CAN PICTORIAL REPRESENTATIONS SUPPORT PROPORTIONAL REASONING? THE CASE OF A MIXING PAINT PROBLEM

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "CAN PICTORIAL REPRESENTATIONS SUPPORT PROPORTIONAL REASONING? THE CASE OF A MIXING PAINT PROBLEM"

Transcription

1 CAN PICTORIAL REPRESENTATIONS SUPPORT PROPORTIONAL REASONING? THE CASE OF A MIXING PAINT PROBLEM Christina Misailidou and Julian Williams University of Manchester Abstract In this paper we report on the use of pictorial representations in ratio problems about mixing paint. Two such problems (1Paint, 2Paint) were included in a diagnostic test for ratio and proportion which was constructed in two versions: one with models thought to facilitate proportional reasoning and one without. For our study sample (N=212) of 11 to 13 years old pupils, the statistical analysis of the test data showed that for both of the 'paint' items, the version with the picture was much easier than the one without it. By interviewing selected pupils that took the test, we found that several pupils answered the paint items correctly by using the pictorial information, others did not make use of it and yet others were confused by it. We present extracts from the individual interviews that support these findings. Then we present a discussion group dialogue consisting of three pupils who provided different responses to one of the paint items when they took the test. These children were found to change their mind as a result of cognitive conflict, which was facilitated by the pictorial representation. We suggest that models can facilitate pupils development of their strategies, by facilitating communication about them. Keywords: Ratio, Proportional Reasoning, Models. INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND In our study we focus on the area of ratio and proportion, a topic in which research in mathematics education reveals that secondary school pupils experience difficulties. In previous papers (e.g. Misailidou and Williams, 2003b; 2003c) we described how we aim to contribute to teaching by developing a test that can help teachers diagnose their pupils use of the additive strategy in a variety of tasks, some easier than others, and some more suggestive of additive strategies than others. These tasks were short written test items, but with different numerical demand and different contexts known to provoke various errors. We constructed two versions of many items, one with models thought to be of service to children s thinking and one without: the aim was to compare the difficulty of the parallel items and explore the potential of models to induce conflicts or changes of strategy. In this paper we will evaluate the use of one particular model by children from our sample. Our ideas for alternative presentations of items came from the literature, and was finalised only after discussions with primary and secondary school teachers on effective ways of teaching ratio problems. Lamon (1994) suggests that the presentation of the situation in the form of pictures facilitates pupils proportional reasoning. Middleton and van den Heuvel-Panhuizen (1995) mention that the presence of tables helps children to

2 solve ratio and proportion problems and Streefland (1984) reported the use of the number line as a helpful tool in the classroom teaching of ratio. Finally, some teachers that we have interviewed suggested the use of models (like cubes) that would help children reason proportionally. We decided to investigate whether models could be implemented in test items, and if so to what effect. We thought that if our test data revealed a significant difference in children s responses to items with or without models then we could use these productively in conflict discussions with children (as in Ryan and Williams, 2002). We drew on the literature to find models suitable for use in test conditions, showed an initial list of models to teachers to comment on their suitability and hypothetical effectiveness and finally we came up with four categories of models: pictorial representations (two types), tables, double number lines and diagrams. A limitation of our approach due to the scale of our study is that we were not able to exhaust all the possible alternative presentations of the items (e.g. all the possible pictorial representations). In any case our aim was not to research the comparative effectiveness of all the models available but to identify some tools for facilitating conflict discussion and classroom teaching in the context of ratio. Initially, we tested an item bank that consisted of 38 items in total. 24 such items were adopted with slight modifications from previous research studies and others have been created based on findings of that research. For 13 of these items we created an additional, alternative version, taking advantage of the models mentioned above. (Misailidou and Williams, 2003c) The statistical (Rasch) analysis of some preliminary test results of our study showed that the presence of models affected the difficulty of both easy and difficult items (Misailidou and Williams 2002a) and the statistical analysis of all our test results showed that the pictorial representations and the use of the double number lines were more successful than the rest of our models in supporting ratio reasoning (Misailidou and Williams, 2003b; in press). On the other hand, the qualitative analysis of the test data complemented by individual interviews showed that even the successful models helped as well as hindered individual pupils in their investigations. They definitely though influenced the strategies that pupils used to solve the problems and the explanations they gave for them (Misailidou and Williams, in press). A Rasch analysis of the data combined with interview data allowed us to select the most interesting diagnostically items and test how they would behave as a test. The resulting diagnostic test was given to a new sample of 212 pupils aged 10 to 13 in two schools in the north west of England in two linked forms: a with-models version (13 items) and a without-models version (13 items). Both of the test versions can be seen in Misailidou and Williams, 2003c. In each class, half of the pupils were given the without-models version and half the with-models one and the data were subjected again to a Rasch analysis. This analysis allowed us, amongst other things, to examine the difficulty of the same item in its with-model and without-model form. This was possible since the two test forms were linked through 5 common items. Table 1 presents the difficulty estimates for the two versions of our items (with and without-models) from the scale which measures ratio reasoning ability. The graph shows that for most items, the parallel forms were equally difficult, that is, there was no significant difference in difficulty of the with-models and without-models versions of items. There were a few exceptions, and the most extreme were the Paint

3 items. The difference in difficulty between the two forms of the 1Paint item was approximately 1.5 logits and the same difference for the 2Paint item was more than a half logit (0.64) which is significant enough for children of this age. Item s' Difficulty Campers Printing Press 1 Campers With models version Fruits'Price Books' Price 2 Paint 1 Paint Class W ithout models version -4 Table 1: Scatter plot of performance for with-models and without-models items Is in this paper we focus on the effect that a pictorial representation has on a mixing paint task. Our research question is: How do (untutored) children sometimes use (pictorial) models to support their proportional reasoning? In order to answer it, we complement the results from the test data with analyses of selected individual interview data. Based on the analyses of these data we planned and conducted group discussions and we present here the results of the analysis for one of them. PRESENTATION OF THE PAINT TASKS AND THE MODELS The Paint items that were used in our test have been adapted from Tourniaire s (1986) study. One of the original items that Tourniaire (1986) used was Sue and Jenny want to paint together. They need the same colour. Sue uses 1 can of yellow paint and 3 cans of blue paint. Jenny uses 5 cans of yellow paint. How much blue does she need?

4 By adapting this we came up with the item that we call 1Paint and which is presented below: Sue and Jenny want to paint together. They want to use each exactly the same colour. Sue uses 3 cans of yellow paint and 6 cans of red paint. Jenny uses 7 cans of yellow paint. How much red paint does Jenny need? Generally, two kinds of ratios can be compared in a given proportion. The ratio of quantities of the same nature, which denotes the scalar relationship in the proportion, and the ratio of quantities of different natures, which denotes the functional relationship. In item 1Paint the easier ratio is the functional one (3:6). This ratio, which involves one quantity that evenly divides another, is called an integer ratio The 2Paint item has exactly the same wording as 1Paint but different numerical structure: the functional ratio is (3:5) and the scalar, which is the easier in this item, is (5:20) Tourniaire (1986) comments that: The presence of a mixture does appear to increase the difficulty of problems. It may be because mixture situations are less familiar to the subjects, because continuous quantities are involved, or because mixtures are more difficult to conceptualise. (p. 408) Hence, we decided to enrich the Paint items with a pictorial representation, the idea for which came from the textbook SMP 11-16, Ratio 1 (1983): Sue and Jenny want to paint together. They want to use each exactly the same colour. Sue uses 3 cans of yellow paint and 6 cans of red paint. 3 cans of yellow paint 6 cans of red paint Jenny uses 7 cans of yellow paint. 7 cans of yellow paint How many cans of red paint?? How much red paint does Jenny need?

5 We hypothesized that by providing the pictures of the cans we introduce a discrete quantisation in the continuous paint context and with the additional diagram we attract the pupils to the easier integer ratio. The latter may well account for the difference in difficulty of the two modes of presentation of the item: perhaps this model is in itself of little help to the children, as was found with many of the other models. This was a key question for the interviews in the next stage. METHODOLOGY In addition to these test analyses, we drew on semi-structured clinical interviews and semi-structured small group interviews with selected pupils about the test items. By analysing and coding the textual responses to the paint models version of the tests, we found the following categories: 1. Nothing written on the pictorial part of the question whatsoever. 2. Numbers only written on the pictorial part, mainly the answer to the problem. 3. Simple drawings of cans are drawn, mainly a number of cans denoting the answer to the problem. 4. A construction drawing is presented which illustrates a method/strategy From each of these categories we selected randomly pupils for individual interviews and the final available sample was 23 pupils. The purpose of the individual interviews was to allow us to validate the items, to confirm our interpretations about the strategies that were used to solve them and to gain a deeper understanding of the way the pupils made use of the model that was provided to them. We decided that a semi-structured type of individual interviews would better serve our purpose and so we constructed a simple interview guide, which consisted of three main points of inquiry rather than specific questions: 1. What was the method that the pupil used to obtain their answer. 2. Whether the pupil had used the picture 3. How the pupil had used the picture. Thus, it was possible for the pupils to influence the interview agenda and provide possibly more and better quality information. We analysed the individual interview data by searching through them for emerging themes or patterns (Taylor and Bogdan, 1984) rather than imposing on the data an existing categorisation. The purpose of the group interviews was again to validate the test items and confirm our interpretations about the pupils strategies but most importantly we wanted to investigate whether conflict groups can learn to reason proportionally through discussion and with the help of appropriate tools. Again the interview guide did not consist of specific questions but rather it was a general structure of each interview: Part 1: The interviewer asks each pupil to present her or his answer and method for obtaining the answer. If possible the presentations starts with the most primitive answer/method, moves on to more sophisticated methods and ends with the correct answer and method. After each pupil s presentation another pupil is asked to repeat what s/he had just heard so that the interviewer makes sure that everyone understands everyone s method

6 Part 2: The pupils are prompted to discuss/compare/discard/defend answers and methods. During this discussion when the interviewer senses that the pupils need help with their explanations, distributes the tool (a sheet of paper with pictures) that is supposed to facilitate their thinking. Part 3: The pupils are asked to write down what was their original answer, if they had changed their minds during their discussion and why. This structure was meant to be only indicative and the pupils were allowed to influence the interview agenda more than the case of the individual interviews. RESULTS Effect of the models-individual interview data Based on some preliminary qualitative analyses of pupils work in the scripts and their individual interviews, we reported (Misailidou and Williams, 2002b) that the addition of pictures sometimes helped and sometimes hindered the pupils depending on the item they were dealing with. Here, we examine more systematically these interviews and scripts. The analysis of the individual interviews provided us with three hypothetical categories of the way that the pupils interacted with the model that was provided to them in the test: Category 1: No evidence of use of the pictorial model Category 2: Use of the pictorial model to keep track of the numbers and/or operations (tally marks etc) Category 3: Use of the pictorial model to represent or help develop a strategy Thus, we assign pupils like Keith and Jane below, to Category 3, which means that they used the model in a way that essentially affected their answer. Keith, for example, was helped by the pictures as shows his test script in Figure 1 to keep track of the numbers and so to find the correct answer 14 for the item 1Paint: Figure 1

7 When he was interviewed and asked to explain his answer, Keith said that: Keith: What I worked out is one equals two red, so I worked it out here as well (shows the pictures) so I thought one can of yellow paint is two red so it s seven so we should have fourteen so that s what I put down. Interviewer: OK and I see here that you drew some cans. Why did you do that? Keith: So I don t get mixed up. I drew one there (shows the yellow paint circle) and then I drew two there. So I can work it out. Because I put seven there (shows the yellow paint circle again) and then I worked it out there (shows the red paint circle) Keith suggests it was easier for him to figure out the ratio 1:2 by drawing cans of paint, then work on the cans, find the answer and then report with numbers his actions on the cans: a classic use of a modelling tool to keep track of what is going on! Jane, on the other hand did not use the cans in the same way. Figure 2

8 Jane s script is shown in Figure 2. She gave the incorrect answer 18 to the problem 2Paint which is the result of using the constant difference or additive strategy. This is a frequently used error strategy where the relationship within the ratios is computed by subtracting one term from another, and then the difference is applied to the second ratio. (Tourniaire & Pulos 1985, p.186) In this particular problem, the answer 18 can be obtained either by thinking that 5+15=20 so 3+15=18 or by thinking that 3+2=5 and so 20-2=18. When Jane was interviewed she explained her work as: Jane: There is 3 and there is 5 there so you take 2 off there so you take 2 off 20 and 18 is your answer. Interviewer: I can see that you ve crossed out 2 cans Jane: If you take 2 cans off here (she shows the circle with the 5 cans) you get the same as there (she shows the circle with the 3 cans) so there was 20 cans I took off 2 and then I found out that it was 18 cans left. Later in the interview she was asked whether she has used the model: Interviewer: In this problem they are not only giving us the numbers here, they are giving us pictures as well. Did you use them in any way? Jane: Yeah I tried to check in the picture how many to take off to get the right answer. Jane was not helped by the model to find the correct answer 12. Instead she used the additive strategy and found the answer 18, and then confirmed this by taking away in the model. We do not believe that the model provoked her additive approach but it might have acted as a visual confirmation for her approach. In neither case (Jane or Keith) did the model appear to be instrumental in helping the child to identify a strategy, but in both cases they were able to use the picture to communicate more clearly what they were doing. We assign Pupils like David and Sarah to Category 2 which means that although we cannot claim that their strategy was essentially influenced by the model, they noticed it, and maybe they used it to keep track of their operations. In addition, the model served to externalise their strategy on their inscriptions, and hence may have supported them, and helped them in confirming their adopted strategies (whether correct or not). David gave the incorrect answer 2 to the 1 Paint question. He used the constant sum strategy (we adopted this term from Mellar, 1987) to answer the problem. He thought that the sum of Sue s cans should be equal to the sum of Jenny s cans: 3+6=9 therefore 7+2=9 and so the answer should be 2. He did not draw anything on the picture of his test he just wrote the number 2 near the question mark. When he was interviewed and was asked to explain his answer he explained: David: I counted how many cans it was altogether Um six cans of red and three cans of yellow [he means Sue s cans] And Jenny uses 7 cans of yellow. She can only use 2 cans of red paint. Interviewer: OK. How did you come up with this idea?

9 David: Because Sue can use six reds and three yellows and that equals nine so she is only using nine cans of paint. Jenny uses seven, so we got 2 cans of red, she can only use 2 cans of red. Then the interviewer tried to investigate whether the pictures influenced his decision. David commented on the pictures of cans: David: I used them. There were these lots of cans and these lots of cans Interviewer: So you actually prefer the problem with pictures? David: Yes. Interviewer: Did they help you find the solution at the end? David: Yes I think so. Because I wasn t adding the three and the six I was looking at the cans altogether and using the cans instead of the writing more David stresses that he used the pictures and from his interview it seems that he probably preferred the problem presented in a pictorial rather than a plain form. It is possible that his concentration on the gestalt of the picture helps to encourage him to adopt the constant sum strategy: he may be distracted from the logic which is wrapped up in the verbalisation of the task. But then he may be facilitated in performing the necessary operations to execute his strategy of adding and subtracting. Sarah gave the correct answer 12 to the 2 Paint question. Her script can be seen in Figure 3: Figure 3

10 When Sarah was interviewed and was asked to explain her answer she said: Sarah: John s got 5 cans of green paint and then George s got 20 and it says they want to use exactly the same colour. So to get from 5 to 20 you have to times by 4. And then John s got 3 cans of yellow paint, how many would George have? So I timesed by 4 to get the answer because I timesed by 4 there. Interviewer: OK. And here what made you decide on using that method? Sarah: We have been doing in class stuff about how to get from one answer to another you have to like times by 4 or by 5 or whatever or I just thought to do it here as well. Then the interviewer tried to investigate whether Sarah has used the pictures to solve the problem: Interviewer: So, I see here that you wrote beside the arrow and I see that you made an arrow here as well? Why did you do these? Sarah: It helped me. Interviewer: So if it wasn t here, what do you think? Sarah: I probably wouldn t have got it I don t know what I would have done! Sarah, just like David, seems to like the pictorial version of the problem. She doesn t actually use the picture to develop the times strategy, which she says comes to her from what we have been doing in class and stuff. But she does use it to organise and record the times operations on the appropriate numbers, and her inscription does appear to confirm her answer. Pupils like Anna and Chris below belong to Category 1, which means that there is no evidence neither in their scripts nor in their interviews that they had used the pictorial model in any way. Anna gave the answer 10 to the 1 Paint question by using an additive approach and she did not write anything to the pictorial part of the problem in her script. Her explanation for obtaining 10 was: Anna: 3 add 7 is 10 so the answer is 10 cans. Interviewer: OK. What helped you think of that answer? Anna: I ve noticed that 6 minus 3 equals 3 so then I ve used the same amount of paint Interviewer: So, it was the numbers that made you think of the answer? Anna: Yeah. When she was asked about the model she appears not to have used it at all Interviewer: OK. Why do you think the cans of paint are painted here? Anna: Probably to help cause you can count them if you want. Interviewer: Did you use this picture to find your answer? Anna: I don t know probably not.

11 Chris gave the correct answer 12 to the 2 Paint problem and just like Anna he did not write anything to the pictorial part of the problem in his script and in his interview he admits not to have used the pictures. First he explained how he found 12: Chris: The green paint was originally five and it was timesed by four to become twenty so I thought it would be the same with the three, the three cans of yellow paint. And later he comments on the model: Interviewer: So here, there is a picture with some cans. Was this picture confusing or helpful or it didn t matter at all having it? Chris: I don t think it would have mattered. In summary, the qualitative analysis of the test data complemented by individual interviews provides a different aspect of the use of models than the statistical analysis of the test data. Judging from their interviews pupils fall in three categories in respect of how they used the pictorial model and it must be noted that in all three categories belong pupils that give correct as well as incorrect answers. Rather few pupils belong to Category 1 though: from the 23 pupils that we have interviewed only 3 explicitly admitted not to have used the model at all. We propose that the analysis shows that the pictorial models influenced one way or another the selection or use of the pupils strategies and the explanations they gave for them. Presentation of a small group interview We have already reported results for the case of a group working on the 1Paint item. By studying this case, we concluded that one of the factors that brought a successful change of mind of two pupils, who had given incorrect answers to this item in the test, was the pictorial representation of the problem (the same one that was used in the test) that was presented to them during the discussion (Misailidou and Williams, 2003). Informed by the results of the above group discussion we designed and then conducted the group discussion that we present in this paper. Three 11 year old pupils, Alan, Victor and Soula were selected form the same class to form a discussion group because they had provided three different responses on the 1 Paint item: 2, 10 and 14 respectively. Alan and Victor worked on the non model version of the test and Soula on the model one. The discussion, with the guidance of one of the authors (who is denoted below as the interviewer ), lasted approximately 1 hour and was audio taped. Initially, the pupils were asked to recall their responses by consulting their test scripts. They were also invited to present an argument for their response to the group. Alan explained his answer 2, which was the result of the use of the constant sum strategy, as 3 add 6 is 9 so then 7 add 2 equals 9 so then the cans are equal so the answer is 2 cans of red paint. Victor could not repeat, when asked, Alan s method so at this point the interviewer distributed the sheet that is shown in Figure 4, as a tool that was supposed to facilitate

12 discussion. In Figure 4 Soula s sheet can be seen after the discussion ended. The drawings on it come not only by Soula but by the other pupils and the interviewer as well. Figure 4 The interviewer encouraged the pupils to use the sheet as an aid to their explanations and she tried to use it herself as much as possible during the discussion. Indeed, it is notable that from now on in the discussion there were continual examples of the way the picture is used by the children and the interviewer to help explain methods and working. The references to the picture or elements of the picture are signalled by the use of that,

13 this, them, here, there and she throughout the discourse, and of course these were accompanied by gestures pointing to the elements represented. Having this visual aid and some time to think about it, Victor was able to repeat Alan s method and then he explained his own method, which is the additive strategy to obtain 10: If Sue got 3 of yellow and 6 of red and Jenny got 7 she got 4 more here [points to the picture] and so she needs 4 more there [points to the picture] so it s 10. Finally Soula explained her answer 14: Sue has 3 cans of yellow paint [she points to the pictures] she s got 6 cans of red paint Jenny s got 7 cans of yellow paint and add another 7 which is 14 cause I thought if 3 and 3 will be 6 then 7 and 7 is 14. Then the interviewer invited the pupils to consider all three answers and explanations and share with each other their opinions. At one point Alan commented: Alan: I think Victor s is the best one you have to find how many would Jenny how many would she need to make it equal with them [points to Sue s cans] so I don t think you have to double that to find out So Victor s method is better. And a little later Alan: Yeah Soula says that 3 and 3 is 6 and 6 there [points to the picture] so the answer is going to be 14 so one is going to have 14 cans of paint and one is only going to have 6 Interviewer: Yes Alan: So it wouldn t be right we have to make it even Alan s use of the phrases make it equal and make it even brought up a discussion about the wording of the problem. Some parts of it are presented below: Interviewer: Maybe we should go back to the problem all of us and see what do they say to us. So, what colours do they need? Victor: Red and yellow. Interviewer: Soula? What does the problem say? Soula: It says they want to use each exactly the same colour. Interviewer: So, what do they mean? Soula: That they want to have equal cans of paint. Interviewer: What this colour is going to look like when they finish? What colour are they going to see? Victor: It might be pink.- Soula: I think that if they want to use exactly the same on each can they will have four different walls in each room and one of them would be yellow, one would be red, another will be yellow, another will be red. Victor: They might have mixed the yellow

14 Interviewer: If you mix the yellow and red you get Victor: Orange I think. Soula: I think they are painting separately. Interviewer: But when they say they want to use each exactly the same colour and not colours then they are going to use one colour or not? What do you think Alan? Are they going to paint the walls separately or not? Alan: They might have the exact same amount. It is obvious that the wording of this specific problem provides opportunities for exploring different interpretations: not mixing paint and not using ratio and proportion to find the answer or mixing paint and using proportional reasoning. Since, in addition to not using proportionality when needed, the misuse of proportionality in non-proportional situations is a classical misconception as mentioned by De Bock, Van Dooren, Janssens and Verschaffel (2002) we believe that problems that encourage such explorations as the above help pupils to clarify the essence of the proportional relationships. Furthermore, problems in real life situations-in contrast to many school textbook problems- have often an ambivalent context that must be deciphered, just like the problem used here. After the clarifications about the context Soula commented Soula: It really depends on what shade of orange do you want because if you mix them together then it will be a dark orange This last comment by Soula gives the opportunity to reject the answer 2 Interviewer: [pointing to Sue s cans in the picture] If you mix this red and this yellow what sort of orange do you think it would make? Alan: It would be dark because there s more red than yellow. Interviewer: OK. [Addressing Alan] Your answer to the problem was 2 reds, wasn t it? Alan: Yeah. Interviewer: So if they have 2 red and 7 yellows, what sort of orange would they make? Soula: A lighter- Alan: Yeah a lighter orange yeah cause there s way more yellow than red Interviewer: So could they use 2 cans? All of them: No. Then Victor explains why 14 should also be rejected as an answer: Victor: Having here [points to the picture] 14 it would be a dark orange! Interviewer: And this is what?

15 Victor: [On the same time pointing to the picture] That one [he points to the answer 14] is one that would make a darker orange than that [he points to Sue s cans in the picture]! Interviewer: Ah you feel like if there is 14 here [points to the picture] it s going to be darker than the shade that we want. So how many cans do you feel that we should have here [points to the picture]? Victor: 10. Because she s [points to Sue s cans in the picture] got three more red and if she [points to Jenny s cans in the picture] had 10 she would have three more. Interviewer: What do you think Soula? Soula: I think that s OK. Interviewer: So you feel that we will have the same shade if here [points to the picture] we have 10. OK? What do you feel Alan? Alan: I think that s OK At this point of the discussion all the three pupils were convinced that the answer 10 which was the product of the additive method was the correct one. The pupils having reached an agreement could not see the point of continuing the discussion and so the interviewer brought up a new element that was proposed by a pupil in a previous group discussion in order to provoke cognitive conflict. (Misailidou and Williams, 2003a). Interviewer: Let me ask you something else. [She draws on Soula s pictures-sheet but makes sure that the others can see as well] If we had 1 can here of yellow paint let us see what would happen with the two methods. [Addressing Victor] With your method, how many cans of red paint would you have? Victor: I think 4. Interviewer: 4. Yes. [She draws 4 cans on the picture sheet] And with that method how many cans of red paint would we have? Soula: 2. Interviewer: [she draws 2 cans on the picture sheet] So, this method gives us 4 and this method gives us 2. So, do you think these two colours are the same? Victor: [Points to the two cans of red and 1 can of yellow on the pictures] That one is lighter. Interviewer: That one is lighter. But comparing to these [shows Sue s cans], which of these two gives us the same shade as this? Victor: This [points to the 4 cans of red and 1 can of yellow] Soula: This [points to the 4 cans of red and 1 can of yellow as well] This new element did not provoke cognitive conflict so the interviewer attempted a resource to zero (0) strategy Interviewer: So, if we have 3 cans of red paint [on the same time she draws the cans in the picture], with your method [she means the additive method] how many cans of yellow paint should we have?

16 Victor: None? Interviewer: It s none. [Victor draws on the picture something that denotes an empty can and the interviewer writes in it the word none ] Would that give us the same colour as here? If we had none? Would that give us the same colour as here if we had none? Victor: It depends which method we are using. Interviewer: With this method if we had 3 cans of red paint we would have what? Victor: 1? Interviewer: Not 1. 1 and a half plus 1 and a half how much is it? Victor: 3 [Soula and the interviewer draw 1 and a half cans on the picture] Interviewer: So, what do you think now? Victor: I don t know. Interviewer: With this [point to the picture] method we have red. We don t have orange. At this point probably the pupils considered too absurd the situation of having none as an answer and red as a colour so they started thinking again the context of the problem: Soula: I don t think they are going to mix them [points to the cans] together Alan: Yeah Interviewer: But as we agreed earlier if we suppose that they are mixing them? Victor: Then mine is not right! Soula: Yeah. Interviewer: Alan? Alan: With Victor s method we can t get an answer at all! Unfortunately at this point the bell rang and the session ended. We believe more discussion was needed in order for the pupils to understand why the additive method does not work. Nevertheless, a change of mind was achieved and the generative element of the discourse which supported it was the interviewer s introduction of a conflict strategy. It cannot be assumed that the pictorial model generated a multiplicative construction and in fact during the discussion the model afforded explanations of correct and incorrect strategies. It did appear though to help the children to recognize the relative darkness of combinations of yellow and red (like for example, (1, 4) compared to (3, 6)) CONCLUSION Our aim was to complement what has already been reported on the children s proportional reasoning by examining the effect of a pictorial model on pupils strategies while attempting to solve a rather difficult type of ratio task: the one which involves mixing of paint. We found that pupils who as a rule were not taught to use pictorial models- were influenced in their strategies when the pictorial model was presented to them in test conditions.

17 We then found when the use of the pictorial model was suggested to pupils in small group discussions by the interviewer that some children (and the interviewer) could use the pictorial model to represent their strategy, and to facilitate discussion and argument. Learning was facilitated by the resolution of conflict, the arguments for which were successfully backed by the use of the model. In conclusion, we believe that pictorial models could facilitate pupils' development of proportional reasoning but that this is mainly through facilitating communication of strategies. Hence the role of the teacher or other sources for managing conflict is crucial in ensuring that the models are used productively by the pupils. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT We gratefully acknowledge the financial support of the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC), Award Number R REFERENCES De Bock, D., Van Dooren, W., Janssens, D. and Verschaffel, L.: 2002, Improper use of linear reasoning: An in depth study of the nature and the irresistibility of secondary school students errors. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 50, Lamon, S.: 1994, Ratio and proportion: Cognitive foundations in unitizing and norming. In G. Harel and J. Confrey (eds.), The Development of Multiplicative Reasoning in the Learning of Mathematics (pp ). Albany: State University of New York Press. Mellar, G.: 1987, The understanding of proportion in young adults: Investigating and teaching a formal level skill through the use of a logo microworld. PhD Thesis. King's College. University of London. Middleton, J. and van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M.: 1995, The ratio table. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 1(4), Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: 2002a, Investigating children s thinking on the topic of ratio. Proceedings of the British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics (BSRLM), 22(1), Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: 2002b, Diagnostic teaching and ratio: The effect of pictorial representations on strategies. Proceedings of the 26th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME), 1, 299. Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: 2003a, Children s arguments in discussion of a difficult ratio problem: The role of a pictorial representation. Proceedings of the 3 rd Conference of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education (CERME3) Also in La Lettre de la Preuve (International Newsletter on the Teaching and Learning of Mathematical Proof), Printemps03.html Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: 2003b, Measuring children's proportional reasoning, the tendency for an additive strategy and the effect of models. Proceedings of the 27th

18 Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME), 3, Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: 2003c, Diagnostic assessment of children s proportional reasoning. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22, Misailidou, C. and Williams, J.: In press, Children s proportional reasoning and tendency for an additive strategy: The Role of Models. Research in Mathematics Education, 5. SMP : 1983, Ratio 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ryan, J. and Williams, J.: 2002, Charting argumentation space in conceptual locales: Tools at the interface between research and practice. Research in Mathematics Education, 4, Streefland, L.: 1984, Search for the roots of ratio: Some thoughts on the long term learning process (Towards a theory) Part I: Reflections on a teaching experiment. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15(4), Taylor, S. and Bogdan, R.: 1984, Introduction to Qualitative Research Methods: The Search for Meanings. New York: Wiley. Tourniaire, F.: 1986, Proportions in elementary school. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 17(4), Tourniaire, F. and Pulos, S.: 1985, Proportional reasoning: A review of the literature. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16, (2),

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 197 ( 2015 )

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 197 ( 2015 ) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 197 ( 2015 ) 113 119 7th World Conference on Educational Sciences, (WCES-2015), 05-07 February 2015, Novotel

More information

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I

Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I RP7-1 Using Proportions to Solve Percentage Problems I Pages 46 48 Standards: 7.RP.A. Goals: Students will write equivalent statements for proportions by keeping track of the part and the whole, and by

More information

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic

Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic Developing a concrete-pictorial-abstract model for negative number arithmetic Jai Sharma and Doreen Connor Nottingham Trent University Research findings and assessment results persistently identify negative

More information

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems

An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems An ICT environment to assess and support students mathematical problem-solving performance in non-routine puzzle-like word problems Angeliki Kolovou* Marja van den Heuvel-Panhuizen*# Arthur Bakker* Iliada

More information

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Primary Mathematics: A Case Study of Two Teachers Monica Baker University of Melbourne mbaker@huntingtower.vic.edu.au Helen Chick University of Melbourne h.chick@unimelb.edu.au

More information

Contents. Foreword... 5

Contents. Foreword... 5 Contents Foreword... 5 Chapter 1: Addition Within 0-10 Introduction... 6 Two Groups and a Total... 10 Learn Symbols + and =... 13 Addition Practice... 15 Which is More?... 17 Missing Items... 19 Sums with

More information

Case study Norway case 1

Case study Norway case 1 Case study Norway case 1 School : B (primary school) Theme: Science microorganisms Dates of lessons: March 26-27 th 2015 Age of students: 10-11 (grade 5) Data sources: Pre- and post-interview with 1 teacher

More information

West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition

West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition Study Guide to accompany West s Paralegal Today The Legal Team at Work Third Edition Roger LeRoy Miller Institute for University Studies Mary Meinzinger Urisko Madonna University Prepared by Bradene L.

More information

Lecturing Module

Lecturing Module Lecturing: What, why and when www.facultydevelopment.ca Lecturing Module What is lecturing? Lecturing is the most common and established method of teaching at universities around the world. The traditional

More information

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets

Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets Ohio s Learning Standards-Clear Learning Targets Math Grade 1 Use addition and subtraction within 20 to solve word problems involving situations of 1.OA.1 adding to, taking from, putting together, taking

More information

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand

Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand Grade 2: Using a Number Line to Order and Compare Numbers Place Value Horizontal Content Strand Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills (TEKS): (2.1) Number, operation, and quantitative reasoning. The student

More information

E-3: Check for academic understanding

E-3: Check for academic understanding Respond instructively After you check student understanding, it is time to respond - through feedback and follow-up questions. Doing this allows you to gauge how much students actually comprehend and push

More information

Why Pay Attention to Race?

Why Pay Attention to Race? Why Pay Attention to Race? Witnessing Whiteness Chapter 1 Workshop 1.1 1.1-1 Dear Facilitator(s), This workshop series was carefully crafted, reviewed (by a multiracial team), and revised with several

More information

Unit 2. A whole-school approach to numeracy across the curriculum

Unit 2. A whole-school approach to numeracy across the curriculum Unit 2 A whole-school approach to numeracy across the curriculum 50 Numeracy across the curriculum Unit 2 Crown copyright 2001 Unit 2 A whole-school approach to numeracy across the curriculum Objectives

More information

RESPONSE TO LITERATURE

RESPONSE TO LITERATURE RESPONSE TO LITERATURE TEACHER PACKET CENTRAL VALLEY SCHOOL DISTRICT WRITING PROGRAM Teacher Name RESPONSE TO LITERATURE WRITING DEFINITION AND SCORING GUIDE/RUBRIC DE INITION A Response to Literature

More information

Chapter 4 - Fractions

Chapter 4 - Fractions . Fractions Chapter - Fractions 0 Michelle Manes, University of Hawaii Department of Mathematics These materials are intended for use with the University of Hawaii Department of Mathematics Math course

More information

Mathematics process categories

Mathematics process categories Mathematics process categories All of the UK curricula define multiple categories of mathematical proficiency that require students to be able to use and apply mathematics, beyond simple recall of facts

More information

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS

CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS CONCEPT MAPS AS A DEVICE FOR LEARNING DATABASE CONCEPTS Pirjo Moen Department of Computer Science P.O. Box 68 FI-00014 University of Helsinki pirjo.moen@cs.helsinki.fi http://www.cs.helsinki.fi/pirjo.moen

More information

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies

Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies Most of us are not what we could be. We are less. We have great capacity. But most of it is dormant; most is undeveloped. Improvement in thinking is like

More information

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role

Formative Assessment in Mathematics. Part 3: The Learner s Role Formative Assessment in Mathematics Part 3: The Learner s Role Dylan Wiliam Equals: Mathematics and Special Educational Needs 6(1) 19-22; Spring 2000 Introduction This is the last of three articles reviewing

More information

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts.

Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts. Recommendation 1 Build on students informal understanding of sharing and proportionality to develop initial fraction concepts. Students come to kindergarten with a rudimentary understanding of basic fraction

More information

Interpreting ACER Test Results

Interpreting ACER Test Results Interpreting ACER Test Results This document briefly explains the different reports provided by the online ACER Progressive Achievement Tests (PAT). More detailed information can be found in the relevant

More information

Functional Skills Mathematics Level 2 assessment

Functional Skills Mathematics Level 2 assessment Functional Skills Mathematics Level 2 assessment www.cityandguilds.com September 2015 Version 1.0 Marking scheme ONLINE V2 Level 2 Sample Paper 4 Mark Represent Analyse Interpret Open Fixed S1Q1 3 3 0

More information

(I couldn t find a Smartie Book) NEW Grade 5/6 Mathematics: (Number, Statistics and Probability) Title Smartie Mathematics

(I couldn t find a Smartie Book) NEW Grade 5/6 Mathematics: (Number, Statistics and Probability) Title Smartie Mathematics (I couldn t find a Smartie Book) NEW Grade 5/6 Mathematics: (Number, Statistics and Probability) Title Smartie Mathematics Lesson/ Unit Description Questions: How many Smarties are in a box? Is it the

More information

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators

Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators Evidence-based Practice: A Workshop for Training Adult Basic Education, TANF and One Stop Practitioners and Program Administrators May 2007 Developed by Cristine Smith, Beth Bingman, Lennox McLendon and

More information

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation

1 3-5 = Subtraction - a binary operation High School StuDEnts ConcEPtions of the Minus Sign Lisa L. Lamb, Jessica Pierson Bishop, and Randolph A. Philipp, Bonnie P Schappelle, Ian Whitacre, and Mindy Lewis - describe their research with students

More information

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students

P-4: Differentiate your plans to fit your students Putting It All Together: Middle School Examples 7 th Grade Math 7 th Grade Science SAM REHEARD, DC 99 7th Grade Math DIFFERENTATION AROUND THE WORLD My first teaching experience was actually not as a Teach

More information

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS

AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS AGS THE GREAT REVIEW GAME FOR PRE-ALGEBRA (CD) CORRELATED TO CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS 1 CALIFORNIA CONTENT STANDARDS: Chapter 1 ALGEBRA AND WHOLE NUMBERS Algebra and Functions 1.4 Students use algebraic

More information

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING

WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING AND TEACHING OF PROBLEM SOLVING From Proceedings of Physics Teacher Education Beyond 2000 International Conference, Barcelona, Spain, August 27 to September 1, 2000 WHY SOLVE PROBLEMS? INTERVIEWING COLLEGE FACULTY ABOUT THE LEARNING

More information

The Task. A Guide for Tutors in the Rutgers Writing Centers Written and edited by Michael Goeller and Karen Kalteissen

The Task. A Guide for Tutors in the Rutgers Writing Centers Written and edited by Michael Goeller and Karen Kalteissen The Task A Guide for Tutors in the Rutgers Writing Centers Written and edited by Michael Goeller and Karen Kalteissen Reading Tasks As many experienced tutors will tell you, reading the texts and understanding

More information

Introduction to the Practice of Statistics

Introduction to the Practice of Statistics Chapter 1: Looking at Data Distributions Introduction to the Practice of Statistics Sixth Edition David S. Moore George P. McCabe Bruce A. Craig Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing and

More information

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham

Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives. Gwenanne Salkind. George Mason University EDCI 856. Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives Gwenanne Salkind George Mason University EDCI 856 Dr. Patricia Moyer-Packenham Spring 2006 Curriculum Design Project with Virtual Manipulatives Table

More information

Activity 2 Multiplying Fractions Math 33. Is it important to have common denominators when we multiply fraction? Why or why not?

Activity 2 Multiplying Fractions Math 33. Is it important to have common denominators when we multiply fraction? Why or why not? Activity Multiplying Fractions Math Your Name: Partners Names:.. (.) Essential Question: Think about the question, but don t answer it. You will have an opportunity to answer this question at the end of

More information

Students Understanding of Graphical Vector Addition in One and Two Dimensions

Students Understanding of Graphical Vector Addition in One and Two Dimensions Eurasian J. Phys. Chem. Educ., 3(2):102-111, 2011 journal homepage: http://www.eurasianjournals.com/index.php/ejpce Students Understanding of Graphical Vector Addition in One and Two Dimensions Umporn

More information

South Carolina English Language Arts

South Carolina English Language Arts South Carolina English Language Arts A S O F J U N E 2 0, 2 0 1 0, T H I S S TAT E H A D A D O P T E D T H E CO M M O N CO R E S TAT E S TA N DA R D S. DOCUMENTS REVIEWED South Carolina Academic Content

More information

Just in Time to Flip Your Classroom Nathaniel Lasry, Michael Dugdale & Elizabeth Charles

Just in Time to Flip Your Classroom Nathaniel Lasry, Michael Dugdale & Elizabeth Charles Just in Time to Flip Your Classroom Nathaniel Lasry, Michael Dugdale & Elizabeth Charles With advocates like Sal Khan and Bill Gates 1, flipped classrooms are attracting an increasing amount of media and

More information

Paper Reference. Edexcel GCSE Mathematics (Linear) 1380 Paper 1 (Non-Calculator) Foundation Tier. Monday 6 June 2011 Afternoon Time: 1 hour 30 minutes

Paper Reference. Edexcel GCSE Mathematics (Linear) 1380 Paper 1 (Non-Calculator) Foundation Tier. Monday 6 June 2011 Afternoon Time: 1 hour 30 minutes Centre No. Candidate No. Paper Reference 1 3 8 0 1 F Paper Reference(s) 1380/1F Edexcel GCSE Mathematics (Linear) 1380 Paper 1 (Non-Calculator) Foundation Tier Monday 6 June 2011 Afternoon Time: 1 hour

More information

1. Professional learning communities Prelude. 4.2 Introduction

1. Professional learning communities Prelude. 4.2 Introduction 1. Professional learning communities 1.1. Prelude The teachers from the first prelude, come together for their first meeting Cristina: Willem: Cristina: Tomaž: Rik: Marleen: Barbara: Rik: Tomaž: Marleen:

More information

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document. Title Mathematical problem solving in Singapore schools Author(s) Berinderjeet Kaur Source Teaching and Learning, 19(1), 67-78 Published by Institute of Education (Singapore) This document may be used

More information

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE

Edexcel GCSE. Statistics 1389 Paper 1H. June Mark Scheme. Statistics Edexcel GCSE Edexcel GCSE Statistics 1389 Paper 1H June 2007 Mark Scheme Edexcel GCSE Statistics 1389 NOTES ON MARKING PRINCIPLES 1 Types of mark M marks: method marks A marks: accuracy marks B marks: unconditional

More information

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview

Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1. Line of Best Fit. Overview Algebra 1, Quarter 3, Unit 3.1 Line of Best Fit Overview Number of instructional days 6 (1 day assessment) (1 day = 45 minutes) Content to be learned Analyze scatter plots and construct the line of best

More information

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D.

Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D. Curriculum Development and the Teaching-Learning Process: The Development of Mathematical Thinking for all children Genevieve L. Hartman, Ph.D. Topics for today Part 1: Background and rationale Current

More information

The Evolution of Random Phenomena

The Evolution of Random Phenomena The Evolution of Random Phenomena A Look at Markov Chains Glen Wang glenw@uchicago.edu Splash! Chicago: Winter Cascade 2012 Lecture 1: What is Randomness? What is randomness? Can you think of some examples

More information

A Pumpkin Grows. Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher

A Pumpkin Grows. Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher GUIDED READING REPORT A Pumpkin Grows Written by Linda D. Bullock and illustrated by Debby Fisher KEY IDEA This nonfiction text traces the stages a pumpkin goes through as it grows from a seed to become

More information

Mathematics Success Level E

Mathematics Success Level E T403 [OBJECTIVE] The student will generate two patterns given two rules and identify the relationship between corresponding terms, generate ordered pairs, and graph the ordered pairs on a coordinate plane.

More information

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo

Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo Concept Acquisition Without Representation William Dylan Sabo Abstract: Contemporary debates in concept acquisition presuppose that cognizers can only acquire concepts on the basis of concepts they already

More information

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez

Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value. Catherine Perez Backwards Numbers: A Study of Place Value Catherine Perez Introduction I was reaching for my daily math sheet that my school has elected to use and in big bold letters in a box it said: TO ADD NUMBERS

More information

End-of-Module Assessment Task

End-of-Module Assessment Task Student Name Date 1 Date 2 Date 3 Topic E: Decompositions of 9 and 10 into Number Pairs Topic E Rubric Score: Time Elapsed: Topic F Topic G Topic H Materials: (S) Personal white board, number bond mat,

More information

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills

Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills Grade 6: Correlated to AGS Basic Math Skills Grade 6: Standard 1 Number Sense Students compare and order positive and negative integers, decimals, fractions, and mixed numbers. They find multiples and

More information

WHAT ARE VIRTUAL MANIPULATIVES?

WHAT ARE VIRTUAL MANIPULATIVES? by SCOTT PIERSON AA, Community College of the Air Force, 1992 BS, Eastern Connecticut State University, 2010 A VIRTUAL MANIPULATIVES PROJECT SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR TECHNOLOGY

More information

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1

Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course (Deciding What to Design) 1 Notes on The Sciences of the Artificial Adapted from a shorter document written for course 17-652 (Deciding What to Design) 1 Ali Almossawi December 29, 2005 1 Introduction The Sciences of the Artificial

More information

Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C

Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C Numeracy Medium term plan: Summer Term Level 2C/2B Year 2 Level 2A/3C Using and applying mathematics objectives (Problem solving, Communicating and Reasoning) Select the maths to use in some classroom

More information

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction

OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction OCR for Arabic using SIFT Descriptors With Online Failure Prediction Andrey Stolyarenko, Nachum Dershowitz The Blavatnik School of Computer Science Tel Aviv University Tel Aviv, Israel Email: stloyare@tau.ac.il,

More information

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful?

Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom: Helpful or Harmful? University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Action Research Projects Math in the Middle Institute Partnership 7-2008 Calculators in a Middle School Mathematics Classroom:

More information

2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half

2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half 2 nd grade Task 5 Half and Half Student Task Core Idea Number Properties Core Idea 4 Geometry and Measurement Draw and represent halves of geometric shapes. Describe how to know when a shape will show

More information

Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney

Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney Kindergarten Lessons for Unit 7: On The Move Me on the Map By Joan Sweeney Aligned with the Common Core State Standards in Reading, Speaking & Listening, and Language Written & Prepared for: Baltimore

More information

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards)

Grade 4. Common Core Adoption Process. (Unpacked Standards) Grade 4 Common Core Adoption Process (Unpacked Standards) Grade 4 Reading: Literature RL.4.1 Refer to details and examples in a text when explaining what the text says explicitly and when drawing inferences

More information

Instructional Supports for Common Core and Beyond: FORMATIVE ASSESMENT

Instructional Supports for Common Core and Beyond: FORMATIVE ASSESMENT Instructional Supports for Common Core and Beyond: FORMATIVE ASSESMENT Defining Date Guiding Question: Why is it important for everyone to have a common understanding of data and how they are used? Importance

More information

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017

Loughton School s curriculum evening. 28 th February 2017 Loughton School s curriculum evening 28 th February 2017 Aims of this session Share our approach to teaching writing, reading, SPaG and maths. Share resources, ideas and strategies to support children's

More information

Appendix L: Online Testing Highlights and Script

Appendix L: Online Testing Highlights and Script Online Testing Highlights and Script for Fall 2017 Ohio s State Tests Administrations Test administrators must use this document when administering Ohio s State Tests online. It includes step-by-step directions,

More information

Mathematics subject curriculum

Mathematics subject curriculum Mathematics subject curriculum Dette er ei omsetjing av den fastsette læreplanteksten. Læreplanen er fastsett på Nynorsk Established as a Regulation by the Ministry of Education and Research on 24 June

More information

COMMUNICATION & NETWORKING. How can I use the phone and to communicate effectively with adults?

COMMUNICATION & NETWORKING. How can I use the phone and  to communicate effectively with adults? 1 COMMUNICATION & NETWORKING Phone and E-mail Etiquette The BIG Idea How can I use the phone and e-mail to communicate effectively with adults? AGENDA Approx. 45 minutes I. Warm Up (5 minutes) II. Phone

More information

Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 (2010)

Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 (2010) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 8 (2010) 312 316 International Conference on Mathematics Education Research 2010 (ICMER 2010) Teachers Choice and Use of

More information

Mental Computation Strategies for Part-Whole Numbers

Mental Computation Strategies for Part-Whole Numbers Mental Computation Strategies for Part-Whole Numbers Annaliese Caney and Jane M. Watson University of Tasmania Abstract Mental strategies used by school students in solving problems involving operations

More information

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes

Stacks Teacher notes. Activity description. Suitability. Time. AMP resources. Equipment. Key mathematical language. Key processes Stacks Teacher notes Activity description (Interactive not shown on this sheet.) Pupils start by exploring the patterns generated by moving counters between two stacks according to a fixed rule, doubling

More information

Junior Fractions. With reference to the work of Peter Hughes, the late Richard Skemp, Van de Walle and other researchers.

Junior Fractions. With reference to the work of Peter Hughes, the late Richard Skemp, Van de Walle and other researchers. Junior Fractions With reference to the work of Peter Hughes, the late Richard Skemp, Van de Walle and other researchers. Fraction rope activity Information about teaching fractions Hands on - Bits and

More information

University of Waterloo School of Accountancy. AFM 102: Introductory Management Accounting. Fall Term 2004: Section 4

University of Waterloo School of Accountancy. AFM 102: Introductory Management Accounting. Fall Term 2004: Section 4 University of Waterloo School of Accountancy AFM 102: Introductory Management Accounting Fall Term 2004: Section 4 Instructor: Alan Webb Office: HH 289A / BFG 2120 B (after October 1) Phone: 888-4567 ext.

More information

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice

Getting Started with Deliberate Practice Getting Started with Deliberate Practice Most of the implementation guides so far in Learning on Steroids have focused on conceptual skills. Things like being able to form mental images, remembering facts

More information

Evaluating Statements About Probability

Evaluating Statements About Probability CONCEPT DEVELOPMENT Mathematics Assessment Project CLASSROOM CHALLENGES A Formative Assessment Lesson Evaluating Statements About Probability Mathematics Assessment Resource Service University of Nottingham

More information

Unit 3 Ratios and Rates Math 6

Unit 3 Ratios and Rates Math 6 Number of Days: 20 11/27/17 12/22/17 Unit Goals Stage 1 Unit Description: Students study the concepts and language of ratios and unit rates. They use proportional reasoning to solve problems. In particular,

More information

Centre for Evaluation & Monitoring SOSCA. Feedback Information

Centre for Evaluation & Monitoring SOSCA. Feedback Information Centre for Evaluation & Monitoring SOSCA Feedback Information Contents Contents About SOSCA... 3 SOSCA Feedback... 3 1. Assessment Feedback... 4 2. Predictions and Chances Graph Software... 7 3. Value

More information

ACTION LEARNING: AN INTRODUCTION AND SOME METHODS INTRODUCTION TO ACTION LEARNING

ACTION LEARNING: AN INTRODUCTION AND SOME METHODS INTRODUCTION TO ACTION LEARNING ACTION LEARNING: AN INTRODUCTION AND SOME METHODS INTRODUCTION TO ACTION LEARNING Action learning is a development process. Over several months people working in a small group, tackle important organisational

More information

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA

DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA DIDACTIC MODEL BRIDGING A CONCEPT WITH PHENOMENA Beba Shternberg, Center for Educational Technology, Israel Michal Yerushalmy University of Haifa, Israel The article focuses on a specific method of constructing

More information

Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching. Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta

Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching. Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta Stimulating Techniques in Micro Teaching Puan Ng Swee Teng Ketua Program Kursus Lanjutan U48 Kolej Sains Kesihatan Bersekutu, SAS, Ulu Kinta Learning Objectives General Objectives: At the end of the 2

More information

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation

Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation Lecture 2: Quantifiers and Approximation Case study: Most vs More than half Jakub Szymanik Outline Number Sense Approximate Number Sense Approximating most Superlative Meaning of most What About Counting?

More information

STRETCHING AND CHALLENGING LEARNERS

STRETCHING AND CHALLENGING LEARNERS STRETCHING AND CHALLENGING LEARNERS Melissa Ling JANUARY 18, 2013 OAKLANDS COLLEGE Contents Introduction... 2 Action Research... 3 Literature Review... 5 Project Hypothesis... 10 Methodology... 11 Data

More information

MODULE FRAMEWORK AND ASSESSMENT SHEET

MODULE FRAMEWORK AND ASSESSMENT SHEET MODULE FRAMEWORK AND ASSESSMENT SHEET LEARNING OUTCOMES (LOS) ASSESSMENT STANDARDS (ASS) FORMATIVE ASSESSMENT ASs Pages and (mark out of ) LOs (ave. out of ) SUMMATIVE ASSESSMENT Tasks or tests Ave for

More information

Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology

Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology INTRODUCTION Improving Conceptual Understanding of Physics with Technology Heidi Jackman Research Experience for Undergraduates, 1999 Michigan State University Advisors: Edwin Kashy and Michael Thoennessen

More information

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT)

STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT) Marshall University College of Science Mathematics Department STA 225: Introductory Statistics (CT) Course catalog description A critical thinking course in applied statistical reasoning covering basic

More information

CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and

CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and CONSTRUCTION OF AN ACHIEVEMENT TEST Introduction One of the important duties of a teacher is to observe the student in the classroom, laboratory and in other settings. He may also make use of tests in

More information

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy

Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the. Inf2A: Chomsky Hierarchy Informatics 2A: Language Complexity and the Chomsky Hierarchy September 28, 2010 Starter 1 Is there a finite state machine that recognises all those strings s from the alphabet {a, b} where the difference

More information

Section 7, Unit 4: Sample Student Book Activities for Teaching Listening

Section 7, Unit 4: Sample Student Book Activities for Teaching Listening Section 7, Unit 4: Sample Student Book Activities for Teaching Listening I. ACTIVITIES TO PRACTICE THE SOUND SYSTEM 1. Listen and Repeat for elementary school students. It could be done as a pre-listening

More information

Individual Component Checklist L I S T E N I N G. for use with ONE task ENGLISH VERSION

Individual Component Checklist L I S T E N I N G. for use with ONE task ENGLISH VERSION L I S T E N I N G Individual Component Checklist for use with ONE task ENGLISH VERSION INTRODUCTION This checklist has been designed for use as a practical tool for describing ONE TASK in a test of listening.

More information

Let s think about how to multiply and divide fractions by fractions!

Let s think about how to multiply and divide fractions by fractions! Let s think about how to multiply and divide fractions by fractions! June 25, 2007 (Monday) Takehaya Attached Elementary School, Tokyo Gakugei University Grade 6, Class # 1 (21 boys, 20 girls) Instructor:

More information

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test

How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test How to Judge the Quality of an Objective Classroom Test Technical Bulletin #6 Evaluation and Examination Service The University of Iowa (319) 335-0356 HOW TO JUDGE THE QUALITY OF AN OBJECTIVE CLASSROOM

More information

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards

NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards NCSC Alternate Assessments and Instructional Materials Based on Common Core State Standards Ricki Sabia, JD NCSC Parent Training and Technical Assistance Specialist ricki.sabia@uky.edu Background Alternate

More information

Using Realistic Mathematics Education with low to middle attaining pupils in secondary schools

Using Realistic Mathematics Education with low to middle attaining pupils in secondary schools Using Realistic Mathematics Education with low to middle attaining pupils in secondary schools Paul Dickinson, Frank Eade, Steve Gough, Sue Hough Manchester Metropolitan University Institute of Education

More information

The Indices Investigations Teacher s Notes

The Indices Investigations Teacher s Notes The Indices Investigations Teacher s Notes These activities are for students to use independently of the teacher to practise and develop number and algebra properties.. Number Framework domain and stage:

More information

Tour. English Discoveries Online

Tour. English Discoveries Online Techno-Ware Tour Of English Discoveries Online Online www.englishdiscoveries.com http://ed242us.engdis.com/technotms Guided Tour of English Discoveries Online Background: English Discoveries Online is

More information

Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School

Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School Journal of Physics: Conference Series PAPER OPEN ACCESS Analysis of Students Incorrect Answer on Two- Dimensional Shape Lesson Unit of the Third- Grade of a Primary School To cite this article: Ulfah and

More information

The Effectiveness of Realistic Mathematics Education Approach on Ability of Students Mathematical Concept Understanding

The Effectiveness of Realistic Mathematics Education Approach on Ability of Students Mathematical Concept Understanding International Journal of Sciences: Basic and Applied Research (IJSBAR) ISSN 2307-4531 (Print & Online) http://gssrr.org/index.php?journal=journalofbasicandapplied ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

More information

MOODLE 2.0 GLOSSARY TUTORIALS

MOODLE 2.0 GLOSSARY TUTORIALS BEGINNING TUTORIALS SECTION 1 TUTORIAL OVERVIEW MOODLE 2.0 GLOSSARY TUTORIALS The glossary activity module enables participants to create and maintain a list of definitions, like a dictionary, or to collect

More information

Introduce yourself. Change the name out and put your information here.

Introduce yourself. Change the name out and put your information here. Introduce yourself. Change the name out and put your information here. 1 History: CPM is a non-profit organization that has developed mathematics curriculum and provided its teachers with professional

More information

FEEDBACK & MARKING POLICY. Little Digmoor Primary School

FEEDBACK & MARKING POLICY. Little Digmoor Primary School FEEDBACK & MARKING POLICY Little Digmoor Primary School This policy complements the Teaching and Learning policy at Little Digmoor Primary School. It is a vital component in maximising the full learning

More information

EXAMPLES OF SPEAKING PERFORMANCES AT CEF LEVELS A2 TO C2. (Taken from Cambridge ESOL s Main Suite exams)

EXAMPLES OF SPEAKING PERFORMANCES AT CEF LEVELS A2 TO C2. (Taken from Cambridge ESOL s Main Suite exams) EXAMPLES OF SPEAKING PERFORMANCES AT CEF LEVELS A2 TO C2 (Taken from Cambridge ESOL s Main Suite exams) MARKS AND COMMENTARIES BEN: LEVEL C1/C1+ ALISER: LEVEL C2 Foreword This document accompanies the

More information

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years

Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years Monitoring Metacognitive abilities in children: A comparison of children between the ages of 5 to 7 years and 8 to 11 years Abstract Takang K. Tabe Department of Educational Psychology, University of Buea

More information

First Grade Standards

First Grade Standards These are the standards for what is taught throughout the year in First Grade. It is the expectation that these skills will be reinforced after they have been taught. Mathematical Practice Standards Taught

More information

Unit Lesson Plan: Native Americans 4th grade (SS and ELA)

Unit Lesson Plan: Native Americans 4th grade (SS and ELA) Unit Lesson Plan: Native Americans 4th grade (SS and ELA) Angie- comments in red Emily's comments in purple Sue's in orange Kasi Frenton-Comments in green-kas_122@hotmail.com 10/6/09 9:03 PM Unit Lesson

More information

AP Statistics Summer Assignment 17-18

AP Statistics Summer Assignment 17-18 AP Statistics Summer Assignment 17-18 Welcome to AP Statistics. This course will be unlike any other math class you have ever taken before! Before taking this course you will need to be competent in basic

More information

Standard 1: Number and Computation

Standard 1: Number and Computation Standard 1: Number and Computation Standard 1: Number and Computation The student uses numerical and computational concepts and procedures in a variety of situations. Benchmark 1: Number Sense The student

More information