA castel@ucla.edu S. Haber The Center for Vital Longevity, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA

2 176 J.M. Logan et al. Memory performance benefits from the repeated presentation of items, and long-term retention benefits when these items are spaced apart in time, rather than massed. This spacing effect has been demonstrated in a number of instances, with different populations, and is a highly robust phenomenon (see Cepeda et al. 2006, for a review; see also Glenberg 1976). On an applied level, spaced schedules would be ideal for students when studying for tests or exams. However, spaced strategies are often not employed, and several lines of evidence suggest that participants often fail to appreciate the benefits accrued by spaced practice. For example, Baddeley and Longman (1978) demonstrated that postal workers preferred massed over distributed training despite the fact that spaced practice lead to better performance. Zechmeister and Shaughnessy (1980) showed that participants memory predictions did not differentiate between massed and spaced rehearsal (or were more likely to favor massed rehearsal), despite the fact that recall was superior for spaced items. Evidence from motor learning also suggests that, although massed practice can benefit short-term retention, participants predict that massed practice will also lead to better later retention, relative to spaced practice (Simon and Bjork 2001). Thus, prior data suggests that, contrary to actual performance, participants consistently regard massed practice as better for learning than spaced practice (cf. Bjork 1999). The present study examines this issue by using a design that includes experience-based learning and feedback about recall performance. Other methods of assessing metacognitive performance are consistent with the idea that participants do not fully appreciate the benefits of spacing. For example, several studies have allowed participants to choose to employ different spacing schedules. When items are presented at a fast presentation rate, Son (2004) showed that participants chose massed practice for certain items, relative to spacing. Thus, even when studying is under participants control, they choose massed practice. Benjamin and Bird (2006) reported that participants would space items when told that they needed to space half of the items (and mass the remaining items), such that participants will space more difficult items that they feel they need to practice at a later time (see also Toppino et al. 2009). Pyc and Dunlosky (2010) found that participants chose to mass both easier and rapidly-presented items. Toppino and Cohen (2010) also noted a preference among participants to space study of difficult items during a cued-recall task. They found that subjects chose to space more items that were given an arbitrary high point value (5 points vs. 1 point or no points) for memory, regardless of difficulty level. Taken together, they argue that these findings support Ariel et al. (2009) agenda-based regulation model, which states that learners create study agendas based on maximizing goal achievement. These studies suggest that although participants may choose study options that include spacing items under certain circumstances, it is not clear if participants know that spacing will lead to better recall, and if predictions of later recall will reflect the benefits of spaced versus massed rehearsal. Individuals tend to greatly underestimate the effect spacing has on memory performance. This can lead to a general illusion of comprehension or competence (e.g., Jacoby et al. 1994), in which students feel better prepared with massed practice, despite spaced practice leading to better later memory performance. The current research extends upon previous work by exploring whether providing subjects with experience, feedback, and information on the benefits of spacing would help them become more aware of the spacing effect. In the context of judgments of learning (JOLs) and spaced versus massed practice, participants often underestimate the influence of spacing on later recall. Zechmeister and Shaughnessy (1980) asked participants to make memory predictions after a single presentation of an item, or after the second presentation of items that were presented twice in either a massed or spaced manner. With a short list of words (24) and a delayed free recall task after a one-minute retention interval, participants predictions were higher for the twice versus once presented items, but participants were more inclined to give slightly higher JOLs for

3 Metacognition and the spacing effect 177 massed than spaced items, despite a recall advantage for spaced items. This design may be improved by asking people to make predictions after each presentation of each item (as opposed to only after the second presentation), as this may lead to more accurate predictions. Research in the area of metacognition suggests that individuals typically prefer massed practice (or fail to understand the benefits of spacing), but that this may be the result of participants having little experience with retrieval following massed or spaced practice. For example, Koriat et al. (2004) have shown that participants JOLs are insensitive to retention interval, and that participants place high priority on the properties of the items (e.g., associative relatedness of word pairs), as opposed to test-related information such as the length of retention interval between study and test. Several studies (e.g., Dunlosky and Hertzog 2000; Koriat and Bjork 2006) have shown that practice with encoding and retrieval conditions can lead to alterations in predictions that better capture memory performance. For instance, dewinstanley and Bjork (2004) found that after studying passages in which both to-be-read and to-be-generated items were present and being tested, people would demonstrate a traditional generation advantage in memory. However, on a subsequent passage and test, participants improved their performance on to-be-read items to match that of the generated items, suggesting some use of prior experience informing later encoding strategies. In a similar vein, Castel (2008) found that participants learned to incorporate serial position information when making JOLs, but only with experience and when the serial position information was easily accessible during encoding. Why do people s memory predictions not differentiate between massed and spaced rehearsal, despite memory being strongly affected by spaced practice? In the present study, we examined participants memory predictions for massed and spaced items, following the work of Zechmeister and Shaughnessy (1980) and others (e.g., Dunlosky and Nelson 1994; Kornell and Bjork 2008; Kornell et al. 2010). In Experiment 1, we examined this issue when participants made ratings after each presentation of the repeated (massed or spaced) items. In particular, participants might become more aware of the benefits of both repetition and spacing when these predictions are made for each presentation of the item (as opposed to only after the second presentation, as done in the study by Zechmeister and Shaughnessy 1980). In addition, participants also engaged in multiple study-test cycles (with unique lists of words), in order to determine if participants learned from experience with previous lists and recall tests and adjusted JOLs to better capture the benefits of spacing, to examine any effect of knowledge updating (e.g., Hertzog et al. 2009). In Experiments 2 and 3, we attempted to make participants aware of the benefits of spacing by allowing them to score their own recall performance in terms of massed and spaced items that were recalled. Our reasoning was that this might draw attention to the differences in performance for spaced and massed items. Finally, in Experiment 4, we directly informed participants about the benefits of spacing in memory performance, to compare how direct cueing influenced JOLs compared to potentially more subtle cues of experience and feedback. The results from these experiments are then discussed in terms of the cues that participants use when making JOLs for massed and spaced items, and how metacognitive predictions can be sensitive or insensitive to critical features that lead to the benefits of spacing in everyday learning. Experiment 1 Although spacing enhances later recall performance, participants predictions do not always reflect this finding, suggesting that people are not aware of the benefits of spaced rehearsal (Dunlosky and Nelson 1994; Zechmeister and Shaughnessy 1980). In Experiment 1 we

4 178 J.M. Logan et al. sought to replicate the main finding in which recall, but not predictions of recall, were influenced by spacing of items. Participants studied a list of items, some of which were presented twice, in either a massed or spaced fashion. Upon the second presentation of these items, participants made a JOL. Based on previous work, we expected recall to favor spaced items, but that JOLs would be less sensitive to spacing. We also were interested in whether participants could learn to assign higher JOLs to spaced items, with experience with multiple lists (study and tests), and making JOLs after each presentation of the item. This was done to draw participants attention to the spaced and massed presentation of items in the context of memory predictions, and to allow for a measure of the change in JOLs from first to second presentation of the various items. Furthermore, several experiments have suggested that repeated testing improves JOL accuracy (e.g., Begg et al. 1989; King et al. 1980; Koriat 1997; Leonesio and Nelson 1990; Lovelace 1984). Thus, as done by others who have examined how experience with encoding and retrieval can influence the accuracy of subsequent metacognitive judgments (e.g., Castel 2008; Koriat1997; Koriat and Bjork 2006; Price et al. 2008; Rhodes and Castel 2008a, b), participants were given experience with study and test conditions, in order to determine if JOLs would change with test experience. This process, referred to as knowledge updating, can lead to improvements in metacognitive accuracy in certain situations (Dunlosky and Hertzog 2000). Thus, the present study expanded on previous work by soliciting JOLs after each presentation of each item, as opposed to just the second presentation of each item (see Zechmeister and Shaughnessy 1980), as well as having participants engage in three study-test cycles with a unique list of words in each cycle, to determine if JOLs become more sensitive to the spacing effect with task experience. Method Participants Twenty-eight participants were recruited from undergraduate courses and received course credit for participating. Apparatus The experiment was run on a Dell computer with a standard 15 in. monitor and implemented using E-prime software (Schneider et al. 2001). The stimuli were presented in the center of the screen, in white lowercase letters on a black background and printed in 18- point Arial font. Materials The stimuli consisted of three lists of 19 medium frequency words selected from Kucera and Francis (1967). The word lists consisted of words that were presented only once (single presentation), words that were repeated immediately (massed presentation), and words that were repeated after a lag of three intervening items (spaced practice). Two words at the beginning and two words at the end of the list were used as buffer words to account for primacy and recency effects. The 15 remaining words five in each condition (single, massed, spaced) were counterbalanced across conditions. Procedure Participants were asked to learn and recall three lists, and provided JOLs after each presentation of every item in each list. In the learning phase of each list, participants were told they would be given a list of words to study, one at a time, and that some words would be repeated at various points in the list. After the presentation of each word, they would be asked to predict how likely they would be to remember the word (also referred to as a judgment of learning, or JOL).

5 Metacognition and the spacing effect 179 At the beginning of a trial in the learning phase, a word was presented on the computer screen for 6 s. After the second presentation of an item, once the word was cleared from the screen, participants were asked How likely are you to remember this word? and shown a rating scale from 0 % to 100 %, marked in 10 % increments. They entered their JOL from 0 % to 100 % using keys marked in the 10 % increments on the keyboard (0 %, 10 %, 20 %, etc.). The JOL question and scale remained on the screen for a total of 6 s or until a response was entered. After the JOL, there was a 500 ms fixation display in which a white crosshair appeared in the center of the screen before the next word was presented. After the learning phase, participants completed a distracter task that involved counting aloud backwards by 3 s for 30 s from a three-digit number. After the counting task was finished, participants were given instructions about the recall test. They were instructed to recall as many of the words from the list as possible by speaking them aloud to the experimenter, who recorded the participants responses on paper. After the learning and final recall phases for the first list of words, participants then proceeded to the learning and recall phases of the second list, followed by the learning and recall phases of the third list. Results and discussion The results from Experiment 1 are displayed in Fig. 1 (in terms of mean recall and JOLs for massed and spaced items as a function of list) and Fig. 2 (mean recall and JOLs for the first and second presentation of massed and spaced items as a function of list). Free recall As expected, there were significant effects of both repetition and spacing on free recall performance. Participants recalled more massed (M051 %; SE04.1 %) than oncepresented items (M040 %; SE04.2 %), F(1, 27)09.89, MSE , p<.01, η 2 p0.27 and recalled more spaced items (M067 %; SE03.7 %) than massed items, F(1, 27)037.51, Fig. 1 Mean recall performance and judgment of learning (JOL) of spaced and massed items for each list in Experiment 1. Error bars reflect standard error of the means in all figures

6 180 J.M. Logan et al. Fig. 2 Mean judgment of learning (JOL) for the first (JOL1) and second (JOL2) presentation of massed and spaced items, and mean recall for massed and spaced items, for each list in Experiment 1 MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.58. There was also a slight trend towards a main effect of List, such that recall improved over lists, from 56 % in List 1 to 65 % in List 3, although this did not reach conventional levels of significance, F(2, 54)02.51, MSE , p0.09, η 2 p0.09. This increasing trend in free recall across trials could be attributed to strategy changes across free-recall study/test cycles (e.g., Delaney and Knowles 2005). The List x Spacing effect was not significant, F<1, as the mean spacing effect remained fairly stable across lists. Judgments of learning JOLs were collected on all trials for all conditions, yielding one JOL for once presented items and two JOLs for items in the massed and spaced conditions. There was no reliable difference between JOLs for once-presented items and massed items on the first presentation (F<1). However, there was a significant difference between JOLs for oncepresented items and the second presentation of massed items, such that participants gave higher JOLs to massed items on their second presentation (M053 %; SE02.9 %) than to the once-presented items (M046 %; SE02.8 %), F(1, 27)020.82, MSE098.52, p<.001, η 2 p0.44. Thus, participants increased their JOL ratings when an item was repeated. Differences in JOLs were also apparent based on study schedule (massed vs. spaced). Specifically, participants gave higher JOLs on the second presentation of an item (M054 %; SE02.9 %) compared to the first presentation of an item (M046 %; SE02.8 %), F(1, 27)027.51, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.51. JOLs were also slightly but reliably higher for spaced items (M052 %; SE02.8 %) than massed items (M050 %; SE02.8 %), F(1, 27)08.88, MSE036.08, p<.01, η 2 p0.25, suggesting some awareness of the spacing effect. However, despite this slight increase in JOLs for spaced items, participants still underestimated the benefits of spacing by a substantial margin. Comparison of recall with JOLs We carried out a direct comparison of JOLs and recall performance, in order to determine how these variables may or may not be related, and how this could change with task experience and/or knowledge updating. However, we note that these analyses should be treated with some caution as participants may use a restricted range when assigning JOLs, whereas recall performance is not limited by these scaling issues. In

7 Metacognition and the spacing effect 181 terms of determining if Measure (JOL and recall) interacted with List (1 st,2 nd,or3 rd )or Study Schedule (spaced or massed), a 2 (Measure: JOL, recall) x 3 (List: first, second, third) x 2 (Study Schedule: spaced, massed) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for each JOL trial (initial judgment vs repeated judgment). For the initial JOL, there was a main effect of Measure such that actual recall was higher (M059 %; SE03.7 %) than JOLs (M047 %; SE04.8 %), F(1, 27)09.70, MSE , p<.01. η 2 p0.26. This difference between actual recall and JOLs increased across lists, as shown by a Measure x List interaction. In particular, while recall increased over lists (55 % to 57 % to 65 % for Lists 1, 2 and 3, respectively), JOLs decreased across lists (50 % to 47 % to 44 %), F(2, 54)06.08, MSE , p<.01, η 2 p0.18. This general decline of JOLs across multiple lists has been demonstrated in previous studies, and has been referred to as the underconfidence with practice effect (e.g., Koriat et al. 2002). As illustrated in Fig. 1, there was also a significant interaction between memory measure and study schedule, such that the spacing effect was much larger for recall (67 % vs 51 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than for initial JOLs (48 % vs 46 % respectively), F(1, 54)034.18, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.56. For the JOLs made on the second presentation of each item, there was a significant interaction between Measure and List, such that recall increased over list (55 % to 57 % to 65 %) but JOLs decreased over list (59 % to 54 % to 50 %), F(2, 54)07.69, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.22. There was also a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (67 % vs 51 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in the repeated JOLs (57 % vs 53 % respectively), F(1, 54)025.89, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0 49. There was a much smaller disparity between JOLs for massed items (M053 %; SE02.9 %) and actual recall of massed items (M051 %; SE04.1 %), compared to JOLs given for spaced items (M056 %; SE03.0 %) and actual recall for the spaced items (M067 %; SE03.7 %). This suggests that although participants were quite accurate at predicting recall for massed items, they underestimated how likely they would be to recall the spaced items. In an exploratory analysis designed to assess the potential influence of experience on participants JOL ratings over lists, we examined the correlation between the size of the spacing effect and difference in JOLs for massed vs spaced items for each participant. It may be the case that when participants show a sizable spacing effect, then JOLs reflect a difference between massed and spaced items. Alternatively, it could be the case that only those participants who show a spacing effect are aware of this difference, due to some participant characteristics. For List 3, the correlation between size of the spacing effect and size of the difference in JOLs for massed and spaced items was r0.02 p>.90, indicating no relationship between the size of the spacing effect in actual memory performance and subjects JOLs for spaced vs massed items. The results from Experiment 1 suggest that, although participants are well aware of the benefits of repetition and massed practice, JOLs were not highly sensitive to the effect of spaced presentation on later recall. Experiment 1 showed that participants JOLs did not differ for spaced compared to massed practice, whereas recall was better for spaced items. This replicates and extends the main findings from Zechmeister and Shaughnessy (1980) in which participants memory predictions failed to differentiate between massed or spaced items, despite actual memory performance being greater for spaced relative to massed items. In general, it appears that JOLs increased by approximately 10 percentage points from the first to the second presentation (massed or spaced), possibly reflecting an anchoring and adjusting mechanism (e.g., Scheck et al. 2004) that is common for both spaced and massed items. What is also present in the results is that overall recall increased with task experience (see also Delaney and Knowles 2005) while JOLs declined, which may reflect underconfidence with practice (Koriat et al. 2002). The main finding is that while JOLs were fairly well calibrated for massed items, JOLs underestimated actual recall for spaced items.

8 182 J.M. Logan et al. Experiment 2 One potential reason that participants memory predictions may not accurately capture the role of spacing in recall may be the lack of feedback regarding recall performance for massed and spaced items. For example, participants may not understand the impact of spacing because, in a free recall task, they are unable to distinguish between those items that are recalled which were presented in a spaced compared to massed fashion. Thus, if participants were aware that they recalled a larger number of spaced items, they might adjust their JOLs accordingly. Experiment 2 addressed this issue by providing specific feedback on recall output. The procedure was identical to Experiment 1, with the exception that participants were informed of their performance after each list. Specifically, in order to make participants aware of their own recall performance, participants in Experiment 2 scored their own recall immediately after the recall session (see also Rawson and Dunlosky 2007), and tabulated the number of spaced, massed, and single items that were recalled. They then engaged in a second and third list of unique items, and continued to score their own recall output after each recall trial. Under these conditions, of specific interest was whether participants JOLs would reflect their performance for spaced and massed items given previous experience and awareness of differences in recall performance for spaced, massed and single items. Method Participants Forty-seven participants were recruited from undergraduate courses and received course credit for participating. Apparatus, materials, and procedure These were identical to Experiment 1, with one exception: participants were given feedback on their performance after every list by scoring their own recall sheets. After the recall phase of each list, participants were given a sheet that listed the words they had just been asked to learn, divided according to condition, with the labels spaced practice, massed practice, studied once, beginning of list, and end of list. Participants were instructed as to what each label meant. Using their recall sheet, participants were instructed to give themselves one point for every word they correctly recalled. They then wrote down the number of points they received in each condition. After grading their recall sheets, they then proceeded to the next list of words until they had studied and been tested on a total of three lists. Results and discussion The results from Experiment 2 are displayed in Fig. 3 (in terms of overall recall and JOLs for massed and spaced items as a function of list) and Fig. 4 (recall and JOLs for the first and second presentation of massed and spaced items as a function of each list). Free recall As expected, there were significant effects of both repetition and spacing on free recall performance. Participants remembered massed items (M053 %; SE02.9 %) better than once-presented items (M045 %; SE03.0 %), F(1, 46)012.09, MSE , p<.01, η 2 p0.21, and recalled more spaced items (M067 %; SE03.1 %) than massed items, F(1, 46)033.21, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.42. Judgments of learning As in Experiment 1, JOLs were collected on all trials for all conditions, yielding one JOL for once presented items and two JOLs for items in the massed

9 Metacognition and the spacing effect 183 Fig. 3 Mean recall performance and judgment of learning (JOL) of spaced and massed items for each list in Experiment 2 and spaced conditions. In terms of repetition effects in JOLs, there was no reliable difference between JOLs for once-presented items and the initial presentation of the massed or spaced items (F<1). However, there was a significant difference between JOLs for once-presented items and the repeated presentation of massed items, such that participants gave higher JOLs to massed items on their second presentation (M056 %; SE01.8 %) than to the oncepresented items (M047 %; SE01.7 %), F(1, 46)078.82, MSE070.65, p<.001, η 2 p0.63. Fig. 4 Mean judgment of learning (JOL) for the first (JOL1) and second (JOL2) presentation of massed and spaced items, and mean recall for massed and spaced items, for each list in Experiment 2

10 184 J.M. Logan et al. Thus, participants increased their JOL ratings when an item was repeated. In general, participants gave higher JOLs on the second presentation of an item (M057 %; SE01.8) compared to the first presentation of an item (M047 %; SE01.6 %), F(1, 46) , MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.72. There was no difference in JOLs for spaced and massed items on the initial JOL (47 % vs 48 %, respectively) but there was a difference on the repeated JOL (59 % vs 56 %, for spaced vs. massed, respectively) Thus, participants were not only reliably giving higher JOLs to repeated items, they were correctly judging spaced items as more likely to be remembered compared to massed items, although they still greatly underestimated actual performance for spaced items. Comparison of recall with JOLs The relation between recall and JOLs was examined in a 2 (Measure) x 3 (List) x 2 (Study Schedule) repeated measures ANOVA each JOL trial (initial judgment vs repeated judgment). For the initial JOL, there was a main effect of Measure such that actual recall was higher (M060 %; SE02.7 %) than JOLs (M047 %; SE01.6 %), F(1, 46)021.42, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.32. As illustrated in Fig. 3, there was also a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 46)027.17, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.37, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (67 % vs 53 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in initial JOLs (48 % vs 47 % for spaced and massed, respectively). For the repeated JOL, there was also a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 46)019.85, MSE , p<.001, η 2 p0.30, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (67 % vs 53 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in the repeated JOLs (59 % vs 56 % respectively). As in Experiment 1, there was a much smaller disparity between JOLs for massed items (M056 %; SE01.8 %) and actual recall of massed items (M053 %; SE02.9 %), whereas JOLs given for spaced items (M059 %; SE01.9 %) were significantly lower than actual recall for the spaced items (M067 %; SE03.1 %). Thus, participants were quite accurate at predicting recall for massed items, and reliably increased their JOLs for spaced compared to massed items upon repetition, but they still underestimated how likely they would be to recall the spaced items within each list. Thus, the impact of spacing on recall was much more substantial than the impact of spacing on JOLs. As in Experiment 1, the correlation between a subjects spacing effect in actual recall and the spacing effect in their JOLs was computed for the final list, List 3. Contrary to Experiment 1, there was a significant correlation between the size of the spacing effect in memory and the size of the spacing effect in JOLs, r (47)0.34, p<.02. Individuals with larger spacing effects were more likely to adjust their JOLs for spaced and massed items accordingly. Experiment 3 In order to determine if participants may become sensitive to spacing when a larger spacing effect is present, we conducted an experiment in which the lag between spaced items was increased, as well as the total number of items in the list. Increasing the lag between spaced items should lead to a greater spacing effect (e.g., Glenberg 1976, 1977; Madigan 1969; Melton 1970; Murdock 1974; Underwood 1969). This manipulation was used to allow for a greater number of spaced items to be recalled at test, which may then draw attention to the presence of a spacing effect when participants score their own recall output. Thus, a lag of eight items between spaced repetitions was used, as compared to a lag of four items in the previous experiments. The combination of a greater lag and longer lists enhanced the overall spacing effect.

11 Metacognition and the spacing effect 185 Method Participants Twenty-four participants were recruited from undergraduate courses and received course credit for participating. Apparatus, materials, and procedure These were identical to Experiment 2, with two key exceptions. A longer lag was used for the spacing condition in order to produce a larger spacing effect, and a longer list was used in an effort to make the spacing between items more salient to participants. A lag of 8 items between spaced repetitions was used, as compared to a lag of 4 items in the previous experiments. In addition, the number of words per condition was increased from five to eight, which increased list length from 29 words in Experiments 1 and 2 to 45 words, so subjects had more experience with all conditions. Results and discussion The results from Experiment 3 are displayed in Fig. 5 (in terms of overall recall and JOLs for massed and spaced items as a function of list) and Fig. 6 (recall and JOLs for the first and second presentation of massed and spaced items as a function of each list). Free recall As expected, there were significant effects of both repetition and spacing on free recall performance. Participants remembered massed items (M056 %; SE03.1 %) better than once-presented items (M056 %; SE03.1 %), F(1, 23)012.0, MSE , p<.01, η2p0.34, and recalled more spaced items (M070 %; SE03.5 %) than massed items, F(1, 23)035.47, MSE , p<.001, η2p0.60. Fig. 5 Mean recall performance and judgment of learning (JOL) of spaced and massed items for each list in Experiment 3

12 186 J.M. Logan et al. Fig. 6 Mean judgment of learning (JOL) for the first (JOL1) and second (JOL2) presentation of massed and spaced items, and mean recall for massed and spaced items, for each list in Experiment 3 Judgments of learning As in Experiment 2, JOLs were collected on all trials for all conditions, yielding one JOL for once presented items and two JOLs for items in the massed and spaced conditions. In terms of repetition effects in JOLs, there was no reliable difference between JOLs for once-presented items and the initial presentation of the massed or spaced items (F<1). There was a significant difference when comparing JOLs for once-presented items and the repeated presentation of massed items, however, such that JOLs were higher for massed items upon repetition (56 %) compared to once-presented items (51 %), F(1, 23)08.50, MSE084.32, p<.01, η2p0.27, indicating that JOLs were sensitive to repetition. There was no difference in JOLs for spaced and massed items on the initial JOL (51.5 % vs 50.6 %, respectively). For the repeated JOL, however, JOLs were significantly higher for spaced (58.4 %) compared to massed (55.7 %) items, F(1, 23)05.10, MSE048.47, p<.04, η2p0.18, indicating that participants were somewhat sensitive to the benefits of spacing in recall. Comparison of recall with JOLs The relation between recall and JOLs was examined in a 2 (Measure) x 3 (List) x 2 (Study Schedule) repeated measures ANOVA for each JOL trial (initial judgment vs repeated judgment). For the initial JOL, as shown in Fig. 6, there was a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 23)025.89, MSE , p<.001, η2p0.53, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (70 % vs 56 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in initial JOLs (51.5 % vs 50.6 % for spaced and massed, respectively). For the repeated JOL, there was also a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 23)023.29, MSE , p<.001, η2p0.50, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (70.5 % vs 55.6 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in the repeated JOLs (58.4 % vs 55.7 % respectively). As in Experiment 2, there was a much smaller disparity between JOLs for massed items (M055.6 %; SE03.1 %) and actual recall of massed items (M055.7 %; SE02.0 %), whereas JOLs given for spaced items (M058.4 %; SE02.1 %) were significantly lower than actual recall for the spaced items (M070.4 %; SE0

13 Metacognition and the spacing effect %). Thus, participants were quite accurate at predicting recall for massed items, and reliably increased their JOLs for spaced compared to massed items upon repetition, but they still underestimated how likely they would be to recall the spaced items within each list. As in previous experiments, the impact of spacing on recall was much more substantial than the impact of spacing on JOLs. As in the previous experiments, the correlation between subjects spacing effect in actual recall and the spacing effect in JOLs was computed for the final list, List 3. There was a marginally significant correlation between the size of the spacing effect in memory and the size of the spacing effect in JOLs, r(24)0.39, p0.06, similar to Experiment 2. Results from Experiment 3 were similar to those in previous experiments. JOLs for massed items were very close to actual recall for massed items, but JOLs for spaced items still significantly underestimated the benefits of spacing to final recall. This was the case despite the fact that lists were changed to make the manipulation of spacing more salient to participants, by increasing the number of spaced and massed items and doubling the lag between spaced items from 4 to 8. Again, as in Experiment 2, by the final list, there was a correlation between the magnitude of participants spacing effects in recall and the spacing effect reflected in their JOLs. This may indicate that participants awareness of the benefits of spacing on memory are most likely to occur when the benefits of spacing are particular obvious and apparent. In these experiments, there is an indication that this awareness is developing after experience with multiple lists, but it is still not enough to greatly increase subsequent JOLs for spaced versus massed items by the final list. Experiment 4 The findings thus far indicate that, although participants are quite accurate at predicting their performance for repeated items in a massed condition, they are still not sensitive enough to the spacing effect to adjust their JOLs for spaced and massed items appropriately, despite increased experience and feedback with the spacing effect. More recent work has shown that some knowledge updating can occur with experience, but only when trials are blocked, making them more apparent to the learner. Price et al. (2008) suggest that one reason JOLs may not show substantial updating after task experience is that participants do not link performance at test with previous encoding operations. To make the link more obvious, Price et al. blocked strategy type at test (e.g., imagery items were tested in a separate block from repetition items). In this case, JOLs showed somewhat better sensitivity to strategy type. Of course, blocking trials is impossible in the case of spacing, by definition, so other ways to draw attention to the benefits of spacing are needed in multi-trial designs. The previous experiments examined this by allowing for self-scoring of recall performance, which led to a trend in increased JOLs for spaced items in subjects showing large spacing effects, but perhaps instituting some factor at encoding that makes spacing salient would allow for greater learning about the effect. Experiment 4 was conducted to explore the role that more direct awareness of the spacing effect in recall could have on subsequent JOLs. Participants first studied a word list and gave JOLs for each item and received feedback on their performance for each class of items after recall, as in previous experiments. Unlike the previous studies, however, after the first list, participants were given instruction on the spacing effect, including what it was and how spacing could benefit their memory. Then, they studied another list of words similar to the first list, this time armed with direct knowledge of the spacing effect. The effect of this direct awareness on JOLs and subsequent recall was the focus of this experiment.

14 188 J.M. Logan et al. Method Participants Twenty-five participants were recruited from undergraduate courses and received course credit for participating. Apparatus, materials, and procedure These were similar to Experiment 3, with two key exceptions. Only two lists of to-be-remembered items were used in Experiment 4. The lists were the same as those used in Experiment 3, with a lag of eight items, with eight items per condition, etc. The procedure for each list was also the same as Experiment 3: Participants studied and made JOLs for each word as it appeared, recalled the words, and graded their recall sheet afterwards. However, before beginning List 2, participants were given information on the spacing effect. After being told that they would now study and rate a new list of words, they read the following instructions on the screen just before List 2 was presented: You may have noticed that some of the words are repeated - sometimes you see a word twice in a row (MASSED STUDY), other times it is repeated a little later in the list (SPACED STUDY). Researchers know that repetition is good for memory, and spaced repetition tends to be much better than massed repetition. The advantage for spaced items compared to massed items is called the Spacing Effect. You may see a Spacing Effect in your own memory if your SPACED STUDY score is higher than your MASSED STUDY score on your grading sheet. The Spacing Effect can be a very powerful effect in memory, but people tend to underestimate how much spacing will help their memory. Keep the Spacing Effect in mind as you study and rate the next words for your memory test! After the second list was presented for study and JOLs, participants again recalled the items and graded their recall sheets. Results & discussion The results from Experiment 4 are displayed in Fig. 7 (in terms of overall recall and JOLs for massed and spaced items as a function of list) and Fig. 8 (recall and JOLs for the first and second presentation of massed and spaced items as a function of each list). Free recall As expected, there were significant effects of both repetition and spacing on free recall performance. Participants remembered massed items (M048 %; SE03.7 %) better than once-presented items (M040 %; SE03.6 %), F(1, 24)06.32, MSE , p<.02, η2p0.21, and recalled more spaced items (M069 %; SE02.9 %) than massed items, F(1, 24)037.70, MSE , p<.001, η2p0.61. There was also a significant List x Spacing interaction, F(1, 24)05.11, MSE , p<.04, η2p0.18, such that the spacing effect was larger on List 2 (29 %) than List 1 (13 %). Judgments of learning As in Experiments 2 and 3, JOLs were collected on all trials for all conditions, yielding one JOL for once presented items and two JOLs for items in the massed and spaced conditions. In terms of repetition effects in JOLs, there was a small but reliable difference between JOLs for once-presented items and the initial presentation of the massed or spaced items, F(1, 24)08.13, MSE024.53, p<.01, η2p0.25, such that initial JOLs for massed items (45 %) were actually slightly smaller than JOLs for once-presented items (47 %). However, when comparing JOLs for once-presented items and the repeated

15 Metacognition and the spacing effect 189 Mean Percentage JOL Rating or Correct Recall MASSED SPACED MASSED SPACED BEFORE INSTRUCTION Study Schedule AFTER INSTRUCTION Fig. 7 Mean recall performance and judgment of learning (JOL) of spaced and massed items before and after instructions on the spacing effect in Experiment 4 presentation of massed items, JOLs for massed items (51 %) were significantly higher than once-presented items, F(1, 24)011.16, MSE021.11, p<.01, η2p0.32, indicating that JOLs were sensitive to repetition. There was no difference in JOLs for spaced and massed items on the initial JOL (48 % vs 46 %, respectively). However, for the repeated JOL, there was a significant difference between spaced (58 %) and massed items (51 %), F(1, 24)033.75, Mean Percentage JOL Rating or Correct Recall JOL 1 JOL 2 RECALL MASSED SPACED MASSED SPACED BEFORE INSTRUCTION AFTER INSTRUCTION Fig. 8 Mean judgment of learning (JOL) for the first (JOL1) and second (JOL2) presentation of massed and spaced items, and mean recall for massed and spaced items, before and after instructions on the spacing effect in Experiment 4

16 190 J.M. Logan et al. MSE033.51, p<.001, η2p0.58. There was also a significant List x Study Schedule interaction, F(1, 24)07.58, MSE033.54, p<.02, η2p0.24, which showed that the spacing effect in JOLs was greater in List 2 (10 %), after instructions on the spacing effect, than List 1 (3 %). Thus, direct information on the spacing effect appeared to increase awareness of the benefits of spacing and subsequently influenced JOLs for spaced versus massed items. Comparison of recall with JOLs The relation between recall and JOLs was examined in a 2 (Measure) x 3 (List) x 2 (Study Schedule) repeated measures ANOVA for each JOL trial (initial judgment vs repeated judgment). For the initial JOL, as shown in Fig. 8, there was a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 24)024.81, MSE , p<.001, η2p0.51, such that the spacing effect was much larger in recall (69 % vs 48 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in initial JOLs (48 % vs 46 % for spaced and massed, respectively). For the repeated JOL, there was also a significant interaction between Measure and Study Schedule, F(1, 24)016.04, MSE , p<.01, η2p0.40, such that the spacing effect was still larger in recall (69 % vs 48 % for spaced and massed, respectively) than in the repeated JOLs (58 % vs 51 % respectively). As in Experiment 3, there was a much smaller disparity between JOLs for massed items (M051 %; SE01.7 %) and actual recall of massed items (M048 %; SE03.7 %), whereas JOLs given for spaced items (M058 %; SE02.3 %) were significantly lower than actual recall for the spaced items (M069 %; SE02.9 %). Thus, participants were more accurate at predicting recall for massed items than spaced items, although they did reliably increase their JOLs for spaced compared to massed items upon repetition after instructions on the spacing effect. Thus, instruction on the spacing effect served to influence JOLs for spaced and massed items to reflect a greater awareness of the spacing effect, but JOLs for spaced items still underestimated actual recall of spaced items. This is especially striking given the instructional information provided prior to the study, and may suggest that participants use more cue-based information, relative to theory-based information, when making JOLs regarding massed and spaced items. Given that this effect did not change substantially across lists, a lack of knowledge updating was observed that is consistent with prior studies (e.g., Hertzog et al. 2009; Price et al. 2008), and suggests that learners may be more focused on specific properties of the information, and thus do not incorporate the benefits of temporal spacing when making item-based JOLs. General discussion The findings from the present studies show that although JOLs are highly sensitive to the repetition of items at encoding, JOLs do not accurately capture the benefits of spaced rehearsal (see also Dunlosky and Nelson 1994; Kornell and Bjork 2008; Zechmeister and Shaughnessy 1980). This effect persisted despite participants making JOLs during each presentation of the item, and even when participants were made aware of the presence of a spacing effect by scoring their own recall prior to engaging in a second similar memory task. Although JOLs did show a slight trend in terms of differentiating between spaced and massed practice, the JOLs were much more accurate for massed than spaced items, as participants greatly underestimated the recall of spaced items. Thus, although participants may not appreciate the benefits of spacing, this type of observation allows for a better understanding of cues that are used when making judgments about memory performance, and why certain cues (repetition) are given more weight than others (schedule of repetition) when making judgments of learning.

17 Metacognition and the spacing effect 191 Our findings may be consistent with Koriat s (1997) cue-utilization approach, which argues that extrinsic cues, such as repetition, are discounted when making JOLs. Recent research has shown that participants may underestimate the benefits of repeated study on later learning (Kornell and Bjork 2009). The present findings suggest that participants may take repetition into account but show less sensitivity to the benefits of spaced repetition. One explanation for the current results is that participants use ease of processing or fluency when making JOLs (e.g., Begg et al. 1989; Castel et al. 2007; Yue et al. 2012), and repeated, massed items are perceived as more fluent than spaced items, despite later recall favoring spaced items. Specifically, in the present study, participants may report that if they just studied some information, an immediate presentation of this same information should enhance learning. However, fluency might have a slightly different effect when items are presented within a longer temporal context (i.e., spaced). In this type of situation, this may lead to a discounted JOL, in which participants may rely on some sort of anchoring and adjustment mechanism (cf. Scheck et al. 2004). With massed items, the initial JOL at first presentation serves as an anchor, and with the second immediate presentation, participants then logically enhance their ratings. However, with spacing, upon the second spaced presentation of an item, the participant may then be reminded that this information was in fact presented earlier, but was possibly not well encoded (due somewhat less fluent processing of spaced items as compared to fluency that is accrued in massed fashion). Although somewhat speculative, this experience and insight regarding the current status of the item in memory may then cause the participant to adjust their JOL to capture this intuition, adjusting downward from what might actually have been an accurate initial anchor. Several additional theoretical explanations may exist regarding why participants do not give accurate JOLs for spaced items. One possibility may be based on encoding variability, in that participants have stronger access to the present and prior instances of massed items, and weaker access to the first item instances for spaced items when making JOLs. Thus, accessibility plays a dominant role when making JOLs, whereas variability may be a better diagnostic cue, but is less accessible. Another possibility is that participants understand that massed presentations lead to good performance for immediate tests, and do not anticipate a long-term memory test, in which spaced items are better recalled relative to massed items. However, if this were the case, then with task experience (e.g., on List 2 or 3), participants should be aware of the retrieval conditions, and it appears that participants do not show knowledge updating (see also Hertzog et al. 2009; Price et al. 2008). While the present results do not provide a firm delineation between various theoretical accounts, the observation of learning about the benefits of spacing with task experience are important to better understand how experience-based learning can inform item-based metacognitive judgments. It should be noted that the present experiment represents an approach to the study of spacing that may be somewhat far removed from actual study and testing that occurs in the classroom, in which longer and more variable retention intervals exist and richer materials are studied and retrieved. Although the benefits of spacing are widely known to cognitive psychologists, educators, and possibly many students, in the present experiments, students may employ a different mental model of how information is retained and forgotten. While the hallmark of learning is often evidenced by tests of long-term memory, students may feel that learning or mastery can be quickly assessed by an immediate test, and in fact, in terms of short-term retention (Bjork 1999), massed practice can lead to better memory performance than spacing. The present study attempted to give participants a certain amount of experience with study-test episodes that lead to a robust spacing effect, but perhaps JOLs are more a measure of immediate processing fluency (Begg et al. 1989) and not a reflection of how encoding variability and retention interval might influence learning.

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney

Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists. By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney Rote rehearsal and spacing effects in the free recall of pure and mixed lists By: Peter P.J.L. Verkoeijen and Peter F. Delaney Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L, & Delaney, P. F. (2008). Rote rehearsal and spacing

More information

Predicting One s Own Forgetting: The Role of Experience-Based and Theory-Based Processes

Predicting One s Own Forgetting: The Role of Experience-Based and Theory-Based Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 133, No. 4, 643 656 0096-3445/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643 Predicting One s

More information

The present study investigated whether subjects were sensitive to negative

The present study investigated whether subjects were sensitive to negative MIYAKE, TINA M., Ph.D. Metacognition, Proactive Interference, and Working Memory: Can People Monitor for Proactive Interference at Encoding and Retrieval? (2007) Directed by Dr. Michael J. Kane 118 pp.

More information

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning

Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:518 523 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0085-x Testing protects against proactive interference in face name learning Yana Weinstein & Kathleen B. McDermott & Karl K. Szpunar Published

More information

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1

Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1 Running head: DELAY AND PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 1 In Press at Memory & Cognition Effects of Delay of Prospective Memory Cues in an Ongoing Task on Prospective Memory Task Performance Dawn M. McBride, Jaclyn

More information

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls

Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving Closed-Head Injured Patients and Controls Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1380-3395/03/2502-274$16.00 2003, Vol. 25, No. 2, pp. 274 282 # Swets & Zeitlinger Comparison Between Three Memory Tests: Cued Recall, Priming and Saving

More information

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory

Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (2), 211-221 Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory MARY ANN FOLEY AND HUGH J. FOLEY

More information

Accuracy and Speed Feedback: Global and Local Effects on Strategy Use

Accuracy and Speed Feedback: Global and Local Effects on Strategy Use Accuracy and Speed Feedback: Global and Local Effects on Strategy Use By: Dayna R. Touron, Christopher Hertzog Touron, D.R., & Hertzog, C. (2014). Accuracy and Speed Feedback: Global and Local Effects

More information

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J.

An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming. Jason R. Perry. University of Western Ontario. Stephen J. An Evaluation of the Interactive-Activation Model Using Masked Partial-Word Priming Jason R. Perry University of Western Ontario Stephen J. Lupker University of Western Ontario Colin J. Davis Royal Holloway

More information

Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall

Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall Memory & Cognition 1993, 21 (1), 48-62 Hypermnesia in free recall and cued recall DAVID G. PAYNE, HELENE A. HEMBROOKE, and JEFFREY S. ANASTASI State University ofnew York, Binghamton, New York In three

More information

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF MATHEMATICS ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MULTIPLE CHOICE MATH TESTS ELIZABETH ANNE SOMERS Spring 2011 A thesis submitted in partial

More information

Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers

Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers Section II Effective Instruction for Struggling Readers Chapter 5 Components of Effective Instruction After conducting assessments, Ms. Lopez should be aware of her students needs in the following areas:

More information

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall

Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall Strategy Abandonment Effects in Cued Recall Stephanie A. Robinson* a, Amy A. Overman a,, & Joseph D.W. Stephens b a Department of Psychology, Elon University, NC b Department of Psychology, North Carolina

More information

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search

Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search Learning By Asking: How Children Ask Questions To Achieve Efficient Search Azzurra Ruggeri (a.ruggeri@berkeley.edu) Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, USA Max Planck Institute

More information

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness

Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness PEARSON EDUCATION Evidence for Reliability, Validity and Learning Effectiveness Introduction Pearson Knowledge Technologies has conducted a large number and wide variety of reliability and validity studies

More information

Running head: DUAL MEMORY 1. A Dual Memory Theory of the Testing Effect. Timothy C. Rickard. Steven C. Pan. University of California, San Diego

Running head: DUAL MEMORY 1. A Dual Memory Theory of the Testing Effect. Timothy C. Rickard. Steven C. Pan. University of California, San Diego Running head: DUAL MEMORY 1 A Dual Memory Theory of the Testing Effect Timothy C. Rickard Steven C. Pan University of California, San Diego Word Count: 14,800 (main text and references) This manuscript

More information

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test Technical Bulletin #6 Evaluation and Examination Service The University of Iowa (319) 335-0356 HOW TO JUDGE THE QUALITY OF AN OBJECTIVE CLASSROOM

More information

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference?

Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference? DOI 10.1007/s12144-015-9330-1 Is Event-Based Prospective Memory Resistant to Proactive Interference? Joyce M. Oates 1 & Zehra F. Peynircioğlu 1 & Kathryn B. Bates 1 # Springer Science+Business Media New

More information

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm

Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 0 (008), p. 8 Abstract Mandarin Lexical Tone Recognition: The Gating Paradigm Yuwen Lai and Jie Zhang University of Kansas Research on spoken word recognition

More information

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume?

Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume? Difficulty of Interruptions 1 Does the Difficulty of an Interruption Affect our Ability to Resume? David M. Cades Deborah A. Boehm Davis J. Gregory Trafton Naval Research Laboratory Christopher A. Monk

More information

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences?

Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences? Memory & Cognition 1983,11 (3),316-323 Levels of processing: Qualitative differences or task-demand differences? SHANNON DAWN MOESER Memorial University ofnewfoundland, St. John's, NewfoundlandAlB3X8,

More information

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation

Cued Recall From Image and Sentence Memory: A Shift From Episodic to Identical Elements Representation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 734 748 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.734

More information

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT:

BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT: National Survey of Student Engagement (NSSE) BENCHMARK TREND COMPARISON REPORT: CARNEGIE PEER INSTITUTIONS, 2003-2011 PREPARED BY: ANGEL A. SANCHEZ, DIRECTOR KELLI PAYNE, ADMINISTRATIVE ANALYST/ SPECIALIST

More information

Levels-of-Processing Effects on a Variety of Memory Tasks: New Findings and Theoretical Implications

Levels-of-Processing Effects on a Variety of Memory Tasks: New Findings and Theoretical Implications CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 5, 142 164 (1996) ARTICLE NO. 0009 Levels-of-Processing Effects on a Variety of Memory Tasks: New Findings and Theoretical Implications BRADFORD H. CHALLIS 1 Institute of Psychology,

More information

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES)

The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility (RES) Seton Hall University erepository @ Seton Hall Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses (ETDs) Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses Spring 5-1-2017 The New Theory of Disuse Predicts Retrieval

More information

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England

The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency. Jennifer Thorne. University of New England THE EFFECTS OF SUPER SPEED 100 ON READING FLUENCY 1 The Effects of Super Speed 100 on Reading Fluency Jennifer Thorne University of New England THE EFFECTS OF SUPER SPEED 100 ON READING FLUENCY 2 Abstract

More information

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students

A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students A Note on Structuring Employability Skills for Accounting Students Jon Warwick and Anna Howard School of Business, London South Bank University Correspondence Address Jon Warwick, School of Business, London

More information

Wonderworks Tier 2 Resources Third Grade 12/03/13

Wonderworks Tier 2 Resources Third Grade 12/03/13 Wonderworks Tier 2 Resources Third Grade Wonderworks Tier II Intervention Program (K 5) Guidance for using K 1st, Grade 2 & Grade 3 5 Flowcharts This document provides guidelines to school site personnel

More information

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge

Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge Innov High Educ (2009) 34:93 103 DOI 10.1007/s10755-009-9095-2 Maximizing Learning Through Course Alignment and Experience with Different Types of Knowledge Phyllis Blumberg Published online: 3 February

More information

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL

SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL SOFTWARE EVALUATION TOOL Kyle Higgins Randall Boone University of Nevada Las Vegas rboone@unlv.nevada.edu Higgins@unlv.nevada.edu N.B. This form has not been fully validated and is still in development.

More information

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE

OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE OVERVIEW OF CURRICULUM-BASED MEASUREMENT AS A GENERAL OUTCOME MEASURE Mark R. Shinn, Ph.D. Michelle M. Shinn, Ph.D. Formative Evaluation to Inform Teaching Summative Assessment: Culmination measure. Mastery

More information

Paradoxical Effects of Testing: Retrieval Enhances Both Accurate Recall and Suggestibility in Eyewitnesses

Paradoxical Effects of Testing: Retrieval Enhances Both Accurate Recall and Suggestibility in Eyewitnesses Psychology Publications Psychology 1-2011 Paradoxical Effects of Testing: Retrieval Enhances Both Accurate Recall and Suggestibility in Eyewitnesses Jason C.K. Chan Iowa State University, ckchan@iastate.edu

More information

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory

The Role of Test Expectancy in the Build-Up of Proactive Interference in Long-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2014, Vol. 40, No. 4, 1039 1048 2014 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0036164 The Role of Test Expectancy

More information

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries

Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries Learning and Retaining New Vocabularies: The Case of Monolingual and Bilingual Dictionaries Mohsen Mobaraki Assistant Professor, University of Birjand, Iran mmobaraki@birjand.ac.ir *Amin Saed Lecturer,

More information

Chunk Formation in Immediate Memory and How It Relates to Data Compression

Chunk Formation in Immediate Memory and How It Relates to Data Compression Chunk Formation in Immediate Memory and How It Relates to Data Compression Mustapha Chekaf Université de Franche-Comté Nelson Cowan University of Missouri-Columbia Fabien Mathy 1 Université Nice Sophia

More information

Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program

Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program Evaluation of a College Freshman Diversity Research Program Sarah Garner University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195 Michael J. Tremmel University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195 Sarah

More information

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3

The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3 The Oregon Literacy Framework of September 2009 as it Applies to grades K-3 The State Board adopted the Oregon K-12 Literacy Framework (December 2009) as guidance for the State, districts, and schools

More information

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form

Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form Orthographic Form 1 Improved Effects of Word-Retrieval Treatments Subsequent to Addition of the Orthographic Form The development and testing of word-retrieval treatments for aphasia has generally focused

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION

THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION THE INFLUENCE OF TASK DEMANDS ON FAMILIARITY EFFECTS IN VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION: A COHORT MODEL PERSPECTIVE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy

More information

Early Warning System Implementation Guide

Early Warning System Implementation Guide Linking Research and Resources for Better High Schools betterhighschools.org September 2010 Early Warning System Implementation Guide For use with the National High School Center s Early Warning System

More information

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning?

How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning? Journal of European Psychology Students, 2013, 4, 37-46 How Does Physical Space Influence the Novices' and Experts' Algebraic Reasoning? Mihaela Taranu Babes-Bolyai University, Romania Received: 30.09.2011

More information

SETTING STANDARDS FOR CRITERION- REFERENCED MEASUREMENT

SETTING STANDARDS FOR CRITERION- REFERENCED MEASUREMENT SETTING STANDARDS FOR CRITERION- REFERENCED MEASUREMENT By: Dr. MAHMOUD M. GHANDOUR QATAR UNIVERSITY Improving human resources is the responsibility of the educational system in many societies. The outputs

More information

Do multi-year scholarships increase retention? Results

Do multi-year scholarships increase retention? Results Do multi-year scholarships increase retention? In the past, Boise State has mainly offered one-year scholarships to new freshmen. Recently, however, the institution moved toward offering more two and four-year

More information

Causal Link Semantics for Narrative Planning Using Numeric Fluents

Causal Link Semantics for Narrative Planning Using Numeric Fluents Proceedings, The Thirteenth AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence and Interactive Digital Entertainment (AIIDE-17) Causal Link Semantics for Narrative Planning Using Numeric Fluents Rachelyn Farrell,

More information

Extending Learning Across Time & Space: The Power of Generalization

Extending Learning Across Time & Space: The Power of Generalization Extending Learning: The Power of Generalization 1 Extending Learning Across Time & Space: The Power of Generalization Teachers have every right to celebrate when they finally succeed in teaching struggling

More information

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task

Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task P.W. Foos ExperimentalP & P. Goolkasian: sychology 2008 Presentation Hogrefe 2008; Vol. & Huber Format 55(4):215 227 Publishers Effects Presentation Format Effects in a Levels-of-Processing Task Paul W.

More information

Piano Safari Sight Reading & Rhythm Cards for Book 1

Piano Safari Sight Reading & Rhythm Cards for Book 1 Piano Safari Sight Reading & Rhythm Cards for Book 1 Teacher Guide Table of Contents Sight Reading Cards Corresponding Repertoire Bk. 1 Unit Concepts Teacher Guide Page Number Introduction 1 Level A Unit

More information

Encoding. Retrieval. Forgetting. Physiology of Memory. Systems and Types of Memory

Encoding. Retrieval. Forgetting. Physiology of Memory. Systems and Types of Memory Encoding Storage Retrieval Forgetting Encoding Storage Retrieval Fraction of red lights missed 0.08 Encoding 0.06 Getting information into memory 0.04 0.02 0 No cell phone With cell phone Divided Attention

More information

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation Case study: Most vs More than half Jakub Szymanik Outline Number Sense Approximate Number Sense Approximating most Superlative Meaning of most What About Counting?

More information

West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition

West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition Study Guide to accompany West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition Roger LeRoy Miller Institute for University Studies Mary Meinzinger Urisko Madonna University Prepared by Bradene L.

More information

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France

Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland b LEAD CNRS UMR 5022, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France This article was downloaded by: [Université de Genève] On: 21 February 2013, At: 09:06 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie

Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics. Delphine Sasanguie Unraveling symbolic number processing and the implications for its association with mathematics Delphine Sasanguie 1. Introduction Mapping hypothesis Innate approximate representation of number (ANS) Symbols

More information

Proficiency Illusion

Proficiency Illusion KINGSBURY RESEARCH CENTER Proficiency Illusion Deborah Adkins, MS 1 Partnering to Help All Kids Learn NWEA.org 503.624.1951 121 NW Everett St., Portland, OR 97209 Executive Summary At the heart of the

More information

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems?

Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems? Classifying combinations: Do students distinguish between different types of combination problems? Elise Lockwood Oregon State University Nicholas H. Wasserman Teachers College, Columbia University William

More information

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful?

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful? University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Action Research Projects Math in the Middle Institute Partnership 7-2008 Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom:

More information

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart

Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility of the Standard Celeration Chart Behavioral Development Bulletin 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 20, No. 2, 163 167 1942-0722/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0101308 Assessing Functional Relations: The Utility

More information

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections

Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Tyler Perrachione LING 451-0 Proseminar in Sound Structure Prof. A. Bradlow 17 March 2006 Intra-talker Variation: Audience Design Factors Affecting Lexical Selections Abstract Although the acoustic and

More information

Mike Cohn - background

Mike Cohn - background Agile Estimating and Planning Mike Cohn August 5, 2008 1 Mike Cohn - background 2 Scrum 24 hours Sprint goal Return Return Cancel Gift Coupons wrap Gift Cancel wrap Product backlog Sprint backlog Coupons

More information

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives Gwenanne Salkind George Mason University EDCI 856 Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham Spring 2006 Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives Table

More information

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany

Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany Entrepreneurial Discovery and the Demmert/Klein Experiment: Additional Evidence from Germany Jana Kitzmann and Dirk Schiereck, Endowed Chair for Banking and Finance, EUROPEAN BUSINESS SCHOOL, International

More information

Application of Virtual Instruments (VIs) for an enhanced learning environment

Application of Virtual Instruments (VIs) for an enhanced learning environment Application of Virtual Instruments (VIs) for an enhanced learning environment Philip Smyth, Dermot Brabazon, Eilish McLoughlin Schools of Mechanical and Physical Sciences Dublin City University Ireland

More information

The Round Earth Project. Collaborative VR for Elementary School Kids

The Round Earth Project. Collaborative VR for Elementary School Kids Johnson, A., Moher, T., Ohlsson, S., The Round Earth Project - Collaborative VR for Elementary School Kids, In the SIGGRAPH 99 conference abstracts and applications, Los Angeles, California, Aug 8-13,

More information

WE GAVE A LAWYER BASIC MATH SKILLS, AND YOU WON T BELIEVE WHAT HAPPENED NEXT

WE GAVE A LAWYER BASIC MATH SKILLS, AND YOU WON T BELIEVE WHAT HAPPENED NEXT WE GAVE A LAWYER BASIC MATH SKILLS, AND YOU WON T BELIEVE WHAT HAPPENED NEXT PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS OF RANDOM SAMPLING IN ediscovery By Matthew Verga, J.D. INTRODUCTION Anyone who spends ample time working

More information

U VA THE CHANGING FACE OF UVA STUDENTS: SSESSMENT. About The Study

U VA THE CHANGING FACE OF UVA STUDENTS: SSESSMENT. About The Study About The Study U VA SSESSMENT In 6, the University of Virginia Office of Institutional Assessment and Studies undertook a study to describe how first-year students have changed over the past four decades.

More information

The generation effect: Software demonstrating the phenomenon

The generation effect: Software demonstrating the phenomenon Behavior Research Methods, nstruments, & Computers 1999,1 (). 81-85 The generation effect: Software demonstrating the phenomenon WLLAM LANGSTON Middle Tennessee State University, Murfreesboro, Tennessee

More information

Firms and Markets Saturdays Summer I 2014

Firms and Markets Saturdays Summer I 2014 PRELIMINARY DRAFT VERSION. SUBJECT TO CHANGE. Firms and Markets Saturdays Summer I 2014 Professor Thomas Pugel Office: Room 11-53 KMC E-mail: tpugel@stern.nyu.edu Tel: 212-998-0918 Fax: 212-995-4212 This

More information

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise

A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise A Game-based Assessment of Children s Choices to Seek Feedback and to Revise Maria Cutumisu, Kristen P. Blair, Daniel L. Schwartz, Doris B. Chin Stanford Graduate School of Education Please address all

More information

Lecture 10: Reinforcement Learning

Lecture 10: Reinforcement Learning Lecture 1: Reinforcement Learning Cognitive Systems II - Machine Learning SS 25 Part III: Learning Programs and Strategies Q Learning, Dynamic Programming Lecture 1: Reinforcement Learning p. Motivation

More information

Retrieval in cued recall

Retrieval in cued recall Memory & Cognition 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 341-348 Retrieval in cued recall JOHN L. SANTA Rutgers University, Douglass College, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08903 ALAN B. RUSKIN University ofcalifornio, Irvine,

More information

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh

The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students. Iman Moradimanesh The Effect of Discourse Markers on the Speaking Production of EFL Students Iman Moradimanesh Abstract The research aimed at investigating the relationship between discourse markers (DMs) and a special

More information

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions

The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions The Good Judgment Project: A large scale test of different methods of combining expert predictions Lyle Ungar, Barb Mellors, Jon Baron, Phil Tetlock, Jaime Ramos, Sam Swift The University of Pennsylvania

More information

Group Assignment: Software Evaluation Model. Team BinJack Adam Binet Aaron Jackson

Group Assignment: Software Evaluation Model. Team BinJack Adam Binet Aaron Jackson Group Assignment: Software Evaluation Model Team BinJack Adam Binet Aaron Jackson Education 531 Assessment of Software and Information Technology Applications Submitted to: David Lloyd Cape Breton University

More information

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting

A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting A Study of the Effectiveness of Using PER-Based Reforms in a Summer Setting Turhan Carroll University of Colorado-Boulder REU Program Summer 2006 Introduction/Background Physics Education Research (PER)

More information

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities.

The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension. Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities. The Effect of Close Reading on Reading Comprehension Scores of Fifth Grade Students with Specific Learning Disabilities By Erica Blouin Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value Catherine Perez Introduction I was reaching for my daily math sheet that my school has elected to use and in big bold letters in a box it said: TO ADD NUMBERS

More information

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER

IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER IMPROVING SPEAKING SKILL OF THE TENTH GRADE STUDENTS OF SMK 17 AGUSTUS 1945 MUNCAR THROUGH DIRECT PRACTICE WITH THE NATIVE SPEAKER Mohamad Nor Shodiq Institut Agama Islam Darussalam (IAIDA) Banyuwangi

More information

+32 (0) https://lirias.kuleuven.be

+32 (0) https://lirias.kuleuven.be Citation Archived version Published version Journal homepage Vanbinst, K., Ghesquière, P. and De Smedt, B. (2012), Numerical magnitude representations and individual differences in children's arithmetic

More information

Using GIFT to Support an Empirical Study on the Impact of the Self-Reference Effect on Learning

Using GIFT to Support an Empirical Study on the Impact of the Self-Reference Effect on Learning 80 Using GIFT to Support an Empirical Study on the Impact of the Self-Reference Effect on Learning Anne M. Sinatra, Ph.D. Army Research Laboratory/Oak Ridge Associated Universities anne.m.sinatra.ctr@us.army.mil

More information

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization

Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization Phonological and Phonetic Representations: The Case of Neutralization Allard Jongman University of Kansas 1. Introduction The present paper focuses on the phenomenon of phonological neutralization to consider

More information

Rule Learning With Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness

Rule Learning With Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness Rule Learning With Negation: Issues Regarding Effectiveness S. Chua, F. Coenen, G. Malcolm University of Liverpool Department of Computer Science, Ashton Building, Ashton Street, L69 3BX Liverpool, United

More information

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC

On Human Computer Interaction, HCI. Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC On Human Computer Interaction, HCI Dr. Saif al Zahir Electrical and Computer Engineering Department UBC Human Computer Interaction HCI HCI is the study of people, computer technology, and the ways these

More information

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role Formative Assessment in Mathematics Part 3: The Learner s Role Dylan Wiliam Equals: Mathematics and Special Educational Needs 6(1) 19-22; Spring 2000 Introduction This is the last of three articles reviewing

More information

Transfer of Training

Transfer of Training Transfer of Training Objective Material : To see if Transfer of training is possible : Drawing Boar with a screen, Eight copies of a star pattern with double lines Experimenter : E and drawing pins. Subject

More information

Evaluation of Teach For America:

Evaluation of Teach For America: EA15-536-2 Evaluation of Teach For America: 2014-2015 Department of Evaluation and Assessment Mike Miles Superintendent of Schools This page is intentionally left blank. ii Evaluation of Teach For America:

More information

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time? Santa Clara University Scholar Commons Teacher Education School of Education & Counseling Psychology 11-2012 Positive turning points for girls in mathematics classrooms: Do they stand the test of time?

More information

Further, Robert W. Lissitz, University of Maryland Huynh Huynh, University of South Carolina ADEQUATE YEARLY PROGRESS

Further, Robert W. Lissitz, University of Maryland Huynh Huynh, University of South Carolina ADEQUATE YEARLY PROGRESS A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years Abstract Takang K. Tabe Department of Educational Psychology, University of Buea

More information

Guru: A Computer Tutor that Models Expert Human Tutors

Guru: A Computer Tutor that Models Expert Human Tutors Guru: A Computer Tutor that Models Expert Human Tutors Andrew Olney 1, Sidney D'Mello 2, Natalie Person 3, Whitney Cade 1, Patrick Hays 1, Claire Williams 1, Blair Lehman 1, and Art Graesser 1 1 University

More information

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity.

Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1. Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity. Running head: DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICTY 1 Examining the Impact of Frustration Levels on Multiplication Automaticity Jessica Hanna Eastern Illinois University DEVELOPING MULTIPLICATION AUTOMATICITY

More information

CHEM 6487: Problem Seminar in Inorganic Chemistry Spring 2010

CHEM 6487: Problem Seminar in Inorganic Chemistry Spring 2010 CHEM 6487: Problem Seminar in Inorganic Chemistry Spring 2010 Instructor: Dr. Stephen M. Holmes Course Time: 10 AM Friday Office Location: 418 Benton Hall Course Location: 451 Benton Hall Email: holmesst@umsl.edu

More information

Evaluating Collaboration and Core Competence in a Virtual Enterprise

Evaluating Collaboration and Core Competence in a Virtual Enterprise PsychNology Journal, 2003 Volume 1, Number 4, 391-399 Evaluating Collaboration and Core Competence in a Virtual Enterprise Rainer Breite and Hannu Vanharanta Tampere University of Technology, Pori, Finland

More information

Training Staff with Varying Abilities and Special Needs

Training Staff with Varying Abilities and Special Needs Training Staff with Varying Abilities and Special Needs by Randy Boardman and Renée Fucilla In your role as a Nonviolent Crisis Intervention Certified Instructor, it is likely that at some point you will

More information

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser

Kelli Allen. Vicki Nieter. Jeanna Scheve. Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser Kelli Allen Jeanna Scheve Vicki Nieter Foreword by Gregory J. Kaiser Table of Contents Foreword........................................... 7 Introduction........................................ 9 Learning

More information

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on

Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on Developmental Science DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00627.x REPORT Blackwell Publishing Ltd Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on processing speed visual processing Christopher W. Robinson

More information

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016

AGENDA LEARNING THEORIES LEARNING THEORIES. Advanced Learning Theories 2/22/2016 AGENDA Advanced Learning Theories Alejandra J. Magana, Ph.D. admagana@purdue.edu Introduction to Learning Theories Role of Learning Theories and Frameworks Learning Design Research Design Dual Coding Theory

More information

CSC200: Lecture 4. Allan Borodin

CSC200: Lecture 4. Allan Borodin CSC200: Lecture 4 Allan Borodin 1 / 22 Announcements My apologies for the tutorial room mixup on Wednesday. The room SS 1088 is only reserved for Fridays and I forgot that. My office hours: Tuesdays 2-4

More information

PEDAGOGICAL LEARNING WALKS: MAKING THE THEORY; PRACTICE

PEDAGOGICAL LEARNING WALKS: MAKING THE THEORY; PRACTICE PEDAGOGICAL LEARNING WALKS: MAKING THE THEORY; PRACTICE DR. BEV FREEDMAN B. Freedman OISE/Norway 2015 LEARNING LEADERS ARE Discuss and share.. THE PURPOSEFUL OF CLASSROOM/SCHOOL OBSERVATIONS IS TO OBSERVE

More information

Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital. Guidance and Information for Teachers

Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital. Guidance and Information for Teachers Dyslexia and Dyscalculia Screeners Digital Guidance and Information for Teachers Digital Tests from GL Assessment For fully comprehensive information about using digital tests from GL Assessment, please

More information

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards Ricki Sabia, JD NCSC Parent Training and Technical Assistance Specialist ricki.sabia@uky.edu Background Alternate

More information

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden)

GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden) GROUP COMPOSITION IN THE NAVIGATION SIMULATOR A PILOT STUDY Magnus Boström (Kalmar Maritime Academy, Sweden) magnus.bostrom@lnu.se ABSTRACT: At Kalmar Maritime Academy (KMA) the first-year students at

More information